1
|
Oleszkiewicz A, Pettke K, Olze H, Uecker FC, Hummel T. Effects of odors on posture. J SENS STUD 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/joss.12796] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Oleszkiewicz
- Smell and Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology TU Dresden Dresden Germany
- Institute of Psychology University of Wroclaw Wroclaw
| | - Karolina Pettke
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery Charité‐Universitätsmedizin Berlin Berlin Germany
| | - Heidi Olze
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery Charité‐Universitätsmedizin Berlin Berlin Germany
| | - Florian C. Uecker
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery Charité‐Universitätsmedizin Berlin Berlin Germany
| | - Thomas Hummel
- Smell and Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology TU Dresden Dresden Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Matsuda E, Misawa D, Yano S, Kondo T. Olfactory Cues to Reduce Retrograde Interference During the Simultaneous Learning of Conflicting Motor Tasks. JOURNAL OF ROBOTICS AND MECHATRONICS 2022. [DOI: 10.20965/jrm.2022.p0746] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
We investigated the ability of humans to adapt to a novel environment by kinematic transformation. This adaptation was studied via behavioural experiments using a robotic manipulandum – a system designed to arbitrarily generate virtual force fields against a human hand and subsequently record the hand’s trajectory. By repeating motor tasks, this study’s participants gradually learned to move correctly under a newly experienced force field, such as rotating in a clockwise direction. However, each participant’s motor memory was destroyed if he/she experienced an opposing force field (e.g., in a counterclockwise direction) immediately after learning the initial movement, which is known as retrograde interference. In some previous studies, it has been considered that by presenting sensory cues to highlight the difference in two opposing force fields, participants can learn both force fields independently without interference. In this study, we investigated the functionality of olfactory cues – specifically lemon and lavender odors – in reducing retrograde interference. Forty-five university students participated in an experiment using a robotic manipulandum. Our results have shown that the presence of lemon odor reduces the destruction of motor memory, while that of lavender did not, suggesting that odors can enhance simultaneous motor learning but the effect depends on the type of odor used.
Collapse
|
3
|
Abstract
Environmental stimuli can have a significant impact on our decisions. Elements of the store atmosphere, such as music, lights and smells, all have effects on choices, but these have been only vaguely investigated. In the present study, we aim to uncover the effect of strawberry scent on the gazing behavior and choices of the 62 recruited participants. A static eye-tracker was used to study the effect of scent, released by a diffuser. In total, 31 participants completed the study under odorless conditions, while another 31 participants had strawberry fragrance sprayed into the air. The objectives of the study were (1) to determine whether the most gazed-upon product in each of the four categories (chocolate, tea, muesli bar, yoghurt) was chosen, (2) whether the presence of the strawberry scent influenced consumer decision making, i.e., whether the strawberry scent influenced more people to choose strawberry-flavored products, and (3) to introduce the application of a fast and easy-to-use technique for the qualitative analysis of strawberry aroma present in the air during eye-tracking measurements. The results show that (1) participants chose the product they had studied the longest, for all four categories, and (2) the presence or absence of the scent had no significant effect on choice, with the same frequencies of choosing each product in the two conditions regardless of the flavor of the products.
