1
|
Terao Y, Tokushige SI, Inomata-Terada S, Miyazaki T, Kotsuki N, Fisicaro F, Ugawa Y. How do patients with Parkinson's disease and cerebellar ataxia read aloud? -Eye-voice coordination in text reading. Front Neurosci 2023; 17:1202404. [PMID: 37638315 PMCID: PMC10452879 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1202404] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2023] [Accepted: 07/18/2023] [Indexed: 08/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Background The coordination between gaze and voice is closely linked when reading text aloud, with the gaze leading the reading position by a certain eye-voice lead (EVL). How this coordination is affected is unknown in patients with cerebellar ataxia and parkinsonism, who show oculomotor deficits possibly impacting coordination between different effectors. Objective To elucidate the role of the cerebellum and basal ganglia in eye-voice coordination during reading aloud, by studying patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) and spinocerebellar degeneration (SCD). Methods Participants were sixteen SCD patients, 18 PD patients, and 30 age-matched normal subjects, all native Japanese speakers without cognitive impairment. Subjects read aloud Japanese texts of varying readability displayed on a monitor in front of their eyes, consisting of Chinese characters and hiragana (Japanese phonograms). The gaze and voice reading the text was simultaneously recorded by video-oculography and a microphone. A custom program synchronized and aligned the gaze and audio data in time. Results Reading speed was significantly reduced in SCD patients (3.53 ± 1.81 letters/s), requiring frequent regressions to compensate for the slow reading speed. In contrast, PD patients read at a comparable speed to normal subjects (4.79 ± 3.13 letters/s vs. 4.71 ± 2.38 letters/s). The gaze scanning speed, excluding regressive saccades, was slower in PD patients (9.64 ± 4.26 letters/s) compared to both normal subjects (12.55 ± 5.42 letters/s) and SCD patients (10.81 ± 4.52 letters/s). PD patients' gaze could not far exceed that of the reading speed, with smaller allowance for the gaze to proceed ahead of the reading position. Spatial EVL was similar across the three groups for all texts (normal: 2.95 ± 1.17 letters/s, PD: 2.95 ± 1.51 letters/s, SCD: 3.21 ± 1.35 letters/s). The ratio of gaze duration to temporal EVL was lowest for SCD patients (normal: 0.73 ± 0.50, PD: 0.70 ± 0.37, SCD: 0.40 ± 0.15). Conclusion Although coordination between voice and eye movements and normal eye-voice span was observed in both PD and SCD, SCD patients made frequent regressions to manage the slowed vocal output, restricting the ability for advance processing of text ahead of the gaze. In contrast, PD patients experience restricted reading speed primarily due to slowed scanning, limiting their maximum reading speed but effectively utilizing advance processing of upcoming text.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yasuo Terao
- Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- Department of Medical Physiology, Kyorin University, Mitaka, Japan
| | - Shin-ichi Tokushige
- Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- Department of Neurology, Kyorin University, Mitaka, Japan
| | - Satomi Inomata-Terada
- Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- Department of Medical Physiology, Kyorin University, Mitaka, Japan
| | - Tai Miyazaki
- Department of Neurology, Kyorin University, Mitaka, Japan
| | - Naoki Kotsuki
- Department of Neurology, Kyorin University, Mitaka, Japan
| | - Francesco Fisicaro
- Department of Human Neurophysiology, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan
| | - Yoshikazu Ugawa
- Department of Human Neurophysiology, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Taha J, Carioti D, Stucchi N, Chailleux M, Granocchio E, Sarti D, De Salvatore M, Guasti MT. Identifying the risk of dyslexia in bilingual children: The potential of language-dependent and language-independent tasks. Front Psychol 2022; 13:935935. [PMID: 36506974 PMCID: PMC9730291 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.935935] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2022] [Accepted: 10/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
