1
|
Clark TK, Newman MC, Karmali F, Oman CM, Merfeld DM. Mathematical models for dynamic, multisensory spatial orientation perception. PROGRESS IN BRAIN RESEARCH 2019; 248:65-90. [PMID: 31239146 DOI: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.04.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Mathematical models have been proposed for how the brain interprets sensory information to produce estimates of self-orientation and self-motion. This process, spatial orientation perception, requires dynamically integrating multiple sensory modalities, including visual, vestibular, and somatosensory cues. Here, we review the progress in mathematical modeling of spatial orientation perception, focusing on dynamic multisensory models, and the experimental paradigms in which they have been validated. These models are primarily "black box" or "as if" models for how the brain processes spatial orientation cues. Yet, they have been effective scientifically, in making quantitative hypotheses that can be empirically assessed, and operationally, in investigating aircraft pilot disorientation, for example. The primary family of models considered, the observer model, implements estimation theory approaches, hypothesizing that internal models (i.e., neural systems replicating the behavior/dynamics of physical systems) are used to produce expected sensory measurements. Expected signals are then compared to actual sensory afference, yielding sensory conflict, which is weighted to drive central perceptions of gravity, angular velocity, and translation. This approach effectively predicts a wide range of experimental scenarios using a small set of fixed free parameters. We conclude with limitations and applications of existing mathematical models and important areas of future work.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Torin K Clark
- Smead Aerospace Engineering Sciences, University of Colorado-Boulder, Boulder, CO, United States.
| | - Michael C Newman
- Environmental Tectonics Corporation, Southampton, PA, United States
| | - Faisal Karmali
- Jenks Vestibular Physiology Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA, United States; Otolaryngology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Charles M Oman
- Human Systems Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States
| | - Daniel M Merfeld
- Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States; Naval Aerospace Medical Research Lab (NAMRL), Naval Medical Research Unit-Dayton (NAMRUD), Dayton, OH, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Matin L, Matin E, Li W. Dipole analysis of the influence of linear arrays of points on visually perceived eye level (VPEL). Vision Res 2016; 126:254-263. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.11.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2014] [Revised: 11/02/2015] [Accepted: 11/05/2015] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
3
|
Wu J, He ZJ, Ooi TL. Visually Perceived Eye Level and Horizontal Midline of the Body Trunk Influenced by Optic Flow. Perception 2016; 34:1045-60. [PMID: 16245484 DOI: 10.1068/p5416] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
The eye level and the horizontal midline of the body trunk can serve, respectively as references for judging the vertical and horizontal egocentric directions. We investigated whether the optic-flow pattern, which is the dynamic motion information generated when one moves in the visual world, can be used by the visual system to determine and calibrate these two references. Using a virtual-reality setup to generate the optic-flow pattern, we showed that judged elevation of the eye level and the azimuth of the horizontal midline of the body trunk are biased toward the positional placement of the focus of expansion (FOE) of the optic-flow pattern. Furthermore, for the vertical reference, prolonged viewing of an optic-flow pattern with lowered FOE not only causes a lowered judged eye level after removal of the optic-flow pattern, but also an overestimation of distance in the dark. This is equivalent to a reduction in the judged angular declination of the object after adaptation, indicating that the optic-flow information also plays a role in calibrating the extraretinal signals used to establish the vertical reference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jun Wu
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40292, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Gambelli CN, Theisen D, Willems PA, Schepens B. Motor Control of Landing from a Jump in Simulated Hypergravity. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0141574. [PMID: 26505472 PMCID: PMC4624769 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141574] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2015] [Accepted: 10/09/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
On Earth, when landing from a counter-movement jump, muscles contract before touchdown to anticipate imminent collision with the ground and place the limbs in a proper position. This study assesses how the control of landing is modified when gravity is increased above 1 g. Hypergravity was simulated in two different ways: (1) by generating centrifugal forces during turns of an aircraft (A300) and (2) by pulling the subject downwards in the laboratory with a Subject Loading System (SLS). Eight subjects were asked to perform counter-movement jumps at 1 g on Earth and at 3 hypergravity levels (1.2, 1.4 and 1.6 g) both in A300 and with SLS. External forces applied to the body, movements of the lower limb segments and muscular activity of 6 lower limb muscles were recorded. Our results show that both in A300 and with SLS, as in 1 g: (1) the anticipation phase is present; (2) during the loading phase (from touchdown until the peak of vertical ground reaction force), lower limb muscles act like a stiff spring, whereas during the second part (from the peak of vertical ground reaction force until the return to the standing position), they act like a compliant spring associated with a damper. (3) With increasing gravity, the preparatory adjustments and the loading phase are modified whereas the second part does not change drastically. (4) The modifications are similar in A300 and with SLS, however the effect of hypergravity is accentuated in A300, probably due to altered sensory inputs. This observation suggests that otolithic information plays an important role in the control of the landing from a jump.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clément N. Gambelli
