1
|
Llamas-Cornejo I, Peterzell DH, Serrano-Pedraza I. Temporal mechanisms in frontoparallel stereomotion revealed by individual differences analysis. Eur J Neurosci 2024; 59:3117-3133. [PMID: 38622053 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.16342] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2023] [Revised: 03/19/2024] [Accepted: 03/20/2024] [Indexed: 04/17/2024]
Abstract
Masking experiments, using vertical and horizontal sinusoidal depth corrugations, have suggested the existence of more than two spatial-frequency disparity mechanisms. This result was confirmed through an individual differences approach. Here, using factor analytic techniques, we want to investigate the existence of independent temporal mechanisms in frontoparallel stereoscopic (cyclopean) motion. To construct stereomotion, we used sinusoidal depth corrugations obtained with dynamic random-dot stereograms. Thus, no luminance motion was present monocularly. We measured disparity thresholds for drifting vertical (up-down) and horizontal (left-right) sinusoidal corrugations of 0.4 cyc/deg at 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 6, and 8 Hz. In total, we tested 34 participants. Results showed a small orientation anisotropy with lower thresholds for horizontal corrugations. Disparity thresholds as a function of temporal frequency were almost constant from 0.25 up to 1 Hz, and then they increased monotonically. Principal component analysis uncovered two significant factors for vertical and two for horizontal corrugations. Varimax rotation showed that one factor loaded from 0.25 to 1-2 Hz and a second factor from 2 to 4 to 8 Hz. Direct Oblimin rotation indicated a moderate intercorrelation of both factors. Our results suggest the possible existence of two somewhat interdependent temporal mechanisms involved in frontoparallel stereomotion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ichasus Llamas-Cornejo
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas, Madrid, Spain
| | - David H Peterzell
- Fielding Graduate University, Santa Barbara, California, and National University (JFK), Pleasant Hill, California, USA
| | - Ignacio Serrano-Pedraza
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Reynaud A, Min SH. Spatial frequency channels depend on stimulus bandwidth in normal and amblyopic vision: an exploratory factor analysis. Front Comput Neurosci 2023; 17:1241455. [PMID: 37941764 PMCID: PMC10627878 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2023.1241455] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2023] [Accepted: 09/11/2023] [Indexed: 11/10/2023] Open
Abstract
The Contrast Sensitivity Function (CSF) is the measure of an observer's contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency. It is a sensitive measure to assess visual function in fundamental and clinical settings. Human contrast sensitivity is subserved by different spatial frequency channels. Also, it is known that amblyopes have deficits in contrast sensitivity, particularly at high spatial frequencies. Therefore, the aim of this study was to assess whether the contrast sensitivity function is subtended by the same spatial frequency channels in control and amblyopic populations. To determine these spatial frequency channels, we performed an exploratory factor analysis on five datasets of contrasts sensitivity functions of amblyopic and control participants measured using either gratings or noise patches, taken from our previous studies. In the range of 0.25-10 c/d, we identified two spatial frequency channels. When the CSF was measured with noise patches, the spatial frequency channels presented very similar tuning in the amblyopic eye and the fellow eye and were also similar to what was observed in controls. The only major difference was that the weight attributed to the high frequency channel was reduced by approximately 50% in the amblyopic eye. However, when the CSF was measured using gratings, the spatial frequency channels of the amblyopic eye were tuned toward lower spatial frequencies. These findings suggest that there is no mechanistic deficit for contrast sensitivity in amblyopia and that amblyopic vision may just be subjected to excessive internal noise and attenuation at higher spatial frequencies, thereby supporting the use of therapeutic strategies that involve rebalancing contrast.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandre Reynaud
- McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada
- Research Institute of the McGill University Health Center, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Seung Hyun Min