Collapse
|
4
|
Motyka P, Akbal M, Litwin P. Forward optic flow is prioritised in visual awareness independently of walking direction. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0250905. [PMID: 33945563 PMCID: PMC8096117 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250905] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/09/2020] [Accepted: 04/15/2021] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
When two different images are presented separately to each eye, one experiences smooth transitions between them-a phenomenon called binocular rivalry. Previous studies have shown that exposure to signals from other senses can enhance the access of stimulation-congruent images to conscious perception. However, despite our ability to infer perceptual consequences from bodily movements, evidence that action can have an analogous influence on visual awareness is scarce and mainly limited to hand movements. Here, we investigated whether one's direction of locomotion affects perceptual access to optic flow patterns during binocular rivalry. Participants walked forwards and backwards on a treadmill while viewing highly-realistic visualisations of self-motion in a virtual environment. We hypothesised that visualisations congruent with walking direction would predominate in visual awareness over incongruent ones, and that this effect would increase with the precision of one's active proprioception. These predictions were not confirmed: optic flow consistent with forward locomotion was prioritised in visual awareness independently of walking direction and proprioceptive abilities. Our findings suggest the limited role of kinaesthetic-proprioceptive information in disambiguating visually perceived direction of self-motion and indicate that vision might be tuned to the (expanding) optic flow patterns prevalent in everyday life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paweł Motyka
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Mert Akbal
- Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- Academy of Fine Arts Saar, Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Piotr Litwin
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Yang Y, Wang X. Odor Modulates Hand Movements in a Reach-to-Grasp Task. Front Neurosci 2020; 14:560. [PMID: 32612498 PMCID: PMC7308559 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00560] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2019] [Accepted: 05/06/2020] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent evidence suggests that target-relevant sensory stimuli (i.e., visual, auditory, and olfactory) can play important roles in the motor system. However, little is known about the effects of olfactory information on reaching and grasping movements. To determine whether odor stimuli affect hand movements, the reaching and grasping kinematic characteristics of 29 human participants were recorded using a three-dimensional video motion capture system. Participants received an odor stimulus by Sniffin’ Sticks and then reached toward and grasped a target. Grasping targets were apple, orange, ginger, and garlic. The odor stimulus was congruent with the target. The size of the odor-cued object (OCO) was the same size, smaller, or larger than a target to be grasped; or participants received odorless air while they viewed that target. They reached the target with one of two grips: a precision grip for a small target or a power grip for a larger target. The visual feedback was lost in half of 80 total trials after a start signal. It was no longer visible when participants reached the target. The results of repeated-measures analyses of variance followed by simple-effects analyses showed that when the size of the hand movement evoked by the odor cue was congruent with the size of the target, either both small or both large, the reaction time was significantly shorter than it was for odorless air. When participants received visual feedback throughout the trial, movement duration was significantly shorter if the odor cue was congruent with the size of the target or if odorless air was dispensed. When the size of hand movement evoked by the odor cue was incongruent with the size of the target, an interference effect was apparent on the maximum aperture time. The result of odorless air control group in a closed loop was shorter than incongruent odor group. In addition, visual feedback influenced the results such that the maximum aperture time occurred later when visibility was blocked only in the odorless air control condition. These results suggest that olfactory information has a positive effect on reach-to-grasp hand movements and that vision and olfaction may interact to optimize motor behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yang Yang
- School of Psychology, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
| | - Xiaochun Wang
- School of Psychology, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Revol P, Collette S, Boulot Z, Foncelle A, Niki C, Thura D, Imai A, Jacquin-Courtois S, Cabanac M, Osiurak F, Rossetti Y. Thirst for Intention? Grasping a Glass Is a Thirst-Controlled Action. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1248. [PMID: 31214073 PMCID: PMC6558183 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01248] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2018] [Accepted: 05/13/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Every day and every hour, we feel we perform numerous voluntary actions, i.e., actions under the control of our will. Individual’s ability to initiate goal-directed movement is classically described as a hierarchical motor organization, from an intentional module, mostly considered as a black box, to muscular activity supporting action execution. The general focus is usually set on the triggering of action by intention, which is assumed to be the only entry to the action cascade, rather than on the preceding formation of intentions. If intentions play a key role in the specification of movement kinematic parameters, it remains largely unknown whether unconscious cognitive processes may also affect action preparation and unfolding. Recently, a seemingly irrelevant variable, thirst, was shown to modulate a simple arbitrary action such as key-pressing. Thirsty individuals