- Laboratory of Physiology and Biomechanics of Locomotion, Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain (UCL), Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Daniel Theisen
- Sports Medicine Research Laboratory, Luxembourg Institute of Health, Luxembourg, Grand Duchy of Luxembourg
| | - Patrick A. Willems
- Laboratory of Physiology and Biomechanics of Locomotion, Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain (UCL), Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Bénédicte Schepens
- Laboratory of Physiology and Biomechanics of Locomotion, Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain (UCL), Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Clark TK, Newman MC, Oman CM, Merfeld DM, Young LR. Human perceptual overestimation of whole body roll tilt in hypergravity. J Neurophysiol 2014; 113:2062-77. [PMID: 25540216 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00095.2014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2014] [Accepted: 12/22/2014] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Hypergravity provides a unique environment to study human perception of orientation. We utilized a long-radius centrifuge to study perception of both static and dynamic whole body roll tilt in hypergravity, across a range of angles, frequencies, and net gravito-inertial levels (referred to as G levels). While studies of static tilt perception in hypergravity have been published, this is the first to measure dynamic tilt perception (i.e., with time-varying canal stimulation) in hypergravity using a continuous matching task. In complete darkness, subjects reported their orientation perception using a haptic task, whereby they attempted to align a hand-held bar with their perceived horizontal. Static roll tilt was overestimated in hypergravity, with more overestimation at larger angles and higher G levels, across the conditions tested (overestimated by ∼35% per additional G level, P < 0.001). As our primary contribution, we show that dynamic roll tilt was also consistently overestimated in hypergravity (P < 0.001) at all angles and frequencies tested, again with more overestimation at higher G levels. The overestimation was similar to that for static tilts at low angular velocities but decreased at higher angular velocities (P = 0.006), consistent with semicircular canal sensory integration. To match our findings, we propose a modification to a previous Observer-type canal-otolith interaction model. Specifically, our data were better modeled by including the hypothesis that the central nervous system treats otolith stimulation in the utricular plane differently than stimulation out of the utricular plane. This modified model was able to simulate quantitatively both the static and the dynamic roll tilt overestimation in hypergravity measured experimentally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Torin K Clark
- Man Vehicle Laboratory, Department of Aeronautics and Astronautics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts; Charles Stark Draper Laboratory, Incorporated, Cambridge, Massachusetts;
| | - Michael C Newman
- National Aerospace Training and Research Center, Southampton, Pennsylvania; and
| | - Charles M Oman
- Man Vehicle Laboratory, Department of Aeronautics and Astronautics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts
| | - Daniel M Merfeld
- Jenks Vestibular Physiology Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Department of Otology and Laryngology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Laurence R Young
- Man Vehicle Laboratory, Department of Aeronautics and Astronautics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Otolith signals contribute to inter-individual differences in the perception of gravity-centered space. Exp Brain Res 2014; 232:1037-45. [PMID: 24430025 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3816-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2013] [Accepted: 12/16/2013] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
The aim of the present study was to investigate (1) the relative contribution of the egocentric reference as well as body orientation perception to visual horizon percept during tilt or during increased gravito-inertial acceleration (GiA, hypergravity environment) conditions and (2) the role of vestibular signals in the inter-individual differences observed in these perceptual modalities. Perceptual estimates analysis showed that backward tilt induced (1) an elevation of the visual horizon, (2) an elevation of the egocentric estimation (visual straight ahead) and (3) an overestimation of body tilt. The increase in the magnitude of GiA induced (1) a lowering of the apparent horizon, (2) a lowering of the straight ahead and (3) a perception of backward tilt. Overall, visual horizon percept can be expressed as the combination of body orientation perception and egocentric estimation. When assessing otolith reactivity using off-vertical axis rotation (OVAR), only visual egocentric estimation was significantly correlated with horizontal OVAR performance. On the one hand, we found a correlation between a low modulation amplitude of the otolith responses and straight ahead accuracy when the head axis was tilted relative to gravity. On the other hand, the bias of otolith responses was significantly correlated with straight ahead accuracy when subjects were submitted to an increase in the GiA. Thus, straight ahead sense would be dependent to some extent to otolith function. These results are discussed in terms of the contribution of otolith inputs in the overall multimodal integration subtending spatial constancy.
