1
|
Maor R. Profiles of Peer-Rejected Individuals: Their Attitudes toward the Intellectual Disability Population and the Mediating Role of Resilience. J Genet Psychol 2024; 185:323-336. [PMID: 38192068 DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2024.2301943] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2023] [Accepted: 01/02/2024] [Indexed: 01/10/2024]
Abstract
Literature shows that public perceptions toward people with intellectual disability (ID) tend to be negative, perhaps more so than toward any other population with disabilities, causing severe consequences on the quality of life of these people. Understanding factors associated with these attitudes may contribute to better integration of the ID population into society. This study focuses on the predictive role of school-age peer rejection and contemporaneous familial support on attitudes toward people with ID. Specifically, it aims to identify distinct profiles of people who experienced peer rejection during their school years and who experienced various levels of familial support during the rejection period, and to examine the disparities between these profiles regarding attitudes toward people with ID. An additional goal is to examine whether resilience can mediate the association between profiles of peer-rejected individuals and their attitudes toward individuals with ID. The research sample comprised 1063 Israeli adults reporting various levels of peer rejection during school years. Cluster analysis revealed two profiles of peer-rejected individuals that significantly differ in the level of familial support provided in the face of peer rejection. In accordance with the hypotheses, attitudes of peer-rejected individuals with poor familial support were more negative than those with high familial support and the mediating effect of resilience was significant. Findings emphasize the protective role of familial support in the face of peer rejection, contributing to the emerging literature that deals with the long-term effects of peer rejection and poor resiliency resources on negative attitudes toward out-groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rotem Maor
- Education, David Yellin College of Education, Jerusalem, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Park JH, Jin KS. The sense of belonging reduces ingroup favoritism in children. Front Psychol 2022; 13:1059415. [DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1059415] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2022] [Accepted: 11/04/2022] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
Belonging is an important motive for intergroup behavior. Adults display pronounced ingroup favoritism when the sense of inclusion by an ingroup is decreased or threatened. The present study investigated whether ingroup belonging reduces ingroup favoritism in 6-year-old children in terms of costly sharing. Children were allocated to a novel group in a minimal-group paradigm. In two conditions, children played a brief ball-tossing game and were either included (ingroup-inclusion condition) or excluded (ingroup-exclusion condition) by their ingroup members. Children in a no-interaction condition did not have any interactions with the members of the ingroup. After this manipulation, we tested the extent to which children shared resources with ingroup and outgroup members. We found that children in the ingroup-exclusion and no-interaction conditions shared more resources with their ingroup member than their outgroup member, while children in the ingroup-inclusion condition shared equally with the ingroup and outgroup members. These results could inform interventions aimed at fostering positive intergroup relations.
Collapse
|
3
|
The moderating effects of group dominance and religiosity on the relationship between social rejection during school years and attitudes toward minorities in adulthood. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-022-03356-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
4
|
Yee KM, Glidden J, Killen M. Group Norms Influence Children's Expectations About Status Based on Wealth and Popularity. Front Psychol 2022; 13:816205. [PMID: 35645917 PMCID: PMC9131005 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.816205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2021] [Accepted: 03/29/2022] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Children’s understanding of status and group norms influence their expectations about social encounters. However, status is multidimensional and children may perceive status stratification (i.e., high- and low-status) differently across multiple status dimensions (i.e., wealth and popularity). The current study investigated the effect of status level and norms on children’s expectations about intergroup affiliation in wealth and popularity contexts. Participants (N = 165; age range: 5–10 years; Mage = 7.72 years) were randomly assigned to hear two scenarios where a high- or low-status target affiliated with opposite-status groups based on either wealth or popularity. In one scenario, the group expressed an inclusive norm. In the other scenario, the group expressed an exclusive norm. For each scenario, children made predictions about children’s expectations for a target to acquire social resources. Novel findings indicated that children associated wealth status to some extent, but they drew stronger inferences from the wealth dimension than from the popularity dimension. In contrast to previous evidence that children distinguish between high- and low-status groups, we did not find evidence to support this in the context of the current study. In addition, norms of exclusion diminished children’s expectations for acquiring social resources from wealth and popularity groups but this effect was more pronounced between wealth groups. We found age differences in children’s expectations in regards to norms, but not in regards to status. The implications of how these effects, in addition to lack of effects, bear on children’s expectations about acquiring resources are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn M Yee
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States
| | - Jacquelyn Glidden
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States
| | - Melanie Killen
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Aral T, Schachner MK, Juang L, Schwarzenthal M. Cultural diversity approaches in schools and adolescents' willingness to support refugee youth. BRITISH JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021; 92:e12458. [PMID: 34514583 DOI: 10.1111/bjep.12458] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/10/2020] [Revised: 08/19/2021] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Culturally diverse schools contribute to adolescents' intergroup relations. Complex and inclusive social identities are mechanisms that can explain the link between structural school cultural diversity (i.e., proportion of students of immigrant descent and the number of different ethnic groups) and positive intergroup relations. We expected that similar mechanisms might be at play linking cultural diversity approaches in schools with adolescents' intergroup relations. AIM We examined the link between two sub-dimensions of cultural diversity approaches (i.e., equal treatment; heritage and intercultural learning) and adolescents' prosocial intentions and behaviour towards refugee youth. Then, we explored the mediating role of identity inclusiveness (i.e., perceived similarity of the self with others). SAMPLE AND METHODS We sampled culturally diverse eighth grade adolescents from 54 classrooms in Berlin (N = 503, Mage = 13.76 years, 50.6% female). Surveys measured perceived cultural diversity norms, adolescents' perceived identity inclusiveness with refugee youth, prosocial intentions to support refugee youth, and willingness to donate to a project for refugee youth. RESULTS Multilevel models revealed that adolescents' perception of heritage and intercultural learning predicted adolescents' prosocial intentions towards refugee youth, but not their willingness to donate. Equal treatment was not a significant predictor of adolescents' prosocial intentions towards refugee youth, or their willingness to donate. Identity inclusiveness did not mediate the relation between cultural diversity approaches and prosocial intentions. However, identity inclusiveness did positively relate adolescents' prosocial intentions and willingness to donate. CONCLUSIONS We conclude that culturally diverse schools that engage in heritage and intercultural learning might help to promote positive relations between local and refugee youth in schools and society. Fostering inclusive identities may enhance local adolescent's prosocial intention and behaviour.
