1
|
Coia AJ, Arizpe JM, Smith PA, Kuyk TK, Lovell JA. Measurements of chromatic adaptation and luminous efficiency while wearing colored filters. J Vis 2024; 24:9. [PMID: 39392444 PMCID: PMC11472893 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2023] [Accepted: 08/03/2024] [Indexed: 10/12/2024] Open
Abstract
The visual system adapts dynamically to stabilize perception over widely varying illuminations. Such adaptation allows the colors of objects to appear constant despite changes in spectral illumination. Similarly, the wearing of colored filters also alters spectral content, but this alteration can be more extreme than typically encountered in nature, presenting a unique challenge to color constancy mechanisms. While it is known that chromatic adaptation is affected by surrounding spatial context, a recent study reported a gradual temporal adaptation effect to colored filters such that colors initially appear strongly shifted but over hours of wear are perceived as closer to an unfiltered appearance. Presently, it is not clear whether the luminance system adapts spatially and temporally like the chromatic system. To address this, spatial and temporal adaptation effects to a colored filter were measured using tasks that assess chromatic and luminance adaptation separately. Prior to and for 1 hour after putting on a pair of colored filters, participants made achromatic and heterochromatic flicker photometry (HFP) settings to measure chromatic and luminance adaptation, respectively. Results showed significant chromatic adaptation with achromatic settings moving closer to baseline settings over 1 hour of wearing the filters and greater adaptation with spatial context. Conversely, there was no significant luminance adaptation and HFP matches fell close to what was predicted photometrically. The results are discussed in the context of prior studies of chromatic and luminance adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew J Coia
- Science Applications International Corporation, JBSA Fort Sam Houston, TX, USA
| | - Joseph M Arizpe
- Science Applications International Corporation, JBSA Fort Sam Houston, TX, USA
| | - Peter A Smith
- Science Applications International Corporation, JBSA Fort Sam Houston, TX, USA
| | - Thomas K Kuyk
- Science Applications International Corporation, JBSA Fort Sam Houston, TX, USA
| | - Julie A Lovell
- Air Force Research Laboratory, 711th Human Performance Wing, Bioeffects Division, JBSA Fort Sam Houston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Gegenfurtner KR, Weiss D, Bloj M. Color constancy in real-world settings. J Vis 2024; 24:12. [PMID: 38411957 PMCID: PMC10910556 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.2.12] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2023] [Accepted: 12/12/2023] [Indexed: 02/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Color constancy denotes the ability to assign a particular and stable color percept to an object, irrespective of its surroundings and illumination. The light reaching the eye confounds illumination and spectral reflectance of the object, making the recovery of constant object color an ill-posed problem. How good the visual system is at accomplishing this task is still a matter of heated debate, despite more than a 100 years of research. Depending on the laboratory task and the specific cues available to observers, color constancy was found to be at levels ranging between 15% and 80%, which seems incompatible with the relatively stable color appearance of objects around us and the consistent usage of color names in real life. Here, we show close-to-perfect color constancy using real objects in a natural task and natural environmental conditions, chosen to mimic the role of color constancy in everyday life. Participants had to identify the color of a (non-present) item familiar to them in an office room under five different experimental illuminations. They mostly selected the same colored Munsell chip as their match to the absent object, even though the light reaching the eye in each case differed substantially. Our results demonstrate that color constancy under ideal conditions in the real world can indeed be exceptionally good. We found it to be as good as visual memory permits and not generally compromised by sensory uncertainty.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karl R Gegenfurtner
- Department of Psychology, Giessen University, Giessen, Germany
- https://www.allpsych.uni-giessen.de/karl/
| | - David Weiss
- Department of Psychology, Giessen University, Giessen, Germany
| | - Marina Bloj
- Faculty of Health Studies, University of Bradford, Bradford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ma S, Teunissen K, Smet KAG. Adaptation under dichromatic illumination. OPTICS EXPRESS 2023; 31:40411-40434. [PMID: 38041343 DOI: 10.1364/oe.494090] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2023] [Accepted: 06/27/2023] [Indexed: 12/03/2023]
Abstract
Over the years, many CATs (chromatic adaptation transforms) have been developed, such as CMCCAT97, CAT02 and CAT16, to predict the corresponding colors under different illuminants. These CATs were derived from uniform simple stimuli surrounded by a uniform background with a single illuminant. Although some mixed adaptation models have been proposed in literature to predict the adaptation under more than one illuminant, these models are typically limited to a certain scene and exclude the impact of spatial complexity. To investigate chromatic adaptation under more complex conditions, an achromatic matching experiment was conducted with (simultaneously) spatially dichromatic illumination for three illumination color pairs and various spatial configurations. Spatial configuration was found to have an impact on both the degree of adaptation and the equivalent illuminant chromaticity, which is the chromaticity of a single uniform adapting illumination that results in the same corresponding colors as for the dichromatic lighting condition. A preliminary CAT model is proposed that considers the spatial and colorimetric complexity of the illumination.
