1
|
Luftig RL. An Analysis of Initial Sign Lexicons as a Function of Eight Learnability Variables. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/154079698400900305] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Recent interest in the use of manual sign intervention for language-impaired, severely mentally retarded individuals has led to creation of initial sign lexicons for such clients. However, the overriding basis for inclusion of sign/concepts in initial lexicons has been concept functionality. The present investigates the studies inclusion of sign/concepts in two initial sign lexicons as a function of eight sign learnability variables. For an adolescent/adult sign lexicon, the referent variables of concept concreteness and word frequency were significantly represented while the learnability variables of sign translucency and other production variables were only marginally represented. For the elementary-aged lexicon, none of the eight variables were significantly represented. The results are discussed in terms of learning ease of the lexicons and possible curricular language intervention implications.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Richard L. Luftig
- Richard L. Luftig, Associate Professor, Special Education, Miami University
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Luftig RL, Page JL, Lloyd LL. Ratings of Translucency in Manual Signs as a Predictor of Sign Learnability. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/152574018300600205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Increased interest has begun to be shown toward manual sign as an augmentative communication mode for language-impaired individuals. Unfortunately, relatively less attention has been given to delineating psycholinguistic variables which might affect sign learning. One attribute shown to facilitate sign learning is the perceived translucency (relatedness) between sign and gloss. The current project was an attempt to obtain translucency ratings for a large sample of manual signs as well as a comparison of the relative translucency of nouns to verbs.
Collapse
|
3
|
Marschark M, Morrison C, Lukomski J, Borgna G, Convertino C. Are Deaf Students Visual Learners? LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2013; 25:156-162. [PMID: 23750095 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
It is frequently assumed that by virtue of their hearing losses, deaf students are visual learners. Deaf individuals have some visual-spatial advantages relative to hearing individuals, but most have been are linked to use of sign language rather than auditory deprivation. How such cognitive differences might affect academic performance has been investigated only rarely. This study examined relations among deaf college students' language and visual-spatial abilities, mathematics problem solving, and hearing thresholds. Results extended some previous findings and clarified others. Contrary to what might be expected, hearing students exhibited visual-spatial skills equal to or better than deaf students. Scores on a Spatial Relations task were associated with better mathematics problem solving. Relations among the several variables, however, suggested that deaf students are no more likely to be visual learners than hearing students and that their visual-spatial skill may be related more to their hearing than to sign language skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc Marschark
- Center for Education Research Partnerships, National Technical Institute for the Deaf - Rochester Institute of Technology, 52 Lomb Memorial Drive, Rochester, NY 14623 USA ; School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, Scotland
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Panda A, Bhattacharya C, Banerjee M. Does Absence of Hearing Ability Enhance Visual Vigilance? It Depends on Task Complexity. PSYCHOLOGICAL STUDIES 2011. [DOI: 10.1007/s12646-011-0089-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022] Open
|
5
|
Marschark M. Memory for language in deaf adults and children. SCANDINAVIAN AUDIOLOGY. SUPPLEMENTUM 1999; 49:87-92. [PMID: 10209782 DOI: 10.1080/010503998420702] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive psychologists have known for a long time that language and memory are intimately related in people who can hear. Why should the situation be any different for deaf children and deaf adults? This article considers the results of previous studies and some new findings in examining the possible impact of spoken language and sign language fluencies/preferences on the structure and process of memory in deaf individuals. Current evidence suggests that there are some differences in the organization of long-term memory in deaf as compared to hearing people, but no one has yet demonstrated such differences to be so