1
|
Matsuda N, Abe MO. Implicit sensorimotor learning in ballistic movement for transporting an object to a target. Sci Rep 2024; 14:21003. [PMID: 39251727 PMCID: PMC11385560 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-71925-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2024] [Accepted: 09/02/2024] [Indexed: 09/11/2024] Open
Abstract
To enhance and sustain movement accuracy, humans make corrections in subsequent trials based on previous errors. Trial-by-trial learning occurs unconsciously and has mostly been studied using reaching movements. Goal-directed projection movements, such as archery, have an inherent delay between releasing an object and observing an outcome (e.g. the arrival position of the object), and this delay may prevent trial-by-trial implicit learning. We aimed to investigate the learning in the projection movement and the impacts of the inherent delay. During the experiment, a joystick was flicked once to transport a cursor from the starting location to a target. To manipulate the length of the delay between the cursor release and outcome observation, the speed of the cursor movement was varied: a fast speed can lead to a short delay. We found trial-by-trial implicit learning under all speed conditions, and the error sensitivity was not significantly different across speed conditions. Furthermore, the error sensitivity depended on the target location, that is, the movement direction. The results indicate that trial-by-trial implicit learning occurred in goal-directed projection movement, despite the length of the inherent delay. Additionally, the degree of this learning was affected by the movement direction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Naoyoshi Matsuda
- Graduate School of Education, Hokkaido University, Kita-11, Nishi-7, Kita-ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-0811, Japan.
| | - Masaki O Abe
- Faculty of Education, Hokkaido University, Kita-11, Nishi-7, Kita-ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-0811, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Matsumura Y, Roach NW, Heron J, Miyazaki M. Body-part specificity for learning of multiple prior distributions in human coincidence timing. NPJ SCIENCE OF LEARNING 2024; 9:34. [PMID: 38698023 PMCID: PMC11066023 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-024-00241-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2023] [Accepted: 04/02/2024] [Indexed: 05/05/2024]
Abstract
During timing tasks, the brain learns the statistical distribution of target intervals and integrates this prior knowledge with sensory inputs to optimise task performance. Daily events can have different temporal statistics (e.g., fastball/slowball in baseball batting), making it important to learn and retain multiple priors. However, the rules governing this process are not yet understood. Here, we demonstrate that the learning of multiple prior distributions in a coincidence timing task is characterised by body-part specificity. In our experiments, two prior distributions (short and long intervals) were imposed on participants. When using only one body part for timing responses, regardless of the priors, participants learned a single prior by generalising over the two distributions. However, when the two priors were assigned to different body parts, participants concurrently learned the two independent priors. Moreover, body-part specific prior acquisition was faster when the priors were assigned to anatomically distant body parts (e.g., hand/foot) than when they were assigned to close body parts (e.g., index/middle fingers). This suggests that the body-part specific learning of priors is organised according to somatotopy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yoshiki Matsumura
- Graduate School of Integrated Science and Technology, Shizuoka University, Hamamatsu, Japan
| | - Neil W Roach
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK
| | - James Heron
- School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Bradford, Bradford, UK
| | - Makoto Miyazaki
- Graduate School of Integrated Science and Technology, Shizuoka University, Hamamatsu, Japan.
- Faculty of Informatics, Shizuoka University, Hamamatsu, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Rubinstein JF, Singh M, Kowler E. Bayesian approaches to smooth pursuit of random dot kinematograms: effects of varying RDK noise and the predictability of RDK direction. J Neurophysiol 2024; 131:394-416. [PMID: 38149327 PMCID: PMC11551001 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00116.2023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2023] [Revised: 11/30/2023] [Accepted: 12/20/2023] [Indexed: 12/28/2023] Open
Abstract
Smooth pursuit eye movements respond on the basis of both immediate and anticipated target motion, where anticipations may be derived from either memory or perceptual cues. To study the combined influence of both immediate sensory motion and anticipation, subjects pursued clear or noisy random dot kinematograms (RDKs) whose mean directions were chosen from Gaussian distributions with SDs = 10° (narrow prior) or 45° (wide prior). Pursuit directions were consistent with Bayesian theory in that transitions over time from dependence on the prior to near total dependence on immediate sensory motion (likelihood) took longer with the noisier RDKs and with the narrower, more reliable, prior. Results were fit to Bayesian models in which parameters representing the variability of the likelihood either were or were not constrained to be the same for both priors. The unconstrained model provided a statistically better fit, with the influence of the prior in the constrained model smaller than predicted from strict reliability-based weighting of prior and likelihood. Factors that may have contributed to this outcome include prior variability different from nominal values, low-level sensorimotor learning with the narrow prior, or departures of pursuit from strict adherence to reliability-based weighting. Although modifications of, or alternatives to, the normative Bayesian model will be required, these results, along with previous studies, suggest that Bayesian approaches are a promising framework to understand how pursuit combines immediate sensory motion, past history, and informative perceptual cues to accurately track the target motion that is most likely to occur in the immediate future.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Smooth pursuit eye movements respond on the basis of anticipated, as well as immediate, target motions. Bayesian models using reliability-based weighting of previous (prior) and immediate target motions (likelihood) accounted for many, but not all, aspects of pursuit of clear and noisy random dot kinematograms with different levels of predictability. Bayesian approaches may solve the long-standing problem of how pursuit combines immediate sensory motion and anticipation of future motion to configure an effective response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason F Rubinstein