Collapse
|
4
|
Ma S, Sun R, Liu Y, Wang Y, Song W. Effect of surrounding objects in the adapting scene on chromatic adaptation. OPTICS EXPRESS 2023; 31:18587-18598. [PMID: 37381568 DOI: 10.1364/oe.489341] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2023] [Accepted: 05/06/2023] [Indexed: 06/30/2023]
Abstract
Most of the existing chromatic adaptation transforms (CATs) were developed for flat uniform stimuli presented in a uniform background, which substantially simplifies the complexity of the real scene by excluding surrounding objects from the viewing field. The impact of the background complexity, in terms of the spatial properties of the objects surrounding the stimulus, on chromatic adaptation is ignored in most CATs. This study systematically investigated how the background complexity and color distribution affect the adaptation state. Achromatic matching experiments were conducted in an immersive lighting booth, with the illumination varying in chromaticity and the adapting scene varying in surrounding objects. Results show that compared to the uniform adapting field, increasing the scene complexity can significantly improve the degree of adaptation for the Planckian illuminations with low CCT levels. In addition, the achromatic matching points are substantially biased by the color of the surrounding object, implying the interactive effect of the illumination color and the dominant scene color on the adapting white point.
Collapse
|
5
|
Ma S, Li S, Sun R, Liu Y, Wang Y, Song W. Transition type influences the adaptation state under dichromatic illuminations. OPTICS EXPRESS 2022; 30:47134-47151. [PMID: 36558650 DOI: 10.1364/oe.478648] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2022] [Accepted: 11/29/2022] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
The CATs models proposed over these years (such as CMCCAT97, CAT02 and CAT16) were derived from simple stimuli surrounded by a uniform background with a single illuminant. However, the real scene always consists of more than one illumination, especially in many artificially lit environments. Some previous studies indicate an influence of the transition type between two illuminations on the adaptation state, but the visual data is insufficient to conclude a general trend applicable for any color pair. To systematically investigate how the transition type and illumination color pair interactively influence the adapted white point and degree of adaptation, a series of achromatic matching experiments were conducted under (simultaneously) spatially dichromatic illuminations. Transition type was found to have an impact on the adaptation state, but it is significant only for an illumination pair with a large color difference. In addition, for those sharp-transitioned dichromatic illuminations, the illumination that more easily gets adapted tends to have a higher contribution to the adapted white point than the other one. A more comprehensive CAT model for dichromatic illuminations was derived from the collected visual data.
Collapse
|
6
|
Candry P, De Visschere P, Neyts K. Line element for the perceptual color space. OPTICS EXPRESS 2022; 30:36307-36331. [PMID: 36258562 DOI: 10.1364/oe.468370] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2022] [Accepted: 08/15/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
It is generally accepted that the perceptual color space is not Euclidean. A new line element for a 3-dimensional Riemannian color space was developed. This line element is based on the Friele line elements and psychophysical color discrimination models, and comprises both the first and second stage of color vision. The line element is expressed in a contrast space based on the MacLeod-Boynton chromaticities. New equations for the contrast thresholds along the cardinal axes and new metric tensor elements were determined. Visual adaptation effects were incorporated into the model. Color discrimination threshold ellipsoids were calculated with the new line element. Adequate agreement with experimental threshold ellipsoids reported in literature was demonstrated. From a comparison with other color difference metrics a better overall predictability of threshold ellipsoids was found with the new line element.