Collapse
|
7
|
Optimal estimator models for spatial orientation and vestibular nystagmus. Exp Brain Res 2011; 210:465-76. [PMID: 21416377 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2595-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2010] [Accepted: 02/04/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Mathematical models have played an important role in research on the vestibular system over the past century, from the torsion pendulum analogies of the semicircular canal to the optimal estimator "observer" models of multisensory interaction and adaptation. This short review is limited to our own contributions in bringing the technology of feedback control theory to bear on the understanding of human spatial orientation, eye movements, and nystagmus, both on Earth and in space. It points to the importance of the "internal model" concept for treatment of the manner in which the brain constantly makes predictions about future sensory feedback, adjusts the weightings of sensors according to their signal-to-noise ratios, and adapts control according to the motion environment, and availability of sensory cues.
Collapse
|
8
|
Paillard A, Denise P, Barraud PA, Roux A, Cian C. The elevator illusion results from the combination of body orientation and egocentric perception. Neurosci Lett 2009; 464:156-9. [PMID: 19683558 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.08.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2009] [Revised: 08/10/2009] [Accepted: 08/10/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
9
|
Bringoux L, Robic G, Gauthier GM, Vercher JL. Judging beforehand the possibility of passing under obstacles without motion: the influence of egocentric and geocentric frames of reference. Exp Brain Res 2007; 185:673-80. [PMID: 17989965 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-1194-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2007] [Accepted: 10/20/2007] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- L Bringoux
- UMR CNRS 6152 Mouvement & Perception, Faculté des Sciences du Sport, Université de la Méditerranée, 163, avenue de Luminy CP 910, 13288 Marseille Cedex 9, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Seidman SH. Translational motion perception and vestiboocular responses in the absence of non-inertial cues. Exp Brain Res 2007; 184:13-29. [PMID: 17680240 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-1072-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2006] [Accepted: 07/18/2007] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Path integration studies in humans show that we have the ability to accurately reproduce our path in the absence of visual information. It has been suggested that this ability is supported by acceleration signals, as transduced by the otolith organs, which may then be integrated twice to produce path excursion. Vestibuloocular responses to linear translations (LVOR), however, show considerable frequency dependence, with substantial attenuation in response to low frequency translational motion. If otolith information were processed similarly by path integration mechanisms, the resulting signal would not be sufficient to account for robust path integration for stimuli typically used in such studies. We hypothesized that such behavior relies upon cognitive skill and transient otolith cues, typically combined with non-directional cues of motion, such as vibration and noise produced by the mechanics apparatus used to produce linear motion. Continuous motion estimation tasks were used to assess translation perception, while eye movement recordings revealed LVOR responses, in 12 normal and 2 vestibulopathic human subjects while riding on a sled designed to specifically minimize non-directional motion cues. In the near absence of such cues, perceptual responses, like the LVOR, showed high-pass characteristics. This implies that otolith signals are not sufficient to support previously observed path integration behaviors, which must be supplemented by non-directional motion cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S H Seidman
- Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14642, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
The linear perspective information in ground surface representation and distance judgment. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2007; 69:654-72. [PMID: 17929690 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193769] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
12
|
Tribukait A, Eiken O. The human sense of the head’s polarity is influenced by changes in the magnitude of gravity. Brain Cogn 2007; 63:24-30. [PMID: 16935402 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.07.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2005] [Revised: 07/11/2006] [Accepted: 07/15/2006] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
The present investigation concerns the integrity of a primary mental function, the egocentric frame of reference and the sense of polarity of one's own head. The visually perceived eye level (VPEL) and the subjective antero-posterior axis of the head were measured by means of a visual indicator in darkness during two stimulus conditions: static pitch (sagittal-plane) tilting in the 1-g environment and gondola centrifugation (2G). It is demonstrated that an increase in the magnitude of the gravitoinertial (G) force, acting in the direction of the head and body long (z) axis, causes a substantial change not only in the VPEL but also in the perceived direction of the antero-posterior axis of the head.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arne Tribukait
- Defence Research Agency, FOI Centre for Environmental Physiology, Berzelius väg 13, Karolinska Institutet, SE 171 77 Stockholm, Sweden.