- State Key Laboratory of Ophthalmology, Optometry and Vision Science, School of Ophthalmology and Optometry, Affiliated Eye Hospital, Wenzhou Medical University, Wenzhou, Zhejiang, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Arranz-Paraíso S, Read JCA, Serrano-Pedraza I. Reduced surround suppression in monocular motion perception. J Vis 2021; 21:10. [PMID: 33450007 PMCID: PMC7814361 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.1.10] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2020] [Accepted: 12/12/2020] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Motion discrimination of large stimuli is impaired at high contrast and short durations. This psychophysical result has been linked with the center-surround suppression found in neurons of area MT. Recent physiology results have shown that most frontoparallel MT cells respond more strongly to binocular than to monocular stimulation. Here we measured the surround suppression strength under binocular and monocular viewing. Thirty-nine participants took part in two experiments: (a) where the nonstimulated eye viewed a blank field of the same luminance (n = 8) and (b) where it was occluded with a patch (n = 31). In both experiments, we measured duration thresholds for small (1 deg diameter) and large (7 deg) drifting gratings of 1 cpd with 85% contrast. For each subject, a Motion Suppression Index (MSI) was computed by subtracting the duration thresholds in logarithmic units of the large minus the small stimulus. Results were similar in both experiments. Combining the MSI of both experiments, we found that the strength of suppression for binocular condition (MSIbinocular = 0.249 ± 0.126 log10 (ms)) is 1.79 times higher than under monocular viewing (MSImonocular = 0.139 ± 0.137 log10 (ms)). This increase is too high to be explained by the higher perceived contrast of binocular stimuli and offers a new way of testing whether MT neurons account for surround suppression. Potentially, differences in surround suppression reported in clinical populations may reflect altered binocular processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jenny C A Read
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
- https://www.jennyreadresearch.com/
| | - Ignacio Serrano-Pedraza
- Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
- https://www.ucm.es/serranopedrazalab/
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Luna R, Serrano-Pedraza I. Evidence for different spatiotemporal mechanisms using duration thresholds: An individual differences approach. Vision Res 2020; 175:58-74. [PMID: 32712430 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2020] [Revised: 07/03/2020] [Accepted: 07/06/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
The study of motion perception through classical psychophysical methods has suggested that independent spatiotemporal filters acting over specific locations in retinal images carry out early motion processing. On the other hand, individual differences approaches have been able to identify a structure of spatiotemporal filters too. In this same fashion-through an individual differences approach-the present study aims to uncover a structure of spatiotemporal frequency selective motion mechanisms. This is done, for the first time, using supra-threshold contrast stimuli in a motion direction discrimination task. Two experiments were performed measuring duration thresholds for drifting 2D Gabor gratings of 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1, 1.5, 2, 3 and 6 c/deg. They moved with a speed of 2 deg/sec, with Michelson contrasts of 0.1 or 0.9 (Experiment 1) or had a contrast of 0.9 drifting with a temporal frequency of 2 Hz or 8 Hz (Experiment 2). Principal component analyses uncover three factors in each of four conditions. When Varimax-rotated, these are seen to be selective to spatial frequencies lower than 0.5 c/deg, intermediate ones from 0.5 to 1-1.5 c/deg, and frequencies greater than 1-1.5 c/deg. Direct Oblimin rotations indicate that factors are moderately correlated. Further analyses show very slight differences in the correlational structures between contrast conditions (0.1 vs. 0.9), and no differences between temporal frequency conditions (2 Hz vs. 8 Hz). To conclude, the idea of a three-factor structure in motion processing for low, intermediate, and high spatial frequencies is supported.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raúl Luna
- Faculty of Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid 28223, Spain.