This study investigates the linguistic processing and non-linguistic cognitive abilities of monolingual and bilingual children with and without reading difficulties and examines the relationship between these skills and reading. There were 72 Italian-speaking children: 18 monolingual good readers (MONO-GR, Mage = 10;4), 19 monolingual poor readers (MONO-PR, Mage = 10;3), 21 bilingual good readers (BI-GR, Mage = 10;6), and 16 bilingual poor readers (BI-PR, Mage = 10;6). All bilingual children spoke Italian as their L2. Children completed a battery of standardized Italian reading tests, language-dependent tasks: nonword repetition (NWR), sentence repetition (SR), and phonological awareness (PA), and language-independent tasks: timing anticipation, beat synchronization, inhibition control, auditory reaction time, and rapid automatized naming (RAN). Poor readers scored below good readers on the language-dependent tasks, including NWR, PA, and SR. Beat synchronization was the only language-independent task sensitive to reading ability, with poor readers showing greater variability than good readers in tapping to fast rhythms. SR was the only task influenced by language experience as bilinguals underperformed monolinguals on the task. Moreover, there were weak to moderate correlations between performance on some language-dependent tasks (NWR, PA), language-independent tasks (inhibition control, RAN), and reading measures. Performance on the experimental tasks (except for RAN) was not associated with the length of exposure to Italian. The results highlight the potential of NWR, PA, SR, and beat synchronization tasks in identifying the risk of dyslexia in bilingual populations. Future research is needed to validate these findings and to establish the tasks' diagnostic accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juhayna Taha
- Department of Psychology, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Desire Carioti
- Department of Psychology, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Natale Stucchi
- Department of Psychology, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Mathilde Chailleux
- Department of Psychology, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Elisa Granocchio
- Developmental Neurology Unit, Foundation IRCCS Neurological Institute Carlo Besta, Milan, Italy
| | - Daniela Sarti
- Developmental Neurology Unit, Foundation IRCCS Neurological Institute Carlo Besta, Milan, Italy
| | - Marinella De Salvatore
- Developmental Neurology Unit, Foundation IRCCS Neurological Institute Carlo Besta, Milan, Italy
| | - Maria Teresa Guasti
- Department of Psychology, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Adedeji VI, Vasilev MR, Kirkby JA, Slattery TJ. Return-sweep saccades in oral reading. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2021; 86:1804-1815. [PMID: 34694488 PMCID: PMC9363329 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-021-01610-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2021] [Accepted: 10/06/2021] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Recent research on return-sweep saccades has improved our understanding of eye movements when reading paragraphs. However, these saccades, which take our gaze from the end of one line to the start of the next line, have been studied only within the context of silent reading. Articulatory demands and the coordination of the eye-voice span (EVS) at line boundaries suggest that the execution of this saccade may be different in oral reading. We compared launch and landing positions of return-sweeps, corrective saccade probability and fixations adjacent to return-sweeps in skilled adult readers while reading paragraphs aloud and silently. Compared to silent reading, return-sweeps were launched from closer to the end of the line and landed closer to the start of the next line when reading aloud. The probability of making a corrective saccade was higher for oral reading than silent reading. These indicate that oral reading may compel readers to rely more on foveal processing at the expense of parafoveal processing. We found an interaction between reading modality and fixation type on fixation durations. The reading modality effect (i.e., increased fixation durations in oral compared to silent reading) was greater for accurate line-initial fixations and marginally greater for line-final fixations compared to intra-line fixations. This suggests that readers may use the fixations adjacent to return-sweeps as natural pause locations to modulate the EVS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria I Adedeji
- Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole House, Talbot Campus, Fern Barrow Poole, Dorset, BH12 5BB, UK.