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Tribukait A, Eiken O. On the role of otoliths and semicircular canals in spatial orientation: Dynamics of the visually perceived eye level during gondola centrifugation. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2006; 67:1242-51. [PMID: 16502845 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193556] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The visually perceived eye level (VPEL) was measured during gondola centrifugation. Subjects (N = 11) were seated upright, facing motion in a swing-out gondola The head was adjusted so that Reid's baseline was tilted 10 degrees anterior end up. The subjects were requested to adjust the position of a small luminous dot so that it was perceived as gravitationally at eye level. In the 1-g environment, the VPEL was a few degrees below the true gravitational eye level (M = -1.75 degrees, SD = 1.90 degrees). After rapid acceleration of the centrifuge to 2 G (vectorial sum of the earth gravity force and the centrifugal force), there was an exponentially increasing depression of the VPEL. The initial value was -6.4 degrees +/- 5.2 degrees. During 10 min at 2 G, the VPEL approached an asymptotic value of -24.8 degrees +/- 5.4 degrees. The time constant showed a large interindividual variability, ranging from 59 to 1,000 sec (M = 261 sec, median = 147 sec). The findings are discussed, taking into consideration otolith-semicircular-canal interaction, as well as memory functions of the vestibular system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arne Tribukait
- Karolinska Institute and Swedish Defense Research Agency, Stockholm, Sweden.
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Carriot J, Charles C, Huffenus AF, Nougier V, Raphel C, Cian C. Effect of low gravitational stimulation on the perception of target elevation: Role of spatial expertise. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005; 67:1044-51. [PMID: 16396012 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193630] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
To examine the interindividual differences in the judgment of the visually perceived eye level (VPEL-upright position) and of the visually perceived apparent zenith (VPAZ-supine position) when the subject is subjected to low gravitational-inertial force (GIF), we independently altered GIF in two different populations: control subjects and spatial experts. Subjects were instructed to set a luminous target to the eye level while they were in total darkness and motionless or undergoing low radial acceleration with respect to the threshold of the otolithic system (0.015-1.67 m/sec2 for the VPEL and 0.55-2.19 m/sec2 for the VPAZ, respectively). Results showed that (1) low GIFs, close to those met during daily life, induced an eye level lowering in the upright and supine positions for the control group, and (2) the spatial expertise modified the influence of low GIF. Whereas an oculogravic illusion was found for the control group, this phenomenon was absent (VPAZ) or weaker (VPEL) for the spatial experts. Thus, the relations that the subjects maintain with their spatial environment and the knowledge acquired through experience modify the processing of sensory information and the perceptive construction resulting from it. The interindividual differences in sensitivity to the oculogravic illusion are discussed in terms of sensory dominance and of a better efficiency in the use of the available sensory information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jérôme Carriot
- Centre de Recherches du Service de Santé des Armées, Grenoble, France
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of adaptation to visuomotor rotations have recently been explored in depth. However, little data are available regarding interlimb transfer of adaptation to novel inertial dynamics. The present study thus investigated interlimb transfer of dynamics by examining the effect of initial training with one arm on subsequent performance with the other in adaptation to a 1.5-kg mass attached eccentrically to the forearm. Using inverse dynamic analysis, we examined the changes in torque strategies associated with adaptation to the extra mass, and with interlimb transfer of that adaptation. Following initial training with the dominant arm, nondominant arm performance improved substantially in terms of linearity and initial direction control as compared with naïve performance. However, initial training with the nondominant arm had no effect on subsequent performance with the dominant arm. Inverse dynamic analysis revealed that improvements in kinematics were implemented by increasing flexor muscle torques at the elbow to counter load-induced increases in extensor interaction torques as well as increasing flexor muscle torques at the shoulder to counter the extensor actions of elbow muscle torque. Following opposite arm adaptation, the nondominant arm adopted this dynamic strategy early in adaptation. These findings suggest that dominant arm adaptation to novel inertial dynamics leads to information that can be accessed and utilized by the opposite arm controller, but not vice versa. When compared with our previous findings on interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations, our current findings suggest that adaptations to visuomotor and dynamic transformations are mediated by distinct neural mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jinsung Wang
- Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Matin L, Li W. Neural model for processing the influence of visual orientation on visually perceived eye level (VPEL). Vision Res 2001; 41:2845-72. [PMID: 11701180 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00150-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