| | - Ignacio Serrano-Pedraza
- Faculty of Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid 28223, Spain; Centre for Behaviour and Evolution, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne NE2 4HH, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kaneko S. Individual Variability in Simultaneous Contrast for Color and Brightness: Small Sample Factor Analyses Reveal Separate Induction Processes for Short and Long Flashes. Iperception 2018; 9:2041669518800507. [PMID: 30263104 PMCID: PMC6153537 DOI: 10.1177/2041669518800507] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2018] [Accepted: 08/21/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
In classic simultaneous color contrast and simultaneous brightness contrast, the color or brightness of a stimulus appears to shift toward the complementary (opposite) color or brightness of its surrounding region. Kaneko and colleagues proposed that simultaneous contrast involves separate "fast" and "slow" mechanisms, with stronger induction effects for fast than slow. Support for the model came from a diverse series of experiments showing that induction by surrounds varying in luminance or color was stronger for brief than long presentation times (10-40 vs. 80-640 ms). Here, to further examine possible underlying processes, we reanalyzed 12 separate small data sets from these studies using correlational and factor analytic techniques. For each analysis, a principal component analysis of induction strength revealed two factors, with one Varimax-rotated factor accounting for brief and one for long durations. In simultaneous brightness experiments, separate factor pairs were obtained for luminance increments and decrements. Despite being based on small sample sizes, the two-factor consistency among 12 analyses would not be expected by chance. The results are consistent with separate fast and slow processes mediating simultaneous contrast for brief and long flashes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sae Kaneko
- Sae Kaneko, Research Institute of Electrical
Communication, Tohoku University, 2-1-1 Katahira, Aoba-ku, Sendai, Miyagi
980-8577, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Grzeczkowski L, Clarke AM, Francis G, Mast FW, Herzog MH. About individual differences in vision. Vision Res 2017; 141:282-292. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.10.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2016] [Revised: 10/23/2016] [Accepted: 10/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
7
|
Reynaud A, Hess RF. Characterization of Spatial Frequency Channels Underlying Disparity Sensitivity by Factor Analysis of Population Data. Front Comput Neurosci 2017; 11:63. [PMID: 28744211 PMCID: PMC5504344 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2017.00063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2017] [Accepted: 06/28/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
It has been suggested that at least two mechanisms mediate disparity processing, one for coarse and one for fine disparities. Here we analyze individual differences in our previously measured normative dataset on the disparity sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency of 61 observers to assess the tuning of the spatial frequency channels underlying disparity sensitivity for oblique corrugations (Reynaud et al., 2015). Inter-correlations and factor analysis of the population data revealed two spatial frequency channels for disparity sensitivity: one tuned to high spatial frequencies and one tuned to low spatial frequencies. Our results confirm that disparity is encoded by spatial frequency channels of different sensitivities tuned to different ranges of corrugation frequencies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandre Reynaud
- McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill UniversityMontreal, QC, Canada
| | - Robert F Hess
- McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill UniversityMontreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Lee TH, Baek J, Lu ZL, Mather M. How arousal modulates the visual contrast sensitivity function. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2014; 14:978-84. [PMID: 24932842 DOI: 10.1037/a0037047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Recent evidence indicates that emotion enhances contrast thresholds in subsequent visual perception (Phelps, Ling, & Carrasco, 2006) and perceptual sensitivity for low-spatial frequency but not high-spatial frequency targets (Bocanegra & Zeelenberg, 2009b). However, these studies just report responses to various frequencies at a fixed contrast level or responses to various contrasts at a fixed frequency. In the current study, we measured the full contrast sensitivity function as a function of emotional arousal in order to investigate potential interactions between spatial frequency and contrast. We used a Bayesian adaptive inference with a trial-to-trial information gain strategy (Lesmes, Lu, Baek, & Albright, 2010) and a fear-conditioned stimulus to manipulate arousal level. The spatial frequency at which people showed peak contrast sensitivity shifted to lower spatial frequencies in the arousing condition compared with the nonarousing condition and people had greater contrast sensitivity function bandwidth in the arousing than in the nonarousing condition.
Collapse
|
9
|
Storm JE, Mazor KA, Aldous KM, Blount BC, Brodie SE, Serle JB. Visual contrast sensitivity in children exposed to tetrachloroethylene. ARCHIVES OF ENVIRONMENTAL & OCCUPATIONAL HEALTH 2011; 66:166-177. [PMID: 21864105 DOI: 10.1080/19338244.2010.539638] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
This study examined relationships between indoor air, breath, and blood tetrachloroethylene (perc) levels and visual contrast sensitivity (VCS) among adult and child residents of buildings with or without a colocated dry cleaner using perc. Decreasing trends in proportions of adults or children with maximum VCS scores indicated decreased VCS at a single spatial frequency (12 cycles per degree [cpd]) among children residing in buildings with colocated dry cleaners when indoor air perc level averaged 336 μg/m³; breath perc level averaged 159.5 μg/m³; and blood perc level averaged 0.51 μg/L. Adjusted logistic regression indicated that increases in indoor air, breath, and blood perc levels among all child participants significantly increased the odds for decreased VCS at 12 cpd. Adult VCS was not significantly decreased by increasing indoor air, breath, or blood perc level. These results suggest that elevated residential perc exposures may alter children's VCS, a possible subclinical central nervous system effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jan E Storm
