1
|
Moura N, Fonseca P, Vilas-Boas JP, Serra S. Increased body movement equals better performance? Not always! Musical style determines motion degree perceived as optimal in music performance. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2024; 88:1314-1330. [PMID: 38329559 PMCID: PMC11142955 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01928-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2023] [Accepted: 01/18/2024] [Indexed: 02/09/2024]
Abstract
Musicians' body behaviour has a preponderant role in audience perception. We investigated how performers' motion is perceived depending on the musical style and musical expertise. To further explore the effect of visual input, stimuli were presented in audio-only, audio-visual and visual-only conditions. We used motion and audio recordings of expert saxophone players playing two contrasting excerpts (positively and negatively valenced). For each excerpt, stimuli represented five motion degrees with increasing quantity of motion (QoM) and distinct predominant gestures. In the experiment (online and in-person), 384 participants rated performance recordings for expressiveness, professionalism and overall quality. Results revealed that, for the positively valenced excerpt, ratings increased as a function of QoM, whilst for the negatively valenced, the recording with predominant flap motion was favoured. Musicianship did not have a significant effect in motion perception. Concerning multisensory integration, both musicians and non-musicians presented visual dominance in the positively valenced excerpt, whereas in the negatively valenced, musicians shifted to auditory dominance. Our findings demonstrate that musical style not only determines the way observers perceive musicians' movement as adequate, but also that it can promote changes in multisensory integration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nádia Moura
- Research Centre in Science and Technology of the Arts (CITAR), School of Arts, Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Porto, Portugal.
- Porto Biomechanics Laboratory (LABIOMEP), Faculty of Sport, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal.
| | - Pedro Fonseca
- Porto Biomechanics Laboratory (LABIOMEP), Faculty of Sport, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - João Paulo Vilas-Boas
- Porto Biomechanics Laboratory (LABIOMEP), Faculty of Sport, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- Centre of Research, Education, Innovation and Intervention in Sport (CIFI2D), Faculty of Sport, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - Sofia Serra
- Research Centre in Science and Technology of the Arts (CITAR), School of Arts, Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Porto, Portugal
- Instituto de Etnomusicologia-Centro de Estudos em Música e Dança (INET-MD), Departamento de Comunicação e Arte, Universidade de Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
O’Connell SR, Nave-Blodgett JE, Wilson GE, Hannon EE, Snyder JS. Elements of musical and dance sophistication predict musical groove perception. Front Psychol 2022; 13:998321. [PMID: 36467160 PMCID: PMC9712211 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998321] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2022] [Accepted: 10/21/2022] [Indexed: 11/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Listening to groovy music is an enjoyable experience and a common human behavior in some cultures. Specifically, many listeners agree that songs they find to be more familiar and pleasurable are more likely to induce the experience of musical groove. While the pleasurable and dance-inducing effects of musical groove are omnipresent, we know less about how subjective feelings toward music, individual musical or dance experiences, or more objective musical perception abilities are correlated with the way we experience groove. Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate how musical and dance sophistication relates to musical groove perception. One-hundred 24 participants completed an online study during which they rated 20 songs, considered high- or low-groove, and completed the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index, the Goldsmiths Dance Sophistication Index, the Beat and Meter Sensitivity Task, and a modified short version of the Profile for Music Perception Skills. Our results reveal that measures of perceptual abilities, musical training, and social dancing predicted the difference in groove rating between high- and low-groove music. Overall, these findings support the notion that listeners' individual experiences and predispositions may shape their perception of musical groove, although other causal directions are also possible. This research helps elucidate the correlates and possible causes of musical groove perception in a wide range of listeners.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samantha R. O’Connell
- Caruso Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Keck School of Medicine of USC, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
| | | | - Grace E. Wilson
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, United States
| | - Erin E. Hannon
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, United States
| | - Joel S. Snyder
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, United States
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Câmara GS, Nymoen K, Lartillot O, Danielsen A. Effects of instructed timing on electric guitar and bass sound in groove performance. THE JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA 2020; 147:1028. [PMID: 32113267 DOI: 10.1121/10.0000724] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2019] [Accepted: 01/24/2020] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
This paper reports on two experiments that investigated the expressive means through which musicians well versed in groove-based music signal the intended timing of a rhythmic event. Data were collected from 21 expert electric guitarists and 21 bassists, who were instructed to perform a simple rhythmic pattern in three different timing styles-"laid-back," "on-the-beat," and "pushed"-in tandem with a metronome. As expected, onset and peak timing locations corresponded to the instructed timing styles for both instruments. Regarding sound, results for guitarists revealed systematic differences across participants in the duration and brightness [spectral centroid (SC)] of the guitar strokes played using these different timing styles. In general, laid-back strokes were played with a longer duration and a lower SC relative to on-the-beat and pushed strokes. Results for the bassists indicated systematic differences in intensity (sound-pressure level): pushed strokes were played with higher intensity than on-the-beat and laid-back strokes. These results lend further credence to the hypothesis that both temporal and sound-related features are important indications of the intended timing of a rhythmic event, and together these features offer deeper insight into the ways in which musicians communicate at the microrhythmic level in groove-based music.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guilherme Schmidt Câmara