An individual line or a combination of lines viewed in darkness has a large influence on the elevation to which an observer sets a target so that it is perceived to lie at eye level (VPEL). These influences are systematically related to the orientation of pitched-from-vertical lines on pitched plane(s) and to the lengths of the lines, as well as to the orientations of lines of 'equivalent pitch' that lie on frontoparallel planes. A three-stage model processes the visual influence: The first stage parallel processes the orientations of the lines utilizing 2 classes of orientation-sensitive neural units in each hemisphere, with the two classes sensitive to opposing ranges of orientations; the signal delivered by each class is of opposite sign in the two hemispheres. The second stage generates the total visual influence from the parallel combination of inputs delivered by the 4 groups of the first stage, and a third stage combines the total visual influence from the second stage with signals from the body-referenced mechanism that contains information about the position and orientation of the eyes, head, and body. The circuit equation describing the combined influence of n separate inputs from stage 1 on the output of the stage 2 integrating neuron is derived for n stimulus lines which possess any combination of orientations and lengths; Each of the n lines is assumed to stimulate one of the groups of orientation-sensitive units in visual cortex (stage 1) whose signals converge on to a dendrite of the integrating neuron (stage 2), and to produce changes in postsynaptic membrane conductance (g(i)) and potential (V(i)) there. The net current from the n dendrites results in a voltage change (V(A)) at the initial segment of the axon of the integrating neuron. Nerve impulse frequency proportional to this voltage change signals the total visual influence on perceived elevation of the visual field. The circuit equation corresponding to the total visual influence for n equal length inducing lines is V(A)= sum V(i)/[n+(g(A)/g(S))], where the potential change due to line i, V(i), is proportional to line orientation, g(A) is the conductance at the axon's summing point, and g(S)=g(i) for each i for the equal length case; the net conductance change due to a line is proportional to the line's length. The circuit equation is interpreted as a basis for quantitative predictions from the model that can be compared to psychophysical measurements of the elevation of VPEL. The interpretation provides the predicted relation for the visual influence on VPEL, V, by n inducing lines each with length l: thus, V=a+[k(i) sum theta(i)/n+(k(2)/l)], where theta(i) is the orientation of line i, a is the effect of the body-referenced mechanism, and k(1) and k(2) are constants. The model's output is fitted to the results of five sets of experiments in which the elevation of VPEL measured with a small target in the median plane is systematically influenced by distantly located 1-line or 2-line inducing stimuli varying in orientation and length and viewed in otherwise total darkness with gaze restricted to the median plane; each line is located at either 25 degrees eccentricity to the left or right of the median plane. The model predicts the negatively accelerated growth of VPEL with line length for each orientation and the change of slope constant of the linear combination rule among lines from 1.00 (linear summation; short lines) to 0.61 (near-averaging; long lines). Fits to the data are obtained over a range of orientations from -30 degrees to +30 degrees of pitch for 1-line visual fields from lengths of 3 degrees to 64 degrees, for parallel 2-line visual fields over the same range of lengths and orientations, for short and long 2-line combinations in which each of the two members may have any orientation (parallel or nonparallel pairs), and for the well-illuminated and fully structured pitchroom. In addition, similar experiments with 2-line stimuli of equivalent pitch in the frontoparallel plane were also fitted to the model. The model accounts for more than 98% of the variance of the results in each case.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Matin
- Clarence H. Graham Memorial Laboratory of Visual Science, Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 1027, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Li W, Dallal N, Matin L. Influences of visual pitch and visual yaw on visually perceived eye level (VPEL) and straight ahead (VPSA) for erect and rolled-to-horizontal observers. Vision Res 2001; 41:2873-94. [PMID: 11701181 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00165-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Localization within the space in front of an observer can be specified along two orthogonal physical dimensions: elevation ('up', 'down') and horizontal ('left','right'). For the erect observer, these correspond to egocentric dimensions along the long and short axes of the body, respectively. However, when subjects are rolled-to-horizontal (lying on their sides), the correspondence between the physical and egocentric dimensions is reversed. Employing egocentric coordinates, localization can be referred to a central perceptual point-visually perceived eye level (VPEL) along the long axis of the body, and visually perceived straight ahead (VPSA) along the short axis of the body. In the present experiment, measurements of VPEL and of VPSA were made on each of eight subjects who were either erect or rolled-to-horizontal