Collapse
|
7
|
Preferred skin tones on mobile displays under illuminants having different correlated colour temperatures. Vision Res 2022; 198:108060. [PMID: 35561464 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2022.108060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2021] [Revised: 04/08/2022] [Accepted: 04/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
A psychophysical experiment was conducted to investigate preferred skin tones of 4 skin types under 5 illuminants varying in correlated colour temperatures. The experiments were performed on calibrated mobile display devices. Four facial skin tone images representing each skin type were captured and were rendered to 25 skin tone images to cover a large colour region. The facial skin tone images were assessed by a panel of 28 Chinese observers using an alternative forced choice psychophysical method. The ellipses based on observer preference rate were derived, and the preferred skin tone centres and 50% tolerance ellipse of different skin types under each test illuminants were obtained. According to the colour appearance shift of preferred skin tones under illuminants with varying CCTs, all data were transformed and compared in the a*b* plane under D65 and CIE 1931 Standard Colorimetric Observer condition. The results on the impact of illuminants on preferred skin tones were reported here. All types of skin tones had a good agreement under different illuminants.
Collapse
|
8
|
Ma S, Teunissen K, Smet KAG. Predictive performance of the standard and the modified von Kries chromatic adaptation transforms. OPTICS EXPRESS 2022; 30:11872-11891. [PMID: 35473122 DOI: 10.1364/oe.451461] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2021] [Accepted: 03/09/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
To investigate chromatic adaptation and develop chromatic adaptation transforms (CATs), many psychophysical experiments have been conducted to collect corresponding colors (CC) under various illumination conditions. Most modern CATs have been developed based on a database of CC sets collected in the 20th century. More recently, several additional CC sets have been collected by Smet et al., Wei et al., and Ma et al. using memory color matching or achromatic matching methods. The analysis of these CC data indicates that for yellowish (located on or close to the Planckian locus) and greenish illuminations, the short-wave (S) sensitive cones show a lower degree of adaptation compared to the long-wave (L) and medium-wave (M) sensitive cones. This can result in a large prediction error of the standard von Kries CAT, which adopts a single degree of adaptation value for all three cone types. A modified von Kries CAT is proposed that accounts for these differences between the L-, M- and S-cone signals by applying a compression to the rescaling factor for the S-cones. It outperforms the standard von Kries CAT for the Breneman-C, Smet, Wei, and Ma data, while for other data sources the two CATs have similar performance.
Collapse
|
9
|
Zhang J, Xu H, Jiang H. Study of display white point based on mixed chromatic adaptation. OPTICS EXPRESS 2022; 30:9181-9192. [PMID: 35299353 DOI: 10.1364/oe.452948] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/04/2022] [Accepted: 02/22/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
In this study, the settings of the display white points were investigated, which presented the color appearance matched with a neutral surface as observed in the state of mixed chromatic adaptation. A psychophysical experiment was conducted under 20 illumination and viewing conditions via successive binocular color matching. It is discovered that the metameric light sources have generally equivalent effects on the observers' adaptation states and the resulting white points. The correlated color temperature (CCT) of the illumination and the adapting luminance, both with a significant influence on the mixed chromatic adaptation, exhibit a positive and a negative relation to the white point CCT, respectively. The immersive illumination affects the white point through the adaptation ratio and the baseline illuminant. Finally, the experimental results were verified to be predictable with an amended mixed chromatic adaptation model, which produced a mean chromaticity error of only 0.0027 units of CIE 1976 u'v'.