- RITMO Center for Interdisciplinary Studies in Time, Rhythm and Motion, Department of Musicology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Kristian Nymoen
- RITMO Center for Interdisciplinary Studies in Time, Rhythm and Motion, Department of Musicology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Olivier Lartillot
- RITMO Center for Interdisciplinary Studies in Time, Rhythm and Motion, Department of Musicology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Anne Danielsen
- RITMO Center for Interdisciplinary Studies in Time, Rhythm and Motion, Department of Musicology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Datseris G, Ziereis A, Albrecht T, Hagmayer Y, Priesemann V, Geisel T. Microtiming Deviations and Swing Feel in Jazz. Sci Rep 2019; 9:19824. [PMID: 31882842 PMCID: PMC6934603 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-55981-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2019] [Accepted: 12/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Jazz music that swings has the fascinating power to elicit a pleasant sensation of flow in listeners and the desire to synchronize body movements with the music. Whether microtiming deviations (MTDs), i.e. small timing deviations below the bar or phrase level, enhance the swing feel is highly debated in the current literature. Studies on other groove related genres did not find evidence for a positive impact of MTDs. The present study addresses jazz music and swing in particular, as there is some evidence that microtiming patterns are genre-specific. We recorded twelve piano jazz standards played by a professional pianist and manipulated the natural MTDs of the recordings in systematic ways by quantizing, expanding and inverting them. MTDs were defined with respect to a grid determined by the average swing ratio. The original and manipulated versions were presented in an online survey and evaluated by 160 listeners with various musical skill levels and backgrounds. Across pieces the quantized versions (without MTDs) were rated slightly higher and versions with expanded MTDs were rated lower with regard to swing than the original recordings. Unexpectedly, inversion had no impact on swing ratings except for two pieces. Our results suggest that naturally fluctuating MTDs are not an essential factor for the swing feel.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- George Datseris
- Max Planck Institute for Dynamics and Self-Organization, 37077, Göttingen, Germany.
- Department of Physics, Georg-August-University Göttingen, 37073, Göttingen, Germany.
| | - Annika Ziereis
- Georg-Elias-Mueller Institute for Psychology, Georg-August-University Göttingen, 37073, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Thorsten Albrecht
- Georg-Elias-Mueller Institute for Psychology, Georg-August-University Göttingen, 37073, Göttingen, Germany
| | - York Hagmayer
- Georg-Elias-Mueller Institute for Psychology, Georg-August-University Göttingen, 37073, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Viola Priesemann
- Max Planck Institute for Dynamics and Self-Organization, 37077, Göttingen, Germany
- Department of Physics, Georg-August-University Göttingen, 37073, Göttingen, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, 37077, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Theo Geisel
- Max Planck Institute for Dynamics and Self-Organization, 37077, Göttingen, Germany
- Department of Physics, Georg-August-University Göttingen, 37073, Göttingen, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, 37077, Göttingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Senn O, Kilchenmann L, Bechtold T, Hoesl F. Groove in drum patterns as a function of both rhythmic properties and listeners' attitudes. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0199604. [PMID: 29958289 PMCID: PMC6025871 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199604] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2018] [Accepted: 06/11/2018] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Music psychology defines groove as humans’ pleasureable urge to move their body in synchrony with music. Past research has found that rhythmic syncopation, event density, beat salience, and rhythmic variability are positively associated with groove. This exploratory study investigates the groove effect of 248 reconstructed drum patterns from different popular music styles (pop, rock, funk, heavy metal, rock’n’roll, hip hop, soul, R&B). It aims at identifying factors that might be relevant for groove and worth investigating in a controlled setting in the future. Drum patterns of eight bars duration, chosen from 248 popular music tracks, have been transcribed and audio reconstructions have been created on the basis of sound samples. During an online listening experiment, 665 participants rated the reconstructions a total of 8,329 times using a groove questionnaire. Results show that, among 15 tested variables, syncopation (R2 = 0.010) and event density (R2 = 0.011) were positively associated with the groove ratings. These effects were stronger in participants who were music professionals, compared to amateur musicians or mere listeners. A categorisation of the stimuli according to structural aspects was also associated with groove (R2 = 0.018). Beat salience, residual microtiming and rhythmic variability showed no effect on the groove ratings. Participants’ familiarity with a drum pattern had a positive influence on the groove ratings (η2 = 0.051). The largest isolated effect was measured for participants’ style bias (R2 = 0.123): groove ratings tended to be high if participants had the impression that the drum pattern belonged to a style they liked. Combined, the effects of style bias and familiarity (R2 = 0.152) exceeded the other effects as predictors for groove by a wide margin. We conclude that listeners’ taste, musical biographies and expertise have a strong effect on their groove experience. This motivates groove research not to focus on the music alone, but to take the listeners into account as well.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olivier Senn
- School of Music, Lucerne University of Applied Sciences and Arts, Lucerne, Switzerland
- * E-mail:
| | - Lorenz Kilchenmann
- School of Music, Lucerne University of Applied Sciences and Arts, Lucerne, Switzerland
| | - Toni Bechtold
- School of Music, Lucerne University of Applied Sciences and Arts, Lucerne, Switzerland
| | - Florian Hoesl
- School of Music, Lucerne University of Applied Sciences and Arts, Lucerne, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|