while monocularly viewing a long 2-line stimulus (two parallel, 64 degrees -long lines separated by 50 degrees ) in otherwise complete darkness that was centered on the eye of the observer and was tilted out of the frontoparallel plane by a variable amount and direction (from -30 degrees to +30 degrees in 10 degrees steps). The stimulus tilt was either around an axis through the center of the two eyes (pitch; VPEL was measured) or around the long axis of the body that passed through the center of the viewing eye (yaw; VPSA was measured). Large variations in the localization settings were measured that were systematic with stimulus tilt. The slopes of the functions plouing the deviations from veridicality against the orientation of the 2-line stimulus ('induction functions') were larger for the rolled-to-horizontal observer than for the erect observer for both VPEL and VPSA, and for a given body orientation were larger for the VPEL discrimination than for the VPSA discrimination; the influences of body orientation in physical space and the direction of the discrimination relative to the body were lineraly additive. Both the y-intercepts of the induction functions and the central perceptual point measured in complete darkness were lower when the norm setting by the subject was along the vertical than when it was along the horizontal; this held for both the VPEL and VPSA discriminations. The systematic effects of body orientation on the slopes and of line orientation on the y-intercepts and dark values result from an effect of gravity on the settings and fit well to a general principle: any departure from erect posture increases the induction effects of the visual stimulus. The effect of gravity is consistent with the effect of gravity in previous work in high-g environments with the VPEL discrimination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- W Li
- Clarence H. Graham Memorial Laboratory of Visual Science, Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Cohen MM, Stoper AE, Welch RB, DeRoshia CW. Effects of gravitational and optical stimulation on the perception of target elevation. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 2001; 63:29-35. [PMID: 11304014 DOI: 10.3758/bf03200500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
To examine the combined effects of gravitational and optical stimulation on perceived target elevation, we independently altered gravitational-inertial force and both the orientation and the structure of a background visual array. While being exposed to 1.0, 1.5, or 2.0 Gz in the human centrifuge at NASA Ames Research Center, observers attempted to set a target to the apparent horizon. The target was viewed against the far wall of a box that was pitched at various angles. The box was brightly illuminated, had only its interior edges dimly illuminated, or was kept dark. Observers lowered their target settings as Gz was increased; this effect was weakened when the box was illuminated. Also, when the box was visible, settings were displaced in the same direction as that in which the box was pitched. We attribute our results to the combined influence of otolith-oculomotor mechanisms that underlie the elevator illusion and visual-oculomotor mechanisms (optostatic responses) that underlie the perceptual effects of viewing pitched visual arrays.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M M Cohen
- NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett Field, California 94035-1000, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Raphel C, Cian C, Barraud PA, Micheyl C. Effects of supine body position and low radial accelerations on the visually perceived apparent zenith. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 2001; 63:36-46. [PMID: 11304015 DOI: 10.3758/bf03200501] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The visually perceived eye level (VPEL) has been shown to shift toward the lower part of the body in upright subjects facing toward the axis of rotation on a centrifuge. This shift occurs in the same direction as the shift in the gravito-inertial forces (Gis) produced by very low radial acceleration (centrifugation) combined with gravity. The purpose of this study was to determine whether the same phenomenon affects the visually perceived apparent zenith (VPAZ) in subjects in a supine position. Twelve supine subjects were instructed to set a luminous target to the VPAZ, either while they were in total darkness and motionless or while undergoing very low centrifugation. Data showed that Gis induced a VPAZ shift similar to that observed for the VPEL. Thus, as is the case for the VPEL, the corresponding logarithmic psychophysical function of the VPAZ may be considered to be a type of oculogravic illusion phenomenon with differences in the subjects' that differs from subject to subject, depending on the subject's sensitivity to low radial accelerations. Data on VPEL and VPAZ support the notion that the subjective perception of eye level in total darkness takes into account changes--even if extremely slight-in the direction of the gravito-inertial forces produced by the combination of gravity and low radial accelerations, although subjects are unaware of the Gi shift. However, depending on the intensity of the radial acceleration and the angular deviation of Gi relative to G, the shift of the VPEL and the VPAZ can be either amplified or attenuated. Moreover, differences between VPEL and VPAZ responses suggest two explanatory assumptions--namely, that this is (1) a peripheral phenomenon dependent on the neurophysiological anisotropy of the otolithic system or (2) a central phenomenon dependent on the relevance assigned to the peripheral information by the integrative sensory functions and the associative processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Raphel