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Piscataway, New Jersey, United States
| | - Manish Singh
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Piscataway, New Jersey, United States
| | - Eileen Kowler
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Piscataway, New Jersey, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Hewitson CL, Kaplan DM, Crossley MJ. Error-independent effect of sensory uncertainty on motor learning when both feedforward and feedback control processes are engaged. PLoS Comput Biol 2023; 19:e1010526. [PMID: 37683013 PMCID: PMC10522034 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010526] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2022] [Revised: 09/26/2023] [Accepted: 08/15/2023] [Indexed: 09/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Integrating sensory information during movement and adapting motor plans over successive movements are both essential for accurate, flexible motor behaviour. When an ongoing movement is off target, feedback control mechanisms update the descending motor commands to counter the sensed error. Over longer timescales, errors induce adaptation in feedforward planning so that future movements become more accurate and require less online adjustment from feedback control processes. Both the degree to which sensory feedback is integrated into an ongoing movement and the degree to which movement errors drive adaptive changes in feedforward motor plans have been shown to scale inversely with sensory uncertainty. However, since these processes have only been studied in isolation from one another, little is known about how they are influenced by sensory uncertainty in real-world movement contexts where they co-occur. Here, we show that sensory uncertainty may impact feedforward adaptation of reaching movements differently when feedback integration is present versus when it is absent. In particular, participants gradually adjust their movements from trial-to-trial in a manner that is well characterised by a slow and consistent envelope of error reduction. Riding on top of this slow envelope, participants exhibit large and abrupt changes in their initial movement vectors that are strongly correlated with the degree of sensory uncertainty present on the previous trial. However, these abrupt changes are insensitive to the magnitude and direction of the sensed movement error. These results prompt important questions for current models of sensorimotor learning under uncertainty and open up new avenues for future exploration in the field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - David M. Kaplan
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
- Macquarie University Performance and Expertise Research Centre, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Matthew J. Crossley
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
- Macquarie University Performance and Expertise Research Centre, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Fassold ME, Locke SM, Landy MS. Feeling lucky? prospective and retrospective cues for sensorimotor confidence. PLoS Comput Biol 2023; 19:e1010740. [PMID: 37363929 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010740] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2022] [Accepted: 06/08/2023] [Indexed: 06/28/2023] Open
Abstract
On a daily basis, humans interact with the outside world using judgments of sensorimotor confidence, constantly evaluating our actions for success. We ask, what sensory and motor-execution cues are used in making these judgements and when are they available? Two sources of temporally distinct information are prospective cues, available prior to the action (e.g., knowledge of motor noise and past performance), and retrospective cues specific to the action itself (e.g., proprioceptive measurements). We investigated the use of these two cues in two tasks, a secondary motor-awareness task and a main task in which participants reached toward a visual target with an unseen hand and then made a continuous judgment of confidence about the success of the reach. Confidence was reported by setting the size of a circle centered on the reach-target location, where a larger circle reflects lower confidence. Points were awarded if the confidence circle enclosed the true endpoint, with fewer points returned for larger circles. This incentivized accurate reaches and attentive reporting to maximize the score. We compared three Bayesian-inference models of sensorimotor confidence based on either prospective cues, retrospective cues, or both sources of information to maximize expected gain (i.e., an ideal-performance model). Our findings showed two distinct strategies: participants either performed as ideal observers, using both prospective and retrospective cues to make the confidence judgment, or relied solely on prospective information, ignoring retrospective cues. Thus, participants can make use of retrospective cues, evidenced by the behavior observed in our motor-awareness task, but these cues are not always included in the computation of sensorimotor confidence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marissa E Fassold
- Dept. of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Shannon M Locke
- Laboratoire des Systèmes Perceptifs, Département d'Études Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France
| | - Michael S Landy
- Dept. of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Weightman M, Brittain JS, Miall RC, Jenkinson N. Residual errors in visuomotor adaptation persist despite extended motor preparation periods. J Neurophysiol 2022; 127:519-528. [PMID: 35044854 PMCID: PMC8836731 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00301.2021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