Collapse
|
6
|
Forbes H, Stark AM, Hopkins SW, Fireman GD. The Effects of Group Membership on College Students' Social Exclusion of Peers and Bystander Behavior. THE JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2019; 154:15-37. [PMID: 31361210 DOI: 10.1080/00223980.2019.1642839] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Bystanders represent one major avenue for reducing the incidence and severity of social exclusion, yet little research has examined behavioral measurement of bystander intervention. Utilizing the most common low risk form of exclusion, this study examined how group membership impacts college students' behavioral response to a peer's social exclusion through an Internet-based ball tossing game (N = 121). Participants played the game with three other virtual players, in which two of these players excluded the third player. Results demonstrated increased inclusive behavior towards the excluded peer across study conditions. This inclusion was strengthened when the excluded player was in the participant's in-group. Participants displayed an initial preference for in-group members, although attitudes towards all peers improved after the shared activity. Findings point to the interaction of social norms of inclusion, group membership, and changes in familiarity in determining bystander responses to social exclusion. In low-risk exclusion, group membership maintains an impact but does not provide sufficient motivation to counteract the social norm of inclusivity. The implication of bystander actions for promotion of community and future research are discussed.
Collapse
|
7
|
Affiliation(s)
- Melanie Killen
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Rutland A, Abrams D, Levy S. Introduction: Extending the conversation: Transdisciplinary approaches to social identity and intergroup attitudes in children and adolescents. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/0165025407083669] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
9
|
Mulvey KL. Children's Reasoning About Social Exclusion: Balancing Many Factors. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12157] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
10
|
Watson-Jones RE, Whitehouse H, Legare CH. In-Group Ostracism Increases High-Fidelity Imitation in Early Childhood. Psychol Sci 2015; 27:34-42. [DOI: 10.1177/0956797615607205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 102] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/16/2014] [Accepted: 08/28/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
The Cyberball paradigm was used to examine the hypothesis that children use high-fidelity imitation as a reinclusion behavior in response to being ostracized by in-group members. Children ( N = 176; 5- to 6-year-olds) were either included or excluded by in- or out-group members and then shown a video of an in-group or an out-group member enacting a social convention. Participants who were excluded by their in-group engaged in higher-fidelity imitation than those who were included by their in-group. Children who were included by an out-group and those who were excluded by an out-group showed no difference in imitative fidelity. Children ostracized by in-group members also displayed increased anxiety relative to children ostracized by out-group members. The data are consistent with the proposal that high-fidelity imitation functions as reinclusion behavior in the context of in-group ostracism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Harvey Whitehouse
- Institute of Cognitive and Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Oxford
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Feddes AR, Monteiro MB, Justo MG. Subjective social status and intergroup attitudes among ethnic majority and minority children in Portugal. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2013; 32:125-40. [DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2013] [Revised: 10/30/2013] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Allard R. Feddes
- Department of Social Psychology; University of Amsterdam; the Netherlands
| | - Maria-Benedicta Monteiro
- Centre for Psychological Research and Social Intervention; Lisbon University Institute (ISCTE-IUL); Lisbon Portugal
| | - Mariline G. Justo
- Centre for Psychological Research and Social Intervention; Lisbon University Institute (ISCTE-IUL); Lisbon Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Cooley S, Elenbaas L, Killen M. Moral judgments and emotions: adolescents' evaluations in intergroup social exclusion contexts. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013; 2012:41-57, 8-9. [PMID: 23359443 DOI: 10.1002/yd.20037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
This article examines children's moral judgments and emotional evaluations in the context of social exclusion. As they age, children and adolescents face increasingly complex situations in which group membership and allegiance are in opposition with morally relevant decisions, such as the exclusion of an individual from a group. While adolescents are often characterized as being conformists to group norms, research demonstrates that their judgments about fairness, justice, and rights can supersede negative or exclusive norms espoused by groups. Additionally, young people's emotional evaluations of members who do not conform to a group norm are in concert with these fairness judgments. Implications for social and moral development will be discussed in the context of empirical findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shelby Cooley
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
MÄHÖNEN TUULIANNA, JASINSKAJA-LAHTI INGA, LIEBKIND KARMELA. The Impact of Perceived Social Norms, Gender, and Intergroup Anxiety on the Relationship Between Intergroup Contact and Ethnic Attitudes of Adolescents1. JOURNAL OF APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00793.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