were shown to produce stronger motor inhibition in no-go trials when a glass of water was present. In the present experiment, we intended to explore whether motor inhibition operates not only upstream from the action cascade but may also affect the unfolding of reaching movements, i.e., at a lower-level control. Thirsty vs. non-thirsty control subjects were asked to reach and grasp green (go trial) or red glasses (no-go trial) filled with either water or transparent gel wax with a central candlewick. Thirsty subjects were faster to initiate actions toward the water glasses. They also exhibited an earlier maximal grip aperture and a global reduction of movement time which was mostly explained by a shortening of deceleration time. The deceleration phase was correlated with individual’s thirst rating. In addition, no-go trial toward a glass of water tended to inhibit the next movement toward a glass filled with gel wax. Thus, our results show that an unintentional influence of an internal state can reorganize voluntary action structure not only at the decision-making level but also at the level of motor control. Although subjects explicitly paid more attention and were more cautious to glasses filled with water, they reported no explicit sensation of an increased urge to grasp it, further suggesting that these effects are controlled by covert mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrice Revol
- Plate-forme "Mouvement et Handicap," Hôpital Henry-Gabrielle, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Saint-Genis-Laval, France.,Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, ImpAct, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Bron, France
| | - Sarah Collette
- Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, ImpAct, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Bron, France
| | - Zoe Boulot
- Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, ImpAct, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Bron, France
| | - Alexandre Foncelle
- Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, ImpAct, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Bron, France
| | - Chiharu Niki
- Tokyo Women's Medical University, Shinjuku, Japan
| | - David Thura
- Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, ImpAct, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Bron, France
| | - Akila Imai
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Shinshu University, Nagano, Japan
| | - Sophie Jacquin-Courtois
- Plate-forme "Mouvement et Handicap," Hôpital Henry-Gabrielle, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Saint-Genis-Laval, France.,Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, ImpAct, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Bron, France
| | - Michel Cabanac
- Faculté de Médecine, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
| | - François Osiurak
- Laboratoire d'Etude des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université de Lyon, Bron, France.,Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France
| | - Yves Rossetti
- Plate-forme "Mouvement et Handicap," Hôpital Henry-Gabrielle, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Saint-Genis-Laval, France.,Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, ImpAct, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Bron, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Fukui T, Inui T. Use of early phase online vision for grip configuration is modulated according to movement duration in prehension. Exp Brain Res 2015; 233:2257-68. [PMID: 25929554 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4295-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2014] [Accepted: 04/18/2015] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Our previous study (Hum Mov Sci 25:349-371, 2006) investigated whether and how online vision in the early phase of movement influences the control of reach-to-grasp movements (movement duration: approximately 1000 ms). We used liquid-crystal shutter goggles to manipulate the duration of available online vision during the movement and specified that online vision during the early phase influences grasping movements. The current study examined the effect of online early phase vision on the grip configuration according to the movement duration and compared it between two different movement durations (approximately 500 and 1000 ms). We found that non-availability of early phase online vision affected the grip configuration (i.e., inducing a larger peak grip aperture) even in the shorter movement duration. The influential period for online vision for grasping control shifts to an earlier time when movement time is shorter (i.e., from approximately 214 to 106 ms after movement onset), indicating a flexible mechanism for grip configuration according to the movement duration and the available online vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Takao Fukui
- Department of Intelligence Science and Technology, Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Yoshida-Honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto, 606-8501, Japan,
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Bieńkiewicz M, Young W, Craig C. Balls to the wall: How acoustic information from a ball in motion guides interceptive movement in people with Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience 2014; 275:508-18. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.050] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2014] [Revised: 06/11/2014] [Accepted: 06/18/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
9
|
Abstract
In this review, we describe the current models of dorsal and ventral streams in vision, audition and touch. Available theories take their first steps from the model of Milner and Goodale, which was developed to explain how human actions can be efficiently carried out using visual information. Since then, similar concepts have also been applied to other sensory modalities. We propose that advances in the knowledge of brain functioning can be achieved through models explaining action and perception patterns independently from sensory modalities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Sedda
- Department of Humanistic Studies- Psychology Section, University of Pavia, Pavia 27100, Italy.