- Centre de Recherches du Service de Santé des Armées, La Tronche, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Raphel C, Barraud PA, Koessler C, Cian C. Effects of a visual frame and of low radial accelerations on the visually perceived eye level. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1996; 58:906-14. [PMID: 8768185 DOI: 10.3758/bf03205492] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to determine how the combined effects of a reference frame and of very low gravito-inertial forces produced by centrifugation affect the visually perceived eye level (VPEL). Twenty subjects were instructed to set a luminous target to the VPEL under various experimental conditions involving two main factors: (1) visual context (frameless, frame centered, frame moved down 50 mm, and frame moved up 50 mm) and (2) gravito-inertial context (motionless, Gi1 = 9.81001 m/sec2 and Gi2 = 9.95 m/sec2). The visual context significantly reduced the lowering of VPEL in darkness as caused by radial acceleration; this confirms the prevailing role of vision versus propriosomesthesis. However, under condition Gi2, there was a significant effect on the VPEL in spite of the presence of the luminous frame; this demonstrates that VPEL processing involves both visual and propriosomesthesic information. Furthermore, the VPEL varied linearly with the vertical shift of the luminous frame for any of the gravito-inertial conditions used in this study, but, under condition Gi2, the VPEL was shifted downward.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Raphel
- Centre de Recherches du Service de Santé des Armées, La Tronche, France
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Affiliation(s)
- S H Seidman
- Department of Neurology, University of Rochester Medical Center, New York 14642, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
This contribution examines the relation between the subjective visual vertical, the subjective visual horizontal, and the perceived body position of human subjects. Firmly fixed on a tiltable chair with head and torso restrained, 11 healthy subjects were rolled sideways and indicated their subjective horizontal body position. In these positions the subjects were also asked to adjust a luminous line alternately to the vertical and to the horizontal. The adjustments of the subjective horizontal body position cluster around a mean of 96.3 degrees with a remarkably broad range (SD: 19.7 degrees). In the subjective horizontal body position, the luminous line does not appear horizontal when in line with one's own spinal axis. It is set further down by 27.4 degrees on average and, therefore, perpendicular to the subjective visual vertical. This finding supports the idea that the judgement of the own body position and the judgement of the orientation of a seen object respective to gravity are based on different references. Contradictory to other investigations [23,24], is the empirical fact that the individual subjects were not able to adjust the horizontal body position with the reported accuracy (range of mean adjustments 77.5 to 117.6 degrees).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- F Mast
- University of Zurich, Department of Psychology, Switzerland
| | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
|
24
|
Welch RB, Cohen MM, DeRoshia CW. Reduction of the elevator illusion from continued hypergravity exposure and visual error-corrective feedback. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1996; 58:22-30. [PMID: 8668516 DOI: 10.3758/bf03205471] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Ten subjects served as their own controls in two conditions of continuous, centrifugally produced hypergravity (+2 Gz) and a 1-G control condition. Before and after exposure, open-loop measures were obtained of (1) motor control, (2) visual localization, and (3) hand-eye coordination. During exposure in the visual feedback/hypergravity condition, subjects received terminal visual error-corrective feedback from their target pointing, and in the no-visual feedback/hypergravity condition they pointed open loop. As expected, the motor control measures for both experimental conditions revealed very short lived underreaching (the muscle-loading effect) at the outset of hypergravity and an equally transient negative aftereffect on returning to 1 G. The substantial (approximately 17 degrees) initial elevator illusion experienced in both hypergravity conditions declined over the course of the exposure period, whether or not visual feedback was provided. This effect was tentatively attributed to habituation of the otoliths. Visual feedback produced a smaller additional decrement and a postexposure negative after-effect, possible evidence for visual recalibration. Surprisingly, the target-pointing error made during hypergravity in the no-visual-feedback condition was substantially less than that predicted by subjects' elevator illusion. This finding calls into question the neural outflow model as a complete explanation of this illusion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R B Welch