- Center for Environmental Health, New York State Department of Health, Troy, New York 12180, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Cass J, Stuit S, Bex P, Alais D. Orientation bandwidths are invariant across spatiotemporal frequency after isotropic components are removed. J Vis 2009; 9:17.1-14. [PMID: 20053108 DOI: 10.1167/9.12.17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2009] [Accepted: 09/23/2009] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
It is well established that mammalian visual cortex possesses a large proportion of orientation-selective neurons. Attempts to measure the bandwidth of these mechanisms psychophysically have yielded highly variable results ( approximately 6 degrees -180 degrees ). Two stimulus factors have been proposed to account for this variability: spatial and temporal frequency; with several studies indicating broader bandwidths at low spatial and high temporal frequencies. We estimated orientation bandwidths using a classic overlay masking paradigm across a range of spatiotemporal frequencies (0.5, 2, and 8 c.p.d.; 1.6 and 12.5 Hz) with target and mask presented either monoptically or dichoptically. A standard three-parameter Gaussian model (amplitude and width, mean fixed at 0 degrees ) confirms that bandwidths generally increase at low spatial and high temporal frequencies. When incorporating an additional orientation-untuned (isotropic) amplitude component, however, we find that not only are the amplitudes of isotropic and orientation-tuned components highly dependent upon stimulus spatiotemporal frequency, but orientation bandwidths are highly invariant ( approximately 30 degrees half width half amplitude). These results suggest that previously reported spatiotemporally contingent bandwidth effects may have confounded bandwidth with isotropic (so-called cross-orientation) masking. Interestingly, the magnitudes of all monoptically derived parameter estimates were found to transfer dichoptically suggesting a cortical locus for both isotropic and orientation-tuned masking.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John Cass
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Australia.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Santos NAD, França VDCRDM. Sensibilidade ao contraste a grades senoidais de freqüências espaciais baixas em crianças. ESTUDOS DE PSICOLOGIA (CAMPINAS) 2008. [DOI: 10.1590/s0103-166x2008000200002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
O objetivo deste trabalho foi determinar a função de sensibilidade ao contraste para freqüências espaciais de 0,25; 0,5; 1,0 e 2,0 ciclos por grau em crianças de 4 a 13 anos. Foram estimados limiares de contraste para 60 participantes (50 crianças e 10 adultos jovens), utilizando o método psicofísico da escolha forçada e nível baixo de luminância. Todos os participantes apresentavam acuidade visual normal e se encontravam livres de doenças oculares identificáveis. Os resultados mostraram que a função de sensibilidade ao contraste de crianças de 4-5, 6-7, 8-9, 10-11 e 12-13 anos melhora significativamente com a idade. Os resultados mostraram ainda que a função de sensibilidade ao contraste de crianças de 12-13 anos é semelhante à de adultos jovens (19-22 anos). Estes resultados sugerem que o desenvolvimento da função de sensibilidade ao contraste para grade senoidal em nível baixo de luminância melhora até os 12-13 anos.
Collapse
|
12
|
Wilmer JB. How to use individual differences to isolate functional organization, biology, and utility of visual functions; with illustrative proposals for stereopsis. SPATIAL VISION 2008; 21:561-79. [PMID: 19017483 PMCID: PMC2586597 DOI: 10.1163/156856808786451408] [Citation(s) in RCA: 87] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
This paper is a call for greater use of individual differences in the basic science of visual perception. Individual differences yield insights into visual perception's functional organization, underlying biological/environmental mechanisms, and utility. I first explain the general approach advocated and where it comes from. Second, I describe five principles central to learning about the nature of visual perception through individual differences. Third, I elaborate on the use of individual differences to gain insights into the three areas mentioned above (function, biology/environment, utility), in each case describing the approach advocated, presenting model examples from the literature, and laying out illustrative research proposals for the case of stereopsis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeremy B Wilmer
- SUNY State College of Optometry, Vision Sciences, 33 West 42nd Street, New York, NY 10036, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Simpson WA, McFadden SM. Spatial frequency channels derived from individual differences. Vision Res 2005; 45:2723-7. [PMID: 16051307 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.01.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2004] [Revised: 11/12/2004] [Accepted: 01/13/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Contrast sensitivity functions differ from observer to observer. We propose that these differences arise because each observer has unique weights for the outputs of the neural channels that underlie the contrast sensitivity function. By applying principal components analysis to individual contrast sensitivity functions of 297 observers, estimates of the channel tuning curves were found. We find evidence for three broadly tuned bandpass channels with peaks at 4, 8, and 16c/deg and bandwidth near 1.3 octaves. These channel tuning curves were reproduced in a cross-validation study of 56 observers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- William A Simpson
- DRDC Toronto Modelling Section, 1133 Sheppard Avenue West, P.O. Box 2000, Toronto, Ont., Canada.