Collapse
|
10
|
Preciado OU, Martin A, Manzano E, Smet KAG, Hanselaer P. CAM18sl brightness prediction for unrelated saturated stimuli including age effects. OPTICS EXPRESS 2021; 29:29257-29274. [PMID: 34615039 DOI: 10.1364/oe.431382] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2021] [Accepted: 08/13/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Modelling the influence of age on the perception of brightness of visual stimuli is an important topic for indoor and outdoor lighting. As people get older, the transmittance of the ocular media becomes lower, especially in the blue wavelength region. This paper reports on an experimental study aiming to evaluate how the brightness perception of red and blue stimuli is affected by the age of the observer. A matching experiment has been set up in which both young (25 years old on average) and older (70 years old on average) adult observers had to match the brightness of a blue stimulus with the brightness of a red stimulus, both surrounded by a dark background (unrelated stimuli). A significant difference in brightness perception between the two groups of observers was found. In particular, older people report a decrease in brightness perception for the blue stimuli compared to younger people. The results show that the brightness correlate of the colour appearance model CAM18sl (applied with zero luminance background) adequately predicts the matching results of young observers, but failed to predict the results obtained by the older observers. As CAM18sl is built on cone fundamentals which include the transmittance of the ocular media and consider the age of the observer as an input parameter, the authors developed the idea to substitute the cone fundamentals for a young observer by the cone fundamentals for a 70 years old observer. This updated CAM18sl performed very well for the older observer as well, on condition that the transmittance of the ocular media is isolated and kept out of the normalization of the cone fundamentals.
Collapse
|
11
|
Babilon S, Klabes J, Myland P, Khanh TQ. Memory colors and the assessment of color quality in lighting applications. OPTICS EXPRESS 2021; 29:28968-28993. [PMID: 34615017 DOI: 10.1364/oe.426774] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2021] [Accepted: 07/26/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Due to their potential use as an internal reference, memory colors may provide an excellent approach for the color rendition evaluation of white light sources in terms of predicting visual appreciation. Because of certain limitations in the design of existing memory-related color quality measures, a new metric based on the outcome of a series of recently conducted memory color appearance rating experiments is proposed in this work. In order to validate its predictive performance, a meta-correlation analysis on a comprehensive set of preference rating data collected from literature is performed. Results indicate that the new metric proposal outperforms established color quality measures and is capable of correctly predicting the rank order of light sources in different lighting scenarios. The future inclusion of this new metric into a comprehensive lighting quality model may serve as a valuable tool for the lighting designer to create optimally lit environments for humans that do not only support the visual task fulfillment but also increase the users' well-being and emotional comfort by rendering the perceived space in such a way that it complies with the people's inherent memory references.
Collapse
|
12
|
Safdar M, Hardeberg JY, Ronnier Luo M. ZCAM, a colour appearance model based on a high dynamic range uniform colour space. OPTICS EXPRESS 2021; 29:6036-6052. [PMID: 33726134 DOI: 10.1364/oe.413659] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2020] [Accepted: 01/25/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
A colour appearance model based on a uniform colour space is proposed. The proposed colour appearance model, ZCAM, comprises of comparatively simple mathematical equations, and plausibly agrees with the psychophysical phenomenon of colour appearance perception. ZCAM consists of ten colour appearance attributes including brightness, lightness, colourfulness, chroma, hue angle, hue composition, saturation, vividness, blackness, and whiteness. Despite its relatively simpler mathematical structure, ZCAM performed at least similar to the CIE standard colour appearance model CIECAM02 and its revision, CAM16, in predicting a range of reliable experimental data.
Collapse
|
13
|
Bai X, Liao N, Wu W, Song W. Color reproduction method based on neural networks and visual matching. APPLIED OPTICS 2020; 59:11027-11033. [PMID: 33361927 DOI: 10.1364/ao.406031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2020] [Accepted: 10/29/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
A new method is proposed to build up a color reproduction model based on neural networks and visual matching. Training data were obtained from a visual matching experiment and the commonly used CIECAM02 model to set up a link between the device parameters and human color perception. The process of training neural networks is presented. An experiment simulating an office environment was conducted, together with a user study, to verify the performance of the proposed method in identifying and reproducing color.