| | - Martin R Vasilev
- Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole House, Talbot Campus, Fern Barrow Poole, Dorset, BH12 5BB, UK
| | - Julie A Kirkby
- Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole House, Talbot Campus, Fern Barrow Poole, Dorset, BH12 5BB, UK
| | - Timothy J Slattery
- Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole House, Talbot Campus, Fern Barrow Poole, Dorset, BH12 5BB, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Smyrnakis I, Andreadakis V, Rina A, Bοufachrentin N, Aslanides IM. Silent versus Reading Out Loud modes: An eye-tracking study. J Eye Mov Res 2021; 14:10.16910/jemr.14.2.1. [PMID: 34745441 PMCID: PMC8565638 DOI: 10.16910/jemr.14.2.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The main purpose of this study is to compare the silent and loud reading ability of typical and dyslexic readers, using eye-tracking technology to monitor the reading process. The participants (156 students of normal intelligence) were first divided into three groups based on their school grade, and each subgroup was then further separated into typical readers and students diagnosed with dyslexia. The students read the same text twice, one time silently and one time out loud. Various eye-tracking parameters were calculated for both types of reading. In general, the performance of the typical students was better for both modes of reading - regardless of age. In the older age groups, typical readers performed better at silent reading. The dyslexic readers in all age groups performed better at reading out loud. However, this was less prominent in secondary and upper secondary dyslexics, reflecting a slow shift towards silent reading mode as they age. Our results confirm that the eye-tracking parameters of dyslexics improve with age in both silent and loud reading, and their reading preference shifts slowly towards silent reading. Typical readers, before 4th grade do not show a clear reading mode preference, however, after that age they develop a clear preference for silent reading.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Andriani Rina
- Harvard Medical School, USA
- Brigham and Women's Hospital, USA
- Jamaica Plain VA Hospital, USA
- University of Tübingen, Germany
- MGH Inst. of Health Professions, USA
| | | | - Ioannis M Aslanides
- Emmetropia Eye Institute, Greece
- Optotech Ltd., Greece
- Hellenic Mediterranean Univ., Greece
- Wenzhou Medical Univ., China
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Zoccolotti P, De Luca M, Marinelli CV, Spinelli D. Predicting individual differences in reading, spelling and maths in a sample of typically developing children: A study in the perspective of comorbidity. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0231937. [PMID: 32352985 PMCID: PMC7192483 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231937] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2019] [Accepted: 04/03/2020] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
We examined reading, spelling, and mathematical skills in an unselected group of 129 Italian fifth graders by testing various cognitive predictors for each behaviour. As dependent variables, we measured performance in behaviours with a clear functional value in everyday life, such as reading a text, spelling under dictation and doing mental and written computations. As predictors, we selected cognitive dimensions having an explicit relation with the target behaviour (called proximal predictors), and prepared various tests in order to select which task had the best predictive power on each behaviour. The aim was to develop a model of proximal predictors of reading (speed and accuracy), spelling (accuracy) and maths (speed and accuracy) characterized by efficacy also in comparison to the prediction based on general cognitive factors (i.e., short-term memory, phonemic verbal fluency, visual perceptual speed, and non-verbal intelligence) and parsimony, pinpointing the role of both common and unique predictors as envisaged in the general perspective of co-morbidity. With one exception (reading accuracy), the proximal predictors models (based on communality analyses) explained a sizeable amount of variance, ranging from 27.5% in the case of calculation (accuracy) to 48.7% of reading (fluency). Models based on general cognitive factors also accounted for some variance (ranging from 6.5% in the case of spelling to 19.5% in the case of reading fluency) but this was appreciably less than that explained by models based on the hypothesized proximal predictors. In general, results confirmed the efficacy of proximal models in predicting reading, spelling and maths although they offered only limited support for common predictors across different learning skills; namely, performance in the Orthographic Decision test entered as a predictor of both reading and spelling indicating that a single orthographic lexicon may account for performance in reading and spelling. Possible lines of research to expand on this approach are illustrated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pierluigi Zoccolotti
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
- Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
| | - Maria De Luca
- Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
| | - Chiara Valeria Marinelli
- Laboratory of Applied Psychology and Intervention, Department of History, Society and Human Studies, University of Salento, Lecce, Italy
| | - Donatella Spinelli