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Croy I, Angelo SD, Olausson H. Reduced pleasant touch appraisal in the presence of a disgusting odor. PLoS One 2014; 9:e92975. [PMID: 24664177 PMCID: PMC3963971 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092975] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2013] [Accepted: 02/27/2014] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Odors are powerful emotional stimuli influencing mood, attention and behavior. Here we examined if odors change the perception of pleasant touch. In line with the warning function of the olfactory system, we proposed that especially unpleasant odors will reduce touch pleasantness, presumably through a disgust-related mechanism. METHODS Forty-five healthy participants (mean age 23.3 +/- 3years SD, 24 females) were presented to slow (3 cm/s) and fast (30 cm/s) brush stroking delivered by a robot to the forearm. Touch pleasantness under the influence of an unpleasant odor (Civette, smelling like feces) and an intensity matched pleasant odor (Rose) was compared to an odorless control condition. In a pilot study with 30 participants (mean age 25.9 +/-6 years, 21 females), the odors were matched according to their intensity, and we studied the influence of disgust sensitivity on the perception of 4 different odor qualities. RESULTS The unpleasant odor decreased touch pleasantness for both stroking velocities compared to the odorless control (p<0.005) whereas the rose odor did not change touch pleasantness significantly. Disgust sensitivity was correlated with the modulation of touch pleasantness. The pilot study revealed a significant correlation between disgust sensitivity and the perception of the unpleasant odor qualities (r = -0.56; p = 0.007), but not with any of the other odors. CONCLUSION Unpleasant odors are powerful in modulating touch pleasantness, and disgust might be a moderating variable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ilona Croy
- Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
- * E-mail:
| | - Silvia D' Angelo
- Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Håkan Olausson
- Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Parma V, Castiello U, Köster EP, Mojet J. Selecting food. The contribution of memory, liking, and action. Appetite 2014; 76:186-96. [PMID: 24560690 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2013] [Revised: 02/01/2014] [Accepted: 02/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The goal of the present experiment was twofold: identifying similarities and differences between flavour memory and visual memory mechanisms and investigating whether kinematics could serve as an implicit measure for food selection. To test flavour and visual memory an 'implicit' paradigm to represent real-life situations in a controlled lab setting was implemented. A target, i.e., a piece of cake shaped like either an orange or a tangerine, covered with either orange- or a tangerine-flavoured icing, was provided to participants on Day 1. On Day 2, without prior notice, participants were requested to recognize the target amongst a set of distractors, characterized by various flavours (orange vs. tangerine) and/or sizes (orange-like vs. tangerine-like). Similarly, targets and distractors consisting of 2D figures varying in shape and size were used to assess visual memory. Reach-to-grasp kinematics towards the targets were recorded and analysed by means of digitalization techniques. Correlations between kinematic parameters, memory and liking for each food item were also calculated. Results concerned with memory recollection indices provided evidence of different key mechanisms which could be based either on novelty of flavour memory or visual memory, respectively. To a moderate extent, kinematics may serve as an implicit index of food selection processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valentina Parma
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.
| | - Umberto Castiello
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Egon Peter Köster
- Psychology Department, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Jos Mojet
- Wageningen-UR, Food and Bio-based Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Visuo-olfactory integration during action observation and execution of reach-to-grasp movements. Neuroreport 2013; 24:768-72. [PMID: 23839303 DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0b013e328363d3ec] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Observing the actions of others prompts the motor system to perform a similar action. However, visual cues are not the only source of sensory information for the motor system, which is affected by stimuli presented in all modalities even when they are irrelevant for action completion. The current experiment explored whether (and how) olfactory stimuli can influence the performance of a reach-to-grasp movement to visual objects differing in size (small and large) in the context of an automatic imitation task. Odours could match-or not- the size of a to-be-grasped visual target, or be nonexistent. Movement duration, an integral index of motor control, was significantly shorter when participants previously observed the same action. Addition of the odour component suggested that when the odour matched the size of a small target, a facilitation effect was found. Results are discussed in terms of olfactory-visual integration mechanisms and how they relate to embodied cognition.