Collapse
|
14
|
Ma S, Hanselaer P, Teunissen K, Smet KAG. Effect of adapting field size on chromatic adaptation. OPTICS EXPRESS 2020; 28:17266-17285. [PMID: 32679938 DOI: 10.1364/oe.392844] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2020] [Accepted: 05/10/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
The human visual system adapts to changes in white tone of the illumination to maintain approximately the same object color appearance. Chromatic adaptation transforms (CAT) were developed to predict corresponding colors, which are colors that look the same under a wide range of illuminants. However, existing CATs fail to accurately predict corresponding colors, particularly under colored illumination, because of an inaccurate estimation of the degree of adaptation. In this study, the impact of the adapting field size on the degree of adaptation was investigated. A memory color matching experiment was conducted, in a real scene, with the background adapting field varying in the field of view, luminance and chromaticity to provide data for the development of a more comprehensive CAT. Results show that a larger field of view leads to a more complete adaptation, despite a much lower background luminance.
Collapse
|
15
|
Ma S, Hanselaer P, Teunissen K, Smet KAG. Impact of the starting point chromaticity on memory color matching accuracy. OPTICS EXPRESS 2019; 27:35308-35324. [PMID: 31878702 DOI: 10.1364/oe.27.035308] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2019] [Accepted: 11/08/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
In this study, the impact of starting point chromaticity and number of observers on memory color matching results was investigated. Matching data were obtained for 3 objects (neutral grey cube, yellow lemon and green apple) under a neutral white and a yellow background illumination. Memory color matchings were made for ten starting points of which eight chromaticities were symmetrically distributed along the hue circle and centered at the equal energy white (EEW) chromaticity of the neutral white background illumination; one starting point at the EEW chromaticity and one with the same chromaticity as the background. The matching track from starting point to the memory matched chromaticity was also recorded. It did not tend to cross over the central region towards the complementary hue, especially for experienced observers. The results also demonstrated a significant starting point bias, whereby the matched chromaticities were biased towards the chromaticity of the starting point. Starting point bias can be minimized by selecting three starting points symmetrically distributed around the expected memory color chromaticity. Furthermore, at least, ten observers are needed to achieve stable results for the grey cube and yellow lemon. For the green apple, the results are less conclusive and around 40 observers would be needed to obtain a stable average estimate for the chromaticity of the memory color. The large inter-observer variation may result from cultural differences or from natural variations in the "green" apple appearance. This study provides a well-founded guidance for future application of the memory color matching method.
Collapse
|
16
|
Hermans S, Smet KAG, Hanselaer P. Exploring the applicability of the CAM18sl brightness prediction. OPTICS EXPRESS 2019; 27:14423-14436. [PMID: 31163892 DOI: 10.1364/oe.27.014423] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2019] [Accepted: 04/26/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
Recently, a color appearance model, CAM18sl, has been published. The model can predict the color perception (hue, colorfulness, brightness) of self-luminous stimuli seen against neutral backgrounds varying in luminance. In this paper, the applicability of CAM18sl to situations different from the one for which it was originally developed is explored. The brightness perception predicted by this model (expressed in "bright") is compared with the outcome of the CIE Unified Glare Rating (UGR) for luminaires, the limiting luminance values for Variable Message Signs as indicated in the European standard for outdoor situations, the visual gloss (VG) formula to characterize the gloss of objects, the CIE grey-scale calculation for self-luminous devices, and the predicted brightness value of illuminated objects according to CIECAM02. Although the application domains mentioned above are very distinct, it appears that the predicted brightness perception of the CAM18sl model correlates well with the outcome of the particular and dedicated metrics. It seems that, for brightness perception, CAM18sl can be considered as a general model applicable for a wide range of applications. This could lead to a reduction of a number of application-specific metrics.