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Foro Italico University of Rome, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Krieber M, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Pokorny FB, Zhang D, Landerl K, Körner C, Pernkopf F, Pock T, Einspieler C, Marschik PB. Eye Movements during Silent and Oral Reading in a Regular Orthography: Basic Characteristics and Correlations with Childhood Cognitive Abilities and Adolescent Reading Skills. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0170986. [PMID: 28151950 PMCID: PMC5289712 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170986] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/10/2016] [Accepted: 01/14/2017] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study aimed to define differences between silent and oral reading with respect to spatial and temporal eye movement parameters. Eye movements of 22 German-speaking adolescents (14 females; mean age = 13;6 years;months) were recorded while reading an age-appropriate text silently and orally. Preschool cognitive abilities were assessed at the participants’ age of 5;7 (years;months) using the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. The participants’ reading speed and reading comprehension at the age of 13;6 (years;months) were determined using a standardized inventory to evaluate silent reading skills in German readers (Lesegeschwindigkeits- und -verständnistest für Klassen 6–12). The results show that (i) reading mode significantly influenced both spatial and temporal characteristics of eye movement patterns; (ii) articulation decreased the consistency of intraindividual reading performances with regard to a significant number of eye movement parameters; (iii) reading skills predicted the majority of eye movement parameters during silent reading, but influenced only a restricted number of eye movement parameters when reading orally; (iv) differences with respect to a subset of eye movement parameters increased with reading skills; (v) an overall preschool cognitive performance score predicted reading skills at the age of 13;6 (years;months), but not eye movement patterns during either silent or oral reading. However, we found a few significant correlations between preschool performances on subscales of sequential and simultaneous processing and eye movement parameters for both reading modes. Overall, the findings suggest that eye movement patterns depend on the reading mode. Preschool cognitive abilities were more closely related to eye movement patterns of oral than silent reading, while reading skills predicted eye movement patterns during silent reading, but less so during oral reading.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magdalena Krieber
- Research Unit iDN–Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience, Institute of Physiology, Center for Physiological Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Katrin D. Bartl-Pokorny
- Research Unit iDN–Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience, Institute of Physiology, Center for Physiological Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Florian B. Pokorny
- Research Unit iDN–Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience, Institute of Physiology, Center for Physiological Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
- BEE-PRI: Brain, Ears & Eyes–Pattern Recognition Initiative, BioTechMed-Graz, Graz, Austria
- Machine Intelligence & Signal Processing Group, Technische Universität München, Munich, Germany
| | - Dajie Zhang
- Research Unit iDN–Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience, Institute of Physiology, Center for Physiological Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Karin Landerl
- Developmental Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Christof Körner
- Cognitive Psychology & Neuroscience, Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Franz Pernkopf
- BEE-PRI: Brain, Ears & Eyes–Pattern Recognition Initiative, BioTechMed-Graz, Graz, Austria
- Signal Processing and Speech Communication Laboratory, Graz University of Technology, Graz, Austria
| | - Thomas Pock
- Institute for Computer Graphics and Vision, Graz University of Technology, Graz, Austria
| | - Christa Einspieler
- Research Unit iDN–Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience, Institute of Physiology, Center for Physiological Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Peter B. Marschik
- Research Unit iDN–Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience, Institute of Physiology, Center for Physiological Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
- BEE-PRI: Brain, Ears & Eyes–Pattern Recognition Initiative, BioTechMed-Graz, Graz, Austria
- Center of Neurodevelopmental Disorders (KIND), Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Silva S, Reis A, Casaca L, Petersson KM, Faísca L. When the Eyes No Longer Lead: Familiarity and Length Effects on Eye-Voice Span. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1720. [PMID: 27853446 PMCID: PMC5089997 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01720] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2016] [Accepted: 10/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
During oral reading, the eyes tend to be ahead of the voice (eye-voice span, EVS). It has been hypothesized that the extent to which this happens depends on the automaticity of reading processes, namely on the speed of print-to-sound conversion. We tested whether EVS is affected by another automaticity component – immunity from interference. To that end, we manipulated word familiarity (high-frequency, low-frequency, and pseudowords, PW) and word length as proxies of immunity from interference, and we used linear mixed effects models to measure the effects of both variables on the time interval at which readers do parallel processing by gazing at word N + 1 while not having articulated word N yet (offset EVS). Parallel processing was enhanced by automaticity, as shown by familiarity × length interactions on offset EVS, and it was impeded by lack of automaticity, as shown by the transformation of offset EVS into voice-eye span (voice ahead of the offset of the eyes) in PWs. The relation between parallel processing and automaticity was strengthened by the fact that offset EVS predicted reading velocity. Our findings contribute to understand how the offset EVS, an index that is obtained in oral reading, may tap into different components of automaticity that underlie reading ability, oral or silent. In addition, we compared the duration of the offset EVS with the average reference duration of stages in word production, and we saw that the offset EVS may accommodate for more than the articulatory programming stage of word N.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susana Silva
- Center for Psychology at the University of Porto, University of PortoPorto, Portugal; Centre for Biomedical Research, University of AlgarveFaro, Portugal
| | - Alexandra Reis
- Centre for Biomedical Research, University of Algarve Faro, Portugal