Collapse
|
13
|
Implicit olfactory processing attenuates motor disturbances in idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Cortex 2013; 49:1241-51. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2012] [Revised: 04/05/2012] [Accepted: 05/17/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
14
|
Parma V, Straulino E, Zanatto D, Cantagallo A, Tirindelli R, Castiello U. Implicit olfactory abilities in traumatic brain injured patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012; 34:977-88. [PMID: 22905854 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2012.711811] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
To investigate implicit olfactory abilities in a group of anosmic traumatic brain injured (TBI) patients, an olfactomotor priming paradigm was administered. A group of matched normosmic/mildly microsmic TBI patients and a group of neurologically healthy participants served as controls. For all the groups, an interference effect was evident on the peak velocity of grip aperture when participants grasped a large target preceded by a "small" odor. The present results suggest that some form of implicit olfactory processing is preserved in TBI patients even when diagnosed as anosmic on the basis of explicit olfactory testing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valentina Parma
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
When flavor guides motor control: an effector independence study. Exp Brain Res 2011; 212:339-46. [PMID: 21618038 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2733-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2011] [Accepted: 05/12/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Research on multisensory integration during natural tasks has revealed how chemical senses contribute to plan and control movements. An aspect which has yet to be investigated regards whether the motor representations evoked by chemosensory stimuli, once established for a particular movement, can be used to control different effectors. Here, we investigate this issue by asking participants to drink a sip of flavored solution, grasp with the hand a visual target, and then bring it to the mouth, miming the action of biting. Results show that hand and lip apertures were scaled according to the size of the object evoked by the flavor. Maximum hand and lip apertures were greater when the action toward a small visual target (e.g., strawberry) was preceded by a sip of a "large" (e.g., orange) than a "small" (e.g., almond) flavor solution. Conversely, maximum hand and lip apertures were smaller when the action toward a large visual target (e.g., apple) was preceded by the presentation of a "small" (e.g., strawberry) rather than a "large" flavor solution. These findings support previous evidence on the presence of a unique motor plan underlying the act of grasping with-the-hand and with-the-mouth, extending the knowledge of chemosensorimotor transformations to motor equivalence.
Collapse
|
16
|
Sedda A, Monaco S, Bottini G, Goodale MA. Integration of visual and auditory information for hand actions: preliminary evidence for the contribution of natural sounds to grasping. Exp Brain Res 2011; 209:365-74. [PMID: 21290243 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2559-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2010] [Accepted: 01/13/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
When we reach out to grasp objects, vision plays a major role in the control of our movements. Nevertheless, other sensory modalities contribute to the fine-tuning of our actions. Even olfaction has been shown to play a role in the scaling of movements directed at objects. Much less is known about how auditory information might be used to program grasping movements. The aim of our study was to investigate how the sound of a target object affects the planning of grasping movements in normal right-handed subjects. We performed an experiment in which auditory information could be used to infer size of targets when the availability of visual information was varied from trial to trial. Classical kinematic parameters (such as grip aperture) were measured to evaluate the influence of auditory information. In addition, an optimal inference modeling was applied to the data. The scaling of grip aperture indicated that the introduction of sound allowed subjects to infer the size of the object when vision was not available. Moreover, auditory information affected grip aperture even when vision was available. Our findings suggest that the differences in the natural impact sounds of objects of different sizes being placed on a surface can be used to plan grasping movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Sedda
- Department of Psychology, University of Pavia, Piazza Botta 6, 27100 Pavia, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Sounds and scents in (social) action. Trends Cogn Sci 2011; 15:47-55. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2010] [Revised: 12/07/2010] [Accepted: 12/07/2010] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
18
|