A consistent finding in sensorimotor adaptation is a persistent undershoot of full compensation, such that performance asymptotes with residual errors greater than seen at baseline. This behavior has been attributed to limiting factors within the implicit adaptation system, which reaches a suboptimal equilibrium between trial-by-trial learning and forgetting. However, recent research has suggested that allowing longer motor planning periods prior to movement eliminates these residual errors. The additional planning time allows required cognitive processes to be completed before movement onset, thus increasing accuracy. Here, we looked to extend these findings by investigating the relationship between increased motor preparation time and the size of imposed visuomotor rotation (30°, 45°, or 60°), with regard to the final asymptotic level of adaptation. We found that restricting preparation time to 0.35 s impaired adaptation for moderate and larger rotations, resulting in larger residual errors compared to groups with additional preparation time. However, we found that even extended preparation time failed to eliminate persistent errors, regardless of magnitude of cursor rotation. Thus, the asymptote of adaptation was significantly less than the degree of imposed rotation, for all experimental groups. In addition, there was a positive relationship between asymptotic error and implicit retention. These data suggest that a prolonged motor preparation period is insufficient to reliably achieve complete adaptation, and therefore, our results suggest that factors beyond that of planning time contribute to asymptotic adaptation levels.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Residual errors in sensorimotor adaptation are commonly attributed to an equilibrium between trial-by-trial learning and forgetting. Recent research suggested that allowing sufficient time for mental rotation eliminates these errors. In a number of experimental conditions, we show that although restricted motor preparation time does limit adaptation-consistent with mental rotation-extending preparation time fails to eliminate the residual errors in motor adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Weightman
- 1School of Sport, Exercise and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom,3MRC-Versus Arthritis Centre for Musculoskeletal Ageing Research, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom,4Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
| | - John-Stuart Brittain
- 2School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom,4Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
| | - R. Chris Miall
- 2School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom,3MRC-Versus Arthritis Centre for Musculoskeletal Ageing Research, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom,4Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
| | - Ned Jenkinson
- 1School of Sport, Exercise and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom,3MRC-Versus Arthritis Centre for Musculoskeletal Ageing Research, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom,4Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Gyoda T, Ishida K, Watanabe T, Nojima I. Repetitive training of contralateral limb through reconsolidation strengthens motor skills. Neurosci Lett 2021; 766:136306. [PMID: 34699943 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2021.136306] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2021] [Revised: 10/06/2021] [Accepted: 10/19/2021] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Consolidated memories become transiently labile after memory reactivation, allowing update through reconsolidation. Although previous reports have indicated that the effects of post-reactivation training depend on the type of practice, it is unclear whether post-reactivation motor skill training of one limb can enhance the performance of the opposite limb. The present study aimed to investigate whether post-reactivation training (performing an isometric pinch force task) under two different training conditions using the left limb would enhance motor skills of the right limb through reconsolidation. Motor skills were measured in 38 healthy right-handed young adults during three sessions (S): S1 (right-hand training), S2 (memory reactivation and left-hand training 6 h after S1), and S3 (right-hand motor skill test 24 h after S1). Participants were assigned to one of three groups according to the task performed during S2: untrained controls (no training), left-hand training (constant force conditions), or left-hand training (variable force conditions). Left-hand training after memory reactivation during S2 significantly enhanced the motor skills of the right hand. Notably, constant training conditions significantly increased performance compared to the control group. These findings suggest that post-reactivation training in one limb effectively enhances motor skills in the opposite limb, and the effects depend on the training strategy, which has important implications for motor rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tomoya Gyoda
- Department of Physical Therapy, Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Nagoya, Japan; Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| | - Kazuto Ishida
- Department of Physical Therapy, Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Nagoya, Japan; Graduate School of Health Sciences, Toyohashi SOZO University, Toyohashi, Japan
| | - Tatsunori Watanabe
- Department of Sensorimotor Neuroscience, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan
| | - Ippei Nojima
- Division of Physical Therapy, Shinshu University School of Health Sciences, Matsumoto, Nagano, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Previous Motor Actions Outweigh Sensory Information in Sensorimotor Statistical Learning. eNeuro 2021; 8:ENEURO.0032-21.2021. [PMID: 34413084 PMCID: PMC8482855 DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0032-21.2021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2021] [Revised: 06/21/2021] [Accepted: 06/23/2021] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans can use their previous experience in form of statistical priors to improve decisions. It is, however, unclear how such priors are learned and represented. Importantly, it has remained elusive whether prior learning is independent of the sensorimotor system involved in the learning process or not, as both modality-specific and modality-general learning have been reported in the past. Here, we used a saccadic eye movement task to probe the learning and representation of a spatial prior across a few trials. In this task, learning occurs in an unsupervised manner and through encountering trial-by-trial visual hints drawn from a distribution centered on the target location. Using a model-comparison approach, we found that participants’ prior knowledge is largely represented in the form of their previous motor actions, with minimal influence from the previously seen visual hints. By using two different motor contexts for response (looking either at the estimated target location, or exactly opposite to it), we could further compare whether prior experience obtained in one motor context can be transferred to the other. Although learning curves were highly similar, and participants seemed to use the same strategy for both response types, they could not fully transfer their knowledge between contexts, as performance and confidence ratings dropped after a switch of the required response. Together, our results suggest that humans preferably use the internal representations of their previous motor actions, rather than past incoming sensory information, to form statistical sensorimotor priors on the timescale of a few trials.
Collapse
|