| | - Luís Casaca
- Centre for Biomedical Research, University of Algarve Faro, Portugal
| | - Karl M Petersson
- Centre for Biomedical Research, University of AlgarveFaro, Portugal; Neurobiology of Language, Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegen, Netherlands; Language in interaction, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and BehaviourNijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Luís Faísca
- Centre for Biomedical Research, University of Algarve Faro, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Suárez-Coalla P, Álvarez-Cañizo M, Martínez C, García N, Cuetos F. Reading prosody in Spanish dyslexics. ANNALS OF DYSLEXIA 2016; 66:275-300. [PMID: 26939793 DOI: 10.1007/s11881-016-0123-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2015] [Accepted: 01/11/2016] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
Reading becomes expressive when word and text reading are quick, accurate and automatic. Recent studies have reported that skilled readers use greater pitch changes and fewer irrelevant pauses than poor readers. Given that developmental dyslexics have difficulty acquiring and automating the alphabetic code and developing orthographic representations of words, it is possible that their use of prosody when reading differs from that of typical readers. The goal of this study was to investigate whether the reading prosody of Spanish-speaking dyslexics differs from that of typical Spanish readers. Two experiments were performed. The first experiment involved 36 children (18 with dyslexia), and the second involved 46 adults (23 with dyslexia). Participants were asked to read aloud a text which included declarative, exclamatory and interrogative sentences. Data on pausing and reading rate (number of pauses, duration of pauses and utterances), pitch changes, intensity changes and syllable lengthening were extracted from the recordings. We found that dyslexic people read more slowly than typical readers and they also made more inappropriate and longer pauses, even as adults with considerable reading experience. We also observed that dyslexics differed from skilled readers in their use of some prosodic features, particularly pitch changes at the end of sentences. This is probably because they have trouble anticipating some structural features of prose, such as sentence ends.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paz Suárez-Coalla
- Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Pza Feijoo s/n, 33003, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain.
| | - Marta Álvarez-Cañizo
- Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Pza Feijoo s/n, 33003, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain
| | - Cristina Martínez
- Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Pza Feijoo s/n, 33003, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain
| | - Noemí García
- Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Pza Feijoo s/n, 33003, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain
| | - Fernando Cuetos
- Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Pza Feijoo s/n, 33003, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Gordon PC, Hoedemaker RS. Effective scheduling of looking and talking during rapid automatized naming. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2016; 42:742-60. [PMID: 26689309 PMCID: PMC4840059 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000171] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Rapid automatized naming (RAN) is strongly related to literacy gains in developing readers, reading disabilities, and reading ability in children and adults. Because successful RAN performance depends on the close coordination of a number of abilities, it is unclear what specific skills drive this RAN-reading relationship. The current study used concurrent recordings of young adult participants' vocalizations and eye movements during the RAN task to assess how individual variation in RAN performance depends on the coordination of visual and vocal processes. Results showed that fast RAN times are facilitated by having the eyes 1 or more items ahead of the current vocalization, as long as the eyes do not get so far ahead of the voice as to require a regressive eye movement to an earlier item. These data suggest that optimizing RAN performance is a problem of scheduling eye movements and vocalization given memory constraints and the efficiency of encoding and articulatory control. Both RAN completion time (conventionally used to indicate RAN performance) and eye-voice relations predicted some aspects of participants' eye movements on a separate sentence reading task. However, eye-voice relations predicted additional features of first-pass reading that were not predicted by RAN completion time. This shows that measurement of eye-voice patterns can identify important aspects of individual variation in reading that are not identified by the standard measure of RAN performance. We argue that RAN performance predicts reading ability because both tasks entail challenges of scheduling cognitive and linguistic processes that operate simultaneously on multiple linguistic inputs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter C Gordon
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Primativo S, Spinelli D, Zoccolotti P, De Luca M, Martelli M. Perceptual and Cognitive Factors Imposing "Speed Limits" on Reading Rate: A Study with the Rapid Serial Visual Presentation. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0153786. [PMID: 27088226 PMCID: PMC4835101 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153786] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2015] [Accepted: 04/04/2016] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Adults read at high speed, but estimates of their reading rate vary greatly, i.e., from 100 to 1500 words per minute (wpm). This discrepancy is likely due to different recording methods and to the different perceptual and cognitive processes involved in specific test conditions. The present study investigated the origins of these notable differences in RSVP reading rate (RR). In six experiments we investigated the role of many different perceptual and cognitive variables. The presence of a mask caused a steep decline in reading rate, with an estimated masking cost of about 200 wpm. When the decoding process was isolated, RR approached values of 1200 wpm. When the number of stimuli exceeded the short-term memory span, RR decreased to 800 wpm. The semantic context contributed to reading speed only by a factor of 1.4. Finally, eye movements imposed an upper limit on RR (around 300 wpm). Overall, data indicate a speed limit of 300 wpm, which corresponds to the time needed for eye movement execution, i.e., the most time consuming mechanism. Results reconcile differences in reading rates reported by different laboratories and thus provide suggestions for targeting different components of reading rate.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvia Primativo