Collapse
|
17
|
Wei M, Chen S. Effects of adapting luminance and CCT on appearance of white and degree of chromatic adaptation. OPTICS EXPRESS 2019; 27:9276-9286. [PMID: 31052735 DOI: 10.1364/oe.27.009276] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2019] [Accepted: 03/11/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
Past studies reported that the degree of chromatic adaptation was affected by viewing medium and adapting luminance. In this study, human observers adjusted the color appearance of a stimulus produced by a self-luminous display to make it appear as white as possible under different adapting conditions, whose adapting luminance and Correlated Color Temperature (CCT) levels were systematically varied. Though an identical display was used as the viewing medium, the chromaticities adjusted under the high adapting luminance levels were generally around the adapting chromaticities, which was similar to the findings in the past studies using reflective surface color samples as the viewing medium. This suggested that the effect of the viewing medium, as reported in the past studies, was actually the effect of viewing mode, due to the change in adapting luminance. Furthermore, the adapting luminance and CCT were found to jointly affect the degree of chromatic adaptation, with a stronger effect of adapting luminance under a lower adapting CCT.
Collapse
|
18
|
Hermans S, Smet KAG, Hanselaer P. Color appearance model for self-luminous stimuli. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2018; 35:2000-2009. [PMID: 30645289 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.35.002000] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2018] [Accepted: 10/14/2018] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
A model for brightness and hue perception of self-luminous stimuli surrounded by a self-luminous achromatic background has been developed based on a series of visual experiments. In the model, only the absolute spectral radiance values of the stimulus and background are used as input. Normalized cone excitations are calculated using the 10° Commission Internationale de l'Éclairage (CIE) 2006 cone fundamentals. A von Kries chromatic adaptation transform applied in the CIE 2006 cone space is adopted, and luminance compression and adaptation due to the self-luminous background are included by using a Michaelis-Menten function. Model parameters are determined by fitting the model to the experimental visual data obtained for brightness, hue, and the amount of color versus neutral. The model is validated with additional experimental data. An absolute brightness scale expressed in "bright" is proposed.
Collapse
|
19
|
Zhai Q, Luo MR. Study of chromatic adaptation via neutral white matches on different viewing media. OPTICS EXPRESS 2018; 26:7724-7739. [PMID: 29609324 DOI: 10.1364/oe.26.007724] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2017] [Accepted: 03/05/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Two experiments were carried out to study the neutral white and the chromatic adaptation in human vision and color science. After matching neutral whites under different illuminants using both surface and self-luminous colors, the result were used to verify the previous study about the chromatic adaptation. Not all the white illuminants were found neutral even the adaptation time is long. The baseline illuminant of the two-step chromatic adaptation transform was found as the illuminant with the same chromaticity of the neutral white under it and depended on viewing medium in the present study. The results were also used as corresponding colors to derive models of the effective degree of chromatic adaptation, which were found highly associated with the chromaticity of the adapting illuminant.
Collapse
|
20
|
Huang HP, Wei M, Ou LC. White appearance of a tablet display under different ambient lighting conditions. OPTICS EXPRESS 2018; 26:5018-5030. [PMID: 29475344 DOI: 10.1364/oe.26.005018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2017] [Accepted: 02/13/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
In comparison to the great efforts made on the enhancement of image quality for tablet displays, little attention has been paid on the concept of white point. Given the increasing popularity of the light sources with chromaticities off the Planckian locus and color-tunable LED lighting, it is important to investigate human's white perception of tablet display under different ambient lighting conditions. This study investigated the white appearance of a tablet display under 17 ambient lighting conditions, including a dark condition, seven conditions with chromaticities on the Planckian locus, and nine conditions with chromaticities off the Planckian locus, (i.e., Duv = + 0.02, -0.02, and -0.04). It was found that both the white appearance boundary defined by the fitted one-standard-deviation error ellipse and the whitest stimulus rated by the observers or identified by the bivariate Gaussian distribution were different under the various ambient lighting conditions. The optimization based on the whitest stimulus under each ambient lighting condition suggested a lower degree of chromatic adaptation under the conditions with a lower Correlated Color Temperature (CCT). For the conditions with a same CCT, a Duv of -0.02 was found to provide a higher degree of chromatic adaptation than Duv values of + 0.02 and -0.04.
Collapse
|