2
|
Murphy MC, Mejia AF, Mejia J, Yan X, Cheryan S, Dasgupta N, Destin M, Fryberg SA, Garcia JA, Haines EL, Harackiewicz JM, Ledgerwood A, Moss-Racusin CA, Park LE, Perry SP, Ratliff KA, Rattan A, Sanchez DT, Savani K, Sekaquaptewa D, Smith JL, Taylor VJ, Thoman DB, Wout DA, Mabry PL, Ressl S, Diekman AB, Pestilli F. Open science, communal culture, and women's participation in the movement to improve science. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2020; 117:24154-24164. [PMID: 32929006 PMCID: PMC7533847 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1921320117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2019] [Accepted: 07/27/2020] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Science is undergoing rapid change with the movement to improve science focused largely on reproducibility/replicability and open science practices. This moment of change-in which science turns inward to examine its methods and practices-provides an opportunity to address its historic lack of diversity and noninclusive culture. Through network modeling and semantic analysis, we provide an initial exploration of the structure, cultural frames, and women's participation in the open science and reproducibility literatures (n = 2,926 articles and conference proceedings). Network analyses suggest that the open science and reproducibility literatures are emerging relatively independently of each other, sharing few common papers or authors. We next examine whether the literatures differentially incorporate collaborative, prosocial ideals that are known to engage members of underrepresented groups more than independent, winner-takes-all approaches. We find that open science has a more connected, collaborative structure than does reproducibility. Semantic analyses of paper abstracts reveal that these literatures have adopted different cultural frames: open science includes more explicitly communal and prosocial language than does reproducibility. Finally, consistent with literature suggesting the diversity benefits of communal and prosocial purposes, we find that women publish more frequently in high-status author positions (first or last) within open science (vs. reproducibility). Furthermore, this finding is further patterned by team size and time. Women are more represented in larger teams within reproducibility, and women's participation is increasing in open science over time and decreasing in reproducibility. We conclude with actionable suggestions for cultivating a more prosocial and diverse culture of science.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mary C Murphy
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47405;
| | - Amanda F Mejia
- Department of Statistics, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47408
| | - Jorge Mejia
- Kelley School of Business, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47405
| | - Xiaoran Yan
- Network Science Institute, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47408
| | - Sapna Cheryan
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195
| | - Nilanjana Dasgupta
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA 01003
| | - Mesmin Destin
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208
- Institute for Policy Research, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208
- School of Education & Social Policy, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208
| | - Stephanie A Fryberg
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor, Ann Arbor, MI 48109
| | - Julie A Garcia
- Department of Psychology and Child Development, California Polytechnic State University, San Luis Obispo, CA 93407
| | | | | | | | | | - Lora E Park
- Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260
| | - Sylvia P Perry
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208
- Institute for Policy Research, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208
- Department of Medical Social Sciences, Northeastern University, Evanston, IL 60208
| | - Kate A Ratliff
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611
| | - Aneeta Rattan
- Organisational Behaviour, London Business School, London NW1 4SA, United Kingdom
| | - Diana T Sanchez
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Piscataway, NJ 08854
| | - Krishna Savani
- Leadership, Management & Organisation, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 639798
| | - Denise Sekaquaptewa
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor, Ann Arbor, MI 48109
| | - Jessi L Smith
- Office of Research, University of Colorado Colorado Springs, Colorado Springs, CO 80918
- Department of Psychology, University of Colorado Colorado Springs, Colorado Springs, CO 80918
| | - Valerie Jones Taylor
- Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA 18015
- Africana Studies, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA 18015
| | - Dustin B Thoman
- Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182
| | - Daryl A Wout
- Department of Psychology, John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, New York, NY 10019
| | - Patricia L Mabry
- Research Division, HealthPartners Institute, Bloomington, MN 55425
| | - Susanne Ressl
- Department of Molecular and Cellular Biochemistry, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47405
- Department of Neuroscience, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712
| | - Amanda B Diekman
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47405
| | - Franco Pestilli
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47405
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Zárate MA, Hall GN, Plaut VC. Researchers of Color, Fame, and Impact. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2018; 12:1176-1178. [PMID: 29149575 DOI: 10.1177/1745691617710511] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Fame and eminence, as traditionally measured, limit the definition of impact to the publication world. We add two types of impact to the traditional measures of fame and eminence. Many of the traditional measures of fame or eminence are based on social-network connections, whereby individuals appoint other people to positions of eminence. Editorial boards are one specific example. Eminence is also limited to number of publications, for example, with little regard for the impact of those publications at the societal level. In addition to the dominant measures of eminence, societal impact broadens the definition of impact to reflect real-world changes. Two examples include mentoring, which is rarely mentioned as a criterion for eminence, and policy value, such as when research influences important public policy. These additions are discussed in reference to the general underrepresentation of researchers of color in academia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Victoria C Plaut
- 3 Department of Psychology, School of Law, University of California, Berkeley
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Marsh EJ. Family Matters: Measuring Impact Through One's Academic Descendants. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2017; 12:1130-1132. [PMID: 29149581 DOI: 10.1177/1745691617719759] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Scientific contributions take many forms, not all of which result in fame or are captured in traditional metrics of success (e.g., h factor). My focus is on one of the most lasting and important contributions a scientist can make: training scientists who go on to train scientists, who in turn train more scientists, etc. Academic genealogies provide many examples of scientists whose names might not be recognizable today but who trained psychologists that went on to publish very influential work. Of course success results from a combination of many factors (including but not limited to the student's abilities and motivation, luck, institutional resources, mentoring, etc.), but the field should find more ways to acknowledge the role that mentoring does play.
Collapse
|
9
|
Ruscio J. Taking Advantage of Citation Measures of Scholarly Impact. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2016; 11:905-908. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691616664436] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Professional decisions about hiring, tenure, promotion, funding, and honors are informed by assessments of scholarly impact. As a measure of influence, citations are produced by experts but accessible to nonexperts. The h index is the largest number h such that an individual has published at least h works cited at least h times apiece. This is easy to understand and calculate, as or more reliable and valid than alternative citation measures, and highly robust to missing or messy data. Striving for a large h index requires both productivity and influence, which provides healthy incentives for researchers striving for eminence through scientific impact. A number of factors that can influence h are discussed to promote the mindful use of what might otherwise be an ambiguous or misleading measure. The h index adds a transparent, objective component to assessments of scholarly impact, and even academic eminence, that merits at least two cheers.
Collapse
|