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
- Dementia Research Centre, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
- * E-mail: (SP); (MM)
| | - Donatella Spinelli
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
- University of Rome «Foro Italico», Rome, Italy
| | - Pierluigi Zoccolotti
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
| | | | - Marialuisa Martelli
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
- * E-mail: (SP); (MM)
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Zoccolotti P, De Luca M, Spinelli D. Discrete versus multiple word displays: a re-analysis of studies comparing dyslexic and typically developing children. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1530. [PMID: 26500588 PMCID: PMC4595781 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01530] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2015] [Accepted: 09/22/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The study examines whether impairments in reading a text can be explained by a deficit in word decoding or an additional deficit in the processes governing the integration of reading subcomponents (including eye movement programming and pronunciation) should also be postulated. We report a re-analysis of data from eleven previous experiments conducted in our lab where the reading performance on single, discrete word displays as well multiple displays (texts, and in few cases also word lists) was investigated in groups of dyslexic children and typically developing readers. The analysis focuses on measures of time and not accuracy. Across experiments, dyslexic children are slower and more variable than typically developing readers in reading texts as well as vocal reaction time (RTs) to singly presented words; the dis-homogeneity in variability between groups points to the inappropriateness of standard measures of size effect (such as Cohen’s d), and suggests the use of the ratio between groups’ performance. The mean ratio for text reading is 1.95 across experiments. Mean ratio for vocal RTs for singly presented words is considerably smaller (1.52). Furthermore, this latter value is probably an overestimation as considering total reading times (i.e., a measure including also the pronunciation component) considerably reduces the group difference in vocal RTs (1.19 according to Martelli et al., 2014). The ratio difference between single and multiple displays does not depend upon the presence of a semantic context in the case of texts as large ratios are also observed with lists of unrelated words (though studies testing this aspect were few). We conclude that, if care is taken in using appropriate comparisons, the deficit in reading texts or lists of words is appreciably greater than that revealed with discrete word presentations. Thus, reading multiple stimuli present a specific, additional challenge to dyslexic children indicating that models of reading should incorporate this aspect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pierluigi Zoccolotti
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome Rome, Italy ; Neuropsychology Unit, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico - Fondazione Santa Lucia Rome, Italy
| | - Maria De Luca
- Neuropsychology Unit, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico - Fondazione Santa Lucia Rome, Italy
| | - Donatella Spinelli
- Neuropsychology Unit, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico - Fondazione Santa Lucia Rome, Italy ; Department of Human Movement Sciences and Health, University of Rome "Foro Italico" Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Laubrock J, Kliegl R. The eye-voice span during reading aloud. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1432. [PMID: 26441800 PMCID: PMC4585246 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01432] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2015] [Accepted: 09/08/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Although eye movements during reading are modulated by cognitive processing demands, they also reflect visual sampling of the input, and possibly preparation of output for speech or the inner voice. By simultaneously recording eye movements and the voice during reading aloud, we obtained an output measure that constrains the length of time spent on cognitive processing. Here we investigate the dynamics of the eye-voice span (EVS), the distance between eye and voice. We show that the EVS is regulated immediately during fixation of a word by either increasing fixation duration or programming a regressive eye movement against the reading direction. EVS size at the beginning of a fixation was positively correlated with the likelihood of regressions and refixations. Regression probability was further increased if the EVS was still large at the end of a fixation: if adjustment of fixation duration did not sufficiently reduce the EVS during a fixation, then a regression rather than a refixation followed with high probability. We further show that the EVS can help understand cognitive influences on fixation duration during reading: in mixed model analyses, the EVS was a stronger predictor of fixation durations than either word frequency or word length. The EVS modulated the influence of several other predictors on single fixation durations (SFDs). For example, word-N frequency effects were larger with a large EVS, especially when word N-1 frequency was low. Finally, a comparison of SFDs during oral and silent reading showed that reading is governed by similar principles in both reading modes, although EVS maintenance and articulatory processing also cause some differences. In summary, the EVS is regulated by adjusting fixation duration and/or by programming a regressive eye movement when the EVS gets too large. Overall, the EVS appears to be directly related to updating of the working memory buffer during reading.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jochen Laubrock
- Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam Germany
| | - Reinhold Kliegl
- Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam Germany
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Suárez-Coalla P, Cuetos F. Reading difficulties in Spanish adults with dyslexia. ANNALS OF DYSLEXIA 2015; 65:33-51. [PMID: 25836629 DOI: 10.1007/s11881-015-0101-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2014] [Accepted: 03/04/2015] [Indexed: 05/21/2023]
Abstract
Recent studies show that dyslexia persists into adulthood, even in highly educated and well-read people. The main characteristic that adults with dyslexia present is a low speed when reading. In Spanish, a shallow orthographic system, no studies about adults with dyslexia are available; and it is possible that the consistency of the orthographic system favours the reading fluency. The aim of this study was to get an insight of the reading characteristics of Spanish adults with dyslexia and also to infer the reading strategies that they are using. For that purpose, a group of 30 dyslexics (M age = 32 years old) and an age-matched group of 30 adults without reading disabilities completed several phonological and reading tasks: phonological awareness tasks, rapid automatic naming, lexical decision, word and pseudoword reading, letter detection and text reading. The results showed that highly educated Spanish dyslexics performed significantly worse than the control group in the majority of the tasks. Specifically, they showed difficulties reading long pseudowords, indicating problems in automating the grapheme-phoneme rules, but they also seem to present difficulties reading words, which indicate problems with the lexical route. It seems that the Spanish dyslexic adults, as in deep orthographies, continue having difficulties in phonological awareness tasks, rapid naming and reading.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paz Suárez-Coalla
- Department of Psychology, Universidad de Oviedo, Pza Feijoo s/n, 33003, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain,
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Zoccolotti P, De Luca M, Marinelli CV, Spinelli D. Modeling individual differences in text reading fluency: a different pattern of predictors for typically developing and dyslexic readers. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1374. [PMID: 25477856 PMCID: PMC4235379 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2014] [Accepted: 09/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
This study was aimed at predicting individual differences in text reading fluency. The basic proposal included two factors, i.e., the ability to decode letter strings (measured by discrete pseudo-word reading) and integration of the various sub-components involved in reading (measured by Rapid Automatized Naming, RAN). Subsequently, a third factor was added to the model, i.e., naming of discrete digits. In order to use homogeneous measures, all contributing variables considered the entire processing of the item, including pronunciation time. The model, which was based on commonality analysis, was applied to data from a group of 43 typically developing readers (11- to 13-year-olds) and a group of 25 chronologically matched dyslexic children. In typically developing readers, both orthographic decoding and integration of reading sub-components contributed significantly to the overall prediction of text reading fluency. The model prediction was higher (from ca. 37 to 52% of the explained variance) when we included the naming of discrete digits variable, which had a suppressive effect on pseudo-word reading. In the dyslexic readers, the variance explained by the two-factor model was high (69%) and did not change when the third factor was added. The lack of a suppression effect was likely due to the prominent individual differences in poor orthographic decoding of the dyslexic children. Analyses on data from both groups of children were replicated by using patches of colors as stimuli (both in the RAN task and in the discrete naming task) obtaining similar results. We conclude that it is possible to predict much of the variance in text-reading fluency using basic processes, such as orthographic decoding and integration of reading sub-components, even without taking into consideration higher-order linguistic factors such as lexical, semantic and contextual abilities. The approach validity of using proximal vs. distal causes to predict reading fluency is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pierluigi Zoccolotti
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome Rome, Italy ; Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia Rome, Italy
| | - Maria De Luca
- Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia Rome, Italy
| | | | - Donatella Spinelli
- Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia Rome, Italy ; Department of Human Movement Sciences and Health, University of Rome "Foro Italico" Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Goswami U, Power AJ, Lallier M, Facoetti A. Oscillatory "temporal sampling" and developmental dyslexia: toward an over-arching theoretical framework. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8:904. [PMID: 25426052 PMCID: PMC4224062 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2014] [Accepted: 10/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Usha Goswami
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Neuroscience in Education, University of Cambridge Cambridge, UK
| | - Alan J Power
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Neuroscience in Education, University of Cambridge Cambridge, UK
| | - Marie Lallier
- Basque Centre on Cognition, Brain and Language San Sebastian, Spain
| | | |
Collapse
|