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Bilson AC, Mizokami Y, Webster MA. Visual adjustments to temporal blur. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2005; 22:2281-8. [PMID: 16277297 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.22.002281] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Abstract
After observers have adapted to an edge that is spatially blurred or sharpened, a focused edge appears too sharp or blurred, respectively. These adjustments to blur may play an important role in calibrating spatial sensitivity. We examined whether similar adjustments influence the perception of temporal edges, by measuring the appearance of a step change in the luminance of a uniform field after adapting to blurred or sharpened transitions. Stimuli were square-wave alternations (at 1 to 8 Hz) filtered by changing the slope of the amplitude spectrum. A two-alternative-forced-choice task was used to adjust the slope until it appeared as a step change, or until it matched the perceived transitions in a reference stimulus. Observers could accurately set the waveform to a square wave, but only at the slower alternation rates. However, these settings were strongly biased by prior adaptation to filtered stimuli, or when the stimuli were viewed within temporally filtered surrounds. Control experiments suggest that the latter induction effects result directly from the temporal blur and are not simply a consequence of brightness induction in the fields. These results suggest that adaptation and induction adjust visual coding so that images are focused not only in space but also in time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron C Bilson
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno 89557, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Scialfa CT, Kline DW, Wood PK. Structural modeling of contrast sensitivity in adulthood. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2002; 19:158-165. [PMID: 11778719 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.19.000158] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Structural equation modeling was used to assess the utility of the sensorineural model of contrast sensitivity proposed by Sekuler et al. [Vision Res. 24, 689 (1984)] to account for spatial vision in adulthood. In Study 1, visual acuity and contrast sensitivity (1.5-18 c/deg) were measured in 84 people between the ages of 19 and 81 yr. No three-filter model fitted the data well. Although a two-filter model was associated with good fit indices, parameter estimates for both filters were inconsistent with physiological and behavioral data. In Study 2, acuity and contrast sensitivity (1.5-18 c/deg) were assessed in 95 observers between the ages of 23 and 73 yr. All measures were gathered once per month over a three-month period. The Sekuler et al. three-filter model did not fit the data from any time of measure, but a two-filter, bandpass model provided a consistent and excellent fit for all three waves. The model suggests that age-related change in the neural mechanisms underlying contrast sensitivity is minimal once acuity is controlled. Discrepancies between this conclusion and that reported by Sekuler et al. may be related to test type, psychophysical method, reliability, and sample selection.
Collapse
|
16
|
Peterzell DH, Chang SK, Teller DY. Spatial frequency tuned covariance channels for red-green and luminance-modulated gratings: psychophysical data from human infants. Vision Res 2000; 40:431-44. [PMID: 10820623 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00188-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
This study concerns the spatial-frequency-tuned channels underlying infants' contrast sensitivity functions (CSFs) for red-green chromatic stimuli, and their relationship to the channels underlying infants' CSFs for luminance-modulated stimuli. Behavioral (forced-choice preferential-looking) techniques and stationary stimuli were used. In experiment 1. contrast thresholds were measured in 4- and 6-month-olds, using isoluminant red-green gratings with spatial frequencies ranging from 0.27 to 1.53 c deg. In experiment 2. contrast thresholds were measured in 4-month-olds. using both red-green and luminance-modulated gratings in the same low spatial frequency range. Covariance analyses of individual differences were performed. Experiment 1 revealed one dominant covariance channel for the detection of red-green gratings, with a second channel contributing to detection of the highest spatial frequencies used. Experiment 2 revealed two to three channels serving color and luminance: but surprisingly these channels were not statistically separable for luminance versus chromatic stimuli. Thus, covariance channels for color and luminance that are independent for adults [Peterzell & Teller (2000). Spatial frequency tuned covariance channels for red-green and luminance-modulated gratings: psychophysical data from human adults. Vision Research, 40, 417-430] are apparently interdependent in infants. These data suggest that for infants, detection thresholds for chromatic and luminance-modulated stimuli may be limited by common mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D H Peterzell
