1
|
Hennion S, Fournier V, Derambure P, Delelis G, Schiaratura L. Effect of familiarity and knowledge about epilepsy on associated cultural stereotypes in French society. Epilepsy Behav 2025; 163:110216. [PMID: 39671738 DOI: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2024.110216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2024] [Revised: 11/12/2024] [Accepted: 12/06/2024] [Indexed: 12/15/2024]
Abstract
People with epilepsy face stigma that impacts numerous aspects of their daily lives. Although the stigma surrounding people with epilepsy has been extensively documented, the mechanisms underlying it-such as cultural stereotypes-remain to be explored. Cultural stereotypes are widely shared beliefs within a cultural context about attributes typically associated with members of a particular group. This study, conducted within French society, has two primary objectives: 1) to define the content of cultural stereotypes associated with people suffering from epilepsy and 2) to examine how familiarity and knowledge about epilepsy influence these stereotypes. To explore these stereotypes, a free association task was conducted across three cultural groups (n = 96): (1) the general population, with low familiarity and knowledge about epilepsy (n = 39); (2) healthcare professionals without epilepsy specialization, who have more familiarity and knowledge than the general population (n = 38); and (3) healthcare professionals specialized in epilepsy, who have the highest familiarity and knowledge of the three groups (n = 29). All participants held higher education qualifications to ensure a more homogeneous socio-cultural background across groups. Using the software program "IraMuTeQ", we analyzed the diversity of terms each group associated with "people with epilepsy." Additionally, we examined the valence and typicality of cultural stereotypes in each group. The results reveal that, regardless of familiarity and knowledge levels, cultural stereotypes linked to epilepsy are generally negative. Across the entire sample, the most prototypical associations with people with epilepsy included "madness," "possession," "tongue," and "intellectual deficiency." The general population shares some cultural stereotypes with non-specialized healthcare professionals (e.g., "photosensitivity"), while non-specialized professionals share other associations with specialized healthcare professionals (e.g., "intellectual deficiency" and "mental illness"). However, no overlap was found between the cultural stereotypes of the general population and those of healthcare professionals specialized in epilepsy. Stereotypes related to epilepsy appear to be less typical among healthcare professionals compared to the general population. This distinction between cultural stereotypes and personal beliefs is further discussed below. Considering cultural stereotypes may allow for more tailored and effective interventions to reduce epilepsy-related stigma by addressing specific socio-cultural groups. Further research within a cross-cultural approach is recommended to deepen these findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie Hennion
- Centre de Référence des Épilepsies Rares, Service de Neurophysiologie Clinique, Unité d'épileptologie, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Lille, Lille, France; Inserm, U1172 - LilNCog - Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Lille, France
| | - Valentyn Fournier
- Univ. Lille, CNRS, UMR 9193 - SCALab - Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, F-59000 Lille, France; Univ. Lille, ULR 4072 - PSITEC - Psychologie: Interactions, Temps, Emotions, Cognition, F-59000 Lille, France
| | - Philippe Derambure
- Centre de Référence des Épilepsies Rares, Service de Neurophysiologie Clinique, Unité d'épileptologie, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Lille, Lille, France; Inserm, U1172 - LilNCog - Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Lille, France
| | - Gérald Delelis
- Univ. Lille, CNRS, UMR 9193 - SCALab - Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, F-59000 Lille, France
| | - Loris Schiaratura
- Univ. Lille, ULR 4072 - PSITEC - Psychologie: Interactions, Temps, Emotions, Cognition, F-59000 Lille, France.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Eyal T, Epley N. Exaggerating Accessible Differences: When Gender Stereotypes Overestimate Actual Group Differences. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2017; 43:1323-1336. [PMID: 28903680 DOI: 10.1177/0146167217713190] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Stereotypes are often presumed to exaggerate group differences, but empirical evidence is mixed. We suggest exaggeration is moderated by the accessibility of specific stereotype content. In particular, because the most accessible stereotype contents are attributes perceived to differ between groups, those attributes are most likely to exaggerate actual group differences due to regression to the mean. We tested this hypothesis using a highly accessible gender stereotype: that women are more socially sensitive than men. We confirmed that the most accessible stereotype content involves attributes perceived to differ between groups (pretest), and that these stereotypes contain some accuracy but significantly exaggerate actual gender differences (Experiment 1). We observe less exaggeration when judging less accessible stereotype content (Experiment 2), or when judging individual men and women (Experiment 3). Considering the accessibility of specific stereotype content may explain when stereotypes exaggerate actual group differences and when they do not.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tal Eyal
- 1 Ben Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Meza-de-Luna ME, Cantera LM, Blanch JM, Beiras A. Stereotypes of Intimate Partner Violence: Do Sex and Sexual Orientation Matter? PSICOLOGIA: TEORIA E PESQUISA 2016. [DOI: 10.1590/0102-3772e323210] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract This study analyzed stereotypes on intimate partner violence (IPV) of heterosexual and same-sex couples. The participants, 232 Mexican college students, evaluated physical and psychological IPV exerted by men and women with different sexual orientations. The data were analyzed using the Wilcoxon test. The results indicate that men evaluated women and gay men as having a similar IPV, while men´s perceptions of IPV for these groups were higher than those of women. Women viewed heterosexual men as the most violent and evaluated the other groups with different degrees of IPV. Physical violence is regarded as natural in men, both gay and heterosexual. To conclude, the results suggest that IPV stereotypes are affected by the sex of the evaluators and by their sexual orientation. It is relevant to expand the scope of prevention programs.