Parma V, Ghirardello D, Tirindelli R, Castiello U. Grasping a fruit. Hands do what flavour says. Appetite 2010; 56:249-54. [PMID: 21182884 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2010] [Revised: 10/14/2010] [Accepted: 12/13/2010] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Previous research on multisensory integration during goal-directed natural actions reported that visual, proprioceptive, auditory and orthonasal olfactory stimulation has the ability to influence motor control. In this study, we used kinematics to investigate the integration between vision and flavour perception during reach-to-grasp movements. Participants were requested to drink a sip of flavoured solution and then grasp an object presented in central vision. The results indicate that when the objects evoked by the flavour and by the visual target were of a similar size (i.e., large or small) and evoked the same kind of hand shaping in order to be grasped (i.e., congruent condition) facilitation effects emerged. Conversely, when the object evoked by the flavour and by the visual target was of a different size and evoked a different kind of hand shaping in order to be grasped (i.e., incongruent condition) interference effects emerged. Interference effects, however, were only evident for the combination involving a large visual target and a 'small' flavour. When comparing hand kinematics between the congruent and a 'no flavour' condition (i.e., water), facilitation effects emerged in favour of the former condition. Taken together, these results indicate the contribution of complex chemosensory stimuli for the planning and execution of visually guided reach to grasp movements. And, contribute to the current debate regarding the multisensory nature of the sensorimotor transformations underlying motor performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valentina Parma
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Via Venezia, 8, 35100 Padova, Italy
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Oess NP, Wanek J, van Hedel HJA. Enhancement of bend sensor properties as applied in a glove for use in neurorehabilitation settings. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE OF THE IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE 2010; 2010:5903-6. [PMID: 21096935 DOI: 10.1109/iembs.2010.5627534] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Following hand function impairment caused by a neurological disorder, the functional level of the upper extremities has to be assessed in the clinical and rehabilitation settings. Current hand function evaluation tests are somewhat imprecise. Instrumented gloves allow finger motion monitoring during the performance of skilled tasks, such as grasping objects. As a result, they provide an objective tool for evaluating slight changes in the fine motor skills of the hand. Numerous gloves are based on resistive bend sensors, given that this is an easy to handle, low-cost, and reliable sensing element. When bending is not applied homogeneously along such a sensor, as is the case with finger-joint bending, its output response varies with the sensor's longitudinal position. Our goal is to determine the optimal sensor position with respect to the finger-joint in order to enhance the resolution of the sensors embedded in a glove. The validity of the integrated sensors is evaluated and the accuracy values are given.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ninja P Oess
- SCI Research, Spinal Cord Injury Center, University of Zurich, Balgrist University Hospital, 8008, Switzerland.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Castiello U, Giordano BL, Begliomini C, Ansuini C, Grassi M. When ears drive hands: the influence of contact sound on reaching to grasp. PLoS One 2010; 5:e12240. [PMID: 20808929 PMCID: PMC2923194 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012240] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2010] [Accepted: 07/23/2010] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Most research on the roles of auditory information and its interaction with vision has focused on perceptual performance. Little is known on the effects of sound cues on visually-guided hand movements. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS We recorded the sound produced by the fingers upon contact as participants grasped stimulus objects which were covered with different materials. Then, in a further session the pre-recorded contact sounds were delivered to participants via headphones before or following the initiation of reach-to-grasp movements towards the stimulus objects. Reach-to-grasp movement kinematics were measured under the following conditions: (i) congruent, in which the presented contact sound and the contact sound elicited by the to-be-grasped stimulus corresponded; (ii) incongruent, in which the presented contact sound was different to that generated by the stimulus upon contact; (iii) control, in which a synthetic sound, not associated with a real event, was presented. Facilitation effects were found for congruent trials; interference effects were found for incongruent trials. In a second experiment, the upper and the lower parts of the stimulus were covered with different materials. The presented sound was always congruent with the material covering either the upper or the lower half of the stimulus. Participants consistently placed their fingers on the half of the stimulus that corresponded to the presented contact sound. CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE Altogether these findings offer a substantial contribution to the current debate about the type of object representations elicited by auditory stimuli and on the multisensory nature of the sensorimotor transformations underlying action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Umberto Castiello
- Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Sela L, Sobel N. Human olfaction: a constant state of change-blindness. Exp Brain Res 2010; 205:13-29. [PMID: 20603708 PMCID: PMC2908748 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-010-2348-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 114] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2010] [Accepted: 06/21/2010] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Paradoxically, although humans have a superb sense of smell, they don’t trust their nose. Furthermore, although human odorant detection thresholds are very low, only unusually high odorant concentrations spontaneously shift our attention to olfaction. Here we suggest that this lack of olfactory awareness reflects the nature of olfactory attention that is shaped by the spatial and temporal envelopes of olfaction. Regarding the spatial envelope, selective attention is allocated in space. Humans direct an attentional spotlight within spatial coordinates in both vision and audition. Human olfactory spatial abilities are minimal. Thus, with no olfactory space, there is no arena for olfactory selective attention. Regarding the temporal envelope, whereas vision and audition consist of nearly continuous input, olfactory input is discreet, made of sniffs widely separated in time. If similar temporal breaks are artificially introduced to vision and audition, they induce “change blindness”, a loss of attentional capture that results in a lack of awareness to change. Whereas “change blindness” is an aberration of vision and audition, the long inter-sniff-interval renders “change anosmia” the norm in human olfaction. Therefore, attentional capture in olfaction is minimal, as is human olfactory awareness. All this, however, does not diminish the role of olfaction through sub-attentive mechanisms allowing subliminal smells a profound influence on human behavior and perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lee Sela
- Department of Neurobiology, The Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, 76100 Israel
| | - Noam Sobel
- Department of Neurobiology, The Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, 76100 Israel
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Tubaldi F, Turella L, Pierno AC, Grodd W, Tirindelli R, Castiello U. Smelling odors, understanding actions. Soc Neurosci 2010; 6:31-47. [PMID: 20379900 DOI: 10.1080/17470911003691089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Previous evidence indicates that we understand others' actions not only by perceiving their visual features but also by their sound. This raises the possibility that brain regions responsible for action understanding respond to cues coming from different sensory modalities. Yet no studies, to date, have examined if this extends to olfaction. Here we addressed this issue by using functional magnetic resonance imaging. We searched for brain activity related to the observation of an action executed towards an object that was smelled rather than seen. The results show that temporal, parietal, and frontal areas were activated when individuals observed a hand grasping a smelled object. This activity differed from that evoked during the observation of a mimed grasp. Furthermore, superadditive activity was revealed when the action target-object was both seen and smelled. Together these findings indicate the influence of olfaction on action understanding and its contribution to multimodal action representations.
Collapse
|
23
|
Seo HS, Roidl E, Müller F, Negoias S. Odors enhance visual attention to congruent objects. Appetite 2010; 54:544-9. [PMID: 20176065 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.02.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2009] [Revised: 02/08/2010] [Accepted: 02/15/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Although it is well known that visual stimuli affect olfactory performance, little is known about the reverse case: the influence of odor on visual performance. This study aimed to determine whether odors can enhance attention towards visually presented objects congruent with the odors. Sixty healthy participants were presented with four odors (orange, lavender, coffee, and liquorice) before and during the presentation of photographic slides containing one congruent and three incongruent objects with the presented odors. The participants' visual attention was assessed as the total number and time of eye fixations by using an eye tracking system. When the participants smelled an odor, they looked more frequently and longer at a corresponding object as compared to the odorless condition. In conclusion, our findings demonstrate for the first time an olfactory priming effect on visual selective attention: odor can increase attention towards a congruent visual object as compared to a non-odor condition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Han-Seok Seo
- Smell & Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of Dresden Medical School, Fetscherstrasse 74, 01307 Dresden, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
The Effects of Task-Irrelevant Olfactory Information on the Planning and the Execution of Reach-to-Grasp Movements. CHEMOSENS PERCEPT 2009. [DOI: 10.1007/s12078-009-9039-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|