- NASA-Ames Research Center, Moffett Field, CA 94035-1000, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Mueller CH, Kornilova L, Wiest G, Steinhoff N. Psychophysical studies of visuo-vestibular interaction in microgravity. ACTA ASTRONAUTICA 1994; 33:9-13. [PMID: 11539543 DOI: 10.1016/0094-5765(94)90102-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Vertical linear vection from stimulation with a sinus velocity profile was investigated in two cosmonauts during two long term space flights and the results were compared to vertical vection responses of a cosmonaut who had been tested during the first days in microgravity of a previous space flight. While the pursuit ability in the eye/hand sensorimotor task was stable in all subjects, vertical vection frequency as well as phase response was altered only in the short term cosmonaut during the immediate adaptation phase. The vection responses of the two long term cosmonauts, who were tested after 4 weeks, 10 weeks and 14 weeks respectively in microgravity were much more stable. These findings correlate with the suggestion of different phases of adaptation to microgravity at different flight stages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C h Mueller
- Department of Neurology, University of Vienna, Austria
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Affiliation(s)
- M M Cohen
- National Aeronautics and Space Administration, Ames Research Center, Moffett Field, California 94035
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Chapter 7 Visual-Motor Control in Altered Gravity. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1992. [DOI: 10.1016/s0166-4115(08)62014-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/21/2023]
|
28
|
Stoper AE, Cohen MM. Effect of structured visual environments on apparent eye level. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1989; 46:469-75. [PMID: 2813032 DOI: 10.3758/bf03210862] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
Each of 12 subjects set a binocularly viewed target to apparent eye level; the target was projected on the rear wall of an open box, the floor of which was horizontal or pitched up and down at angles of 7.5 degrees and 15 degrees. Settings of the target were systematically biased by 60% of the pitch angle when the interior of the box was illuminated, but by only 5% when the interior of the box was darkened. Within-subjects variability of the settings was less under illuminated viewing conditions than in the dark, but was independent of box pitch angle. In a second experiment, 11 subjects were tested with an illuminated pitched box, yielding biases of 53% and 49% for binocular and monocular viewing conditions, respectively. The results are discussed in terms of individual and interactive effects of optical, gravitational, and extraretinal eye-position information in determining judgements of eye level.
Collapse
|
29
|
Matin L, Fox CR. Visually perceived eye level and perceived elevation of objects: linearly additive influences from visual field pitch and from gravity. Vision Res 1989; 29:315-24. [PMID: 2773342 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90080-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 70] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
Observing a pitched visual field (i.e. tilted around a horizontal axis in the observer's frontal plane) results in large changes in the elevation visually perceived to correspond to eye level (VPEL) and in the perceived elevation and size of stationary objects viewed against the field. With topforward pitch (top toward observer) VPEL lies above true eye level and objects appear smaller and lower; with topbackward pitch VPEL lies below true eye level and objects appear larger and higher. Oscillation of the pitched field induces synchronous perceived oscillation of elevation of a stationary target viewed against the field. Typical VPEL settings deviated from true eye level by 20 degrees with the field pitched at 40 degrees, although some individuals mislocalized by as much as 40 degrees. VPEL varied linearly with visual field pitch with individual slopes for the relation between VPEL and visual field pitch ranging from +0.42 to +0.78 (avg = +0.56). The linear correlation (r) between VPEL in darkness and against an erect visual field was +0.91. The two relations--VPEL vs visual field pitch, VPEL in darkness vs VPEL in the erect illuminated visual field (slope approximately equal to 0.5)--are both accurately predicted by the linear model: VPEL = kvV + kbB; in which V is the influence of visual field structure and B is the influence of the body-referenced mechanism which combines information regarding the orientation of the head relative to gravity, the position of the eye in the orbit, and the vertical location of the image on the retina; kv and kb are the relative weights of V and B with kv + kb = 1. In an illuminated field kv = kb approximately equal to 0.5; in the dark kv = 0, kb = 1.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Matin
- Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Stoper AE, Cohen MM. Judgments of eye level in light and in darkness. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1986; 40:311-6. [PMID: 3786100 DOI: 10.3758/bf03203022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
|
31
|
Eye-position aftereffects of backward head tilt manifested by illusory visual direction. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1977. [DOI: 10.3758/bf03199472] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
32
|
Ormsby CC, Young LR. Integration of semicircular canal and otolith information for multisensory orientation stimuli. Math Biosci 1977. [DOI: 10.1016/0025-5564(77)90032-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
33
|
Ebenholtz SM, Shebilske W. The Doll Reflex: ocular counterrolling with head-body tilt in the median plane. Vision Res 1975; 15:713-7. [PMID: 1138488 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(75)90289-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
|
34
|
|