Collapse
|
4
|
Tantleff-Dunn S, Barnes RD, Larose JG. It's not just a "woman thing:" the current state of normative discontent. Eat Disord 2011; 19:392-402. [PMID: 21932970 PMCID: PMC3760219 DOI: 10.1080/10640266.2011.609088] [Citation(s) in RCA: 84] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
This study assessed "normative discontent," the concept that most women experience weight dissatisfaction, as an emerging societal stereotype for women and men (Rodin, Silberstein, & Streigel-Moore, 1984). Participants (N = 472) completed measures of stereotypes, eating disorders, and body image. Normative discontent stereotypes were pervasive for women and men. Endorsing stereotypes varied by sex and participants' own disturbance, with trends towards eating disorder symptomotology being positively correlated with stereotype endorsement. Individuals with higher levels of body image and eating disturbance may normalize their behavior by perceiving that most people share their experiences. Future research needs to test prevention and intervention strategies that incorporate the discrepancies between body image/eating-related stereotypes and reality with focus on preventing normalization of such experiences.
Collapse
|
5
|
Felin T, Foss NJ. Social Reality, the Boundaries of Self-Fulfilling Prophecy, and Economics. ORGANIZATION SCIENCE 2009. [DOI: 10.1287/orsc.1090.0431] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
6
|
Krueger JI, Hall JH, Villano P, Jones MC. Attribution and Categorization Effects in the Representation of Gender Stereotypes. GROUP PROCESSES & INTERGROUP RELATIONS 2008. [DOI: 10.1177/1368430208092542] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Social stereotypes involve judgments of how typical certain personality traits are of a group. According to the attribution hypothesis, judgments of trait typicality depend on the perceived prevalence of the trait in the target group. According to the categorization hypothesis, such judgments depend on the degree to which a trait is thought to be more or less prevalent in the target group than in a relevant comparison group. A study conducted with women and men as target groups showed that the attribution hypothesis fit the data best when typicality ratings were made in an absolute format. When, however, typicality ratings were made in a comparative format (how typical is the trait of women as compared with men?), both hypotheses received support. Analytical derivation, supported by empirical evidence, showed an inverse relationship between the size of perceived group differences and their weight given in stereotyping. Implications for stereotype measurement and the rationality of social perception are discussed.
Collapse
|
7
|
Boseovski JJ, Lee K. Seeing The World Through Rose-colored Glasses? Neglect of Consensus Information in Young Children's Personality Judgments. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2008; 17:399-416. [PMID: 20930951 PMCID: PMC2949979 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00431.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined the use of consensus information in early childhood. Ninety-six three- to six-year-olds watched a demonstration that depicted the positive or negative behavior of one or several actors toward a recipient (low vs. high consensus, respectively). Subsequently, participants made behavioral predictions and personality judgments about the actors and recipients. Participants viewed all story characters favorably and were reluctant to assign blame for negative outcomes, although the appropriate use of consensus information increased with age for behavioral predictions. These findings suggest that there is a positivity bias in young children's personality judgments even in the face of explicit contradictory behavioral evidence. Children's early `theory of personality' is apparently driven by a baseline assumption that people are nice.
Collapse
|
8
|
Krueger JI, DiDonato TE. Social Categorization and the Perception of Groups and Group Differences. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2008. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00083.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
9
|
Accuracy in Social Perception: Criticisms, Controversies, Criteria, Components, and Cognitive Processes. ADVANCES IN EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2601(05)37001-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
|