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Peterzell DH, Teller DY. Spatial frequency tuned covariance channels for red-green and luminance-modulated gratings: psychophysical data from human adults. Vision Res 2000; 40:417-30. [PMID: 10820622 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00187-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Both chromatic and luminance-modulated stimuli are served by multiple spatial-frequency-tuned channels. This experiment investigated the independence versus interdependence of spatial frequency channels that serve the detection of red-green chromatic versus yellow-black luminance-modulated stimuli at low spatial frequencies. Contrast thresholds for both chromatic and luminance-modulated gratings were measured within 12 individual subjects using a repeated-measures design. Spatial frequencies ranged from 0.27 to 2.16 c/deg. A covariance structure analysis of individual differences was applied to the data. We computed statistical sources of individual variability, used them to define covariance channels, and determined the number and frequency tuning of these channels. For luminance-modulated gratings, two covariance channels were found, including one above and one below 1 c/deg [cf. Peterzell, & Teller (1996). Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions: the coarsest spatial pattern analyzer. Vision Research, 36, 3077-3085]. For chromatic gratings, correlations between thresholds for most spatial frequencies were uniformly high, yielding a single covariance channel covering all but the highest spatial frequency tested. A combined analysis of both data sets recovered the same three covariance channels, and showed that detection thresholds for low-frequency red-green chromatic and luminance-modulated stimuli are served by separate, statistically independent processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D H Peterzell
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Dobkins KR, Gunther KL, Peterzell DH. What covariance mechanisms underlie green/red equiluminance, luminance contrast sensitivity and chromatic (green/red) contrast sensitivity? Vision Res 2000; 40:613-28. [PMID: 10824265 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00211-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In order to investigate the mechanisms underlying green/red equiluminance matches in human observers and their relationship to mechanisms subserving luminance and/or chromatic (green/red) contrast sensitivity, we tested 21 human subjects along these dimensions at 16 different spatial and temporal frequencies (spatial frequency, 0.25-2 c/deg; temporal frequency, 2-16 Hz) and applied factor analysis to extract mechanisms underlying the data set. The results from our factor analysis revealed separate sources of variability for green/red equiluminance, luminance sensitivity and chromatic sensitivity, thus suggesting separate mechanisms underlying each of the three main conditions. When factor analysis was applied separately to green/red equiluminance data, two temporally-tuned factors were revealed (factor 1, 2-4 Hz; factor 2, 8-16 Hz), suggesting the existence of separate mechanisms underlying equiluminance settings at low versus high temporal frequencies. In addition, although the three main conditions remained separate in our factor analysis of the entire data set, our correlation matrix nonetheless revealed systematic correlations between equiluminance settings and luminance sensitivity at high temporal frequencies, and between equiluminance settings and chromatic sensitivity at low temporal frequencies. Taken together, these data suggest that the high temporal frequency factor underlying green/red equiluminance is governed predominantly by luminance mechanisms, while the low temporal frequency factor receives contribution from chromatic mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K R Dobkins
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla 92093, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Peterzell DH, Teller DY. Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions: the lowest spatial frequency channels. Vision Res 1996; 36:3077-85. [PMID: 8917770 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00061-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
The number and nature of spatial channels tuned to low spatial frequencies in photopic vision was examined by measuring individual differences in the contrast sensitivity functions (CSFs) of seven visually normal adults. Stationary, 51 cd/m2, low spatial frequency sinusoidal gratings between 0.27 and 2.16 c/deg were used as stimuli. Correlational and factor analyses revealed that the set of CSFs contained only one statistical source of individual variability at spatial frequencies below 1 c/deg (tuned to a peak of about 0.8 c/deg), and a second source above 1 c/deg (tuned to about 1.4 c/deg). The sources ("factor-channels") mapped well onto the two coarsest spatial frequency channels from some existing computational models. The analysis was applied also to earlier data from 4-, 6- and 8-month-old infants, in which two sources of variability have been found below 1 c/deg [Peterzell, D. H., Werner, J. S. & Kaplan, P. S. (1995). Vision Research, 35, 961-980]. The combined results are consistent with the hypothesis that in photopic vision of the neonate, there are two channels with peak sensitivities below 1 c/deg, and that these channels shift their tuning from lower to higher spatial frequencies by about a factor of four during development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D H Peterzell
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Scattle 98195-1525, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|