14
|
Sapouna M, Wolke D, Vannini N, Watson S, Woods S, Schneider W, Enz S, Aylett R. Individual and social network predictors of the short-term stability of bullying victimization in the United Kingdom and Germany. BRITISH JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011; 82:225-40. [PMID: 22583088 DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02022.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND There is still relatively little research on the social context within which bullying develops and remains stable. AIM This study examined the short-term stability of bullying victimization among primary school students in the United Kingdom and Germany (mean age, 8.9 years) and the individual and social network factors that contributed to remaining a victim of bullying. SAMPLE The sample consisted of 454 children (247 males and 207 females). METHODS Participants completed questionnaires on bullying victimization at three assessment points over a 9-week period. Other measures consisted of self-reported demographic, peer, and family relationship characteristics. Social network indices of density, reciprocity, and hierarchy were constructed using friendship and peer acceptance nominations. RESULTS Relative risk analyses indicated a six-fold increased risk of remaining a victim at consequent follow-ups, compared to a child not victimized at baseline becoming a victim over the follow-up period. Individual characteristics explained substantially more variance in the stability of bullying victimization than class-level factors. Hierarchical logistic regression analyses revealed that being victimized by siblings and being rejected by peers predicted remaining a victim over a 9-week period. CONCLUSIONS Bullying victimization among primary school students proved moderately stable over a 9-week period. Individual characteristics were more influential in predicting the stable victim role than class-level factors. Our findings have implications for the identification of stable victims in primary school and early preventative bullying programs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Sapouna
- Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Nesdale D, Duffy A. Social identity, peer group rejection, and young children's reactive, displaced, and proactive aggression. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011; 29:823-41. [DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-835x.2010.02012.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
16
|
Nesdale D, Dalton D. Children's social groups and intergroup prejudice: assessing the influence and inhibition of social group norms. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011; 29:895-909. [PMID: 21995743 DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-835x.2010.02017.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
A simulation group study examined whether the effects of group norms on 7- and 9-year-old children's intergroup attitudes can be moderated by a contrary school norm. Children learnt that their school had an inclusion norm, were assigned to a group with an outgroup inclusion or exclusion norm, and indicated their ingroup and outgroup attitudes under teacher surveillance or not. Results revealed reduced outgroup liking when the group had an exclusion norm, but that the effect was moderated when the school had an inclusion norm, especially among the older children. The participants' ingroup liking was also reduced, but teacher surveillance had no effect on attitudes. The findings are discussed in relation to possible strategies to moderate social group norm effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Drew Nesdale
- School of Psychology, Griffith University, Queensland, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Nesdale D, Maass A, Kiesner J, Durkin K, Griffiths J, James B. Effects of peer group rejection and a new group's norms on children's intergroup attitudes. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2010; 27:799-814. [DOI: 10.1348/026151008x381690] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
|
18
|
Nesdale D, Milliner E, Duffy A, Griffiths JA. Group membership, group norms, empathy, and young children's intentions to aggress. Aggress Behav 2009; 35:244-58. [PMID: 19309004 DOI: 10.1002/ab.20303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
This study assessed the effect of ingroup norms and empathy on 6 and 9-year-old children's (N=161) attitudes and aggressive intentions toward outgroup members. Prior to an intergroup drawing competition against an outgroup, participants' empathy was measured, and they were randomly assigned to a simulated group with a norm of direct or indirect aggression, or no aggression norm. Results indicated participants' attitudes were less positive toward the outgroup vs. the ingroup, and that both direct and indirect aggressive intentions were displayed toward the outgroup. Most importantly, the ingroup was liked less when it had an aggression norm, and the participants' aggressive intentions were not enhanced by the group aggression norm. Empathy was a significant negative predictor of direct but not indirect aggression intentions. Implications for understanding the instigation and inhibition of children's aggression intentions are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Drew Nesdale
- School of Psychology, Griffith University, Gold Coast, Queensland, Australia.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Nesdale D, Durkin K, Maass A, Kiesner J, Griffiths JA. Effects of Group Norms on Children's Intentions to Bully. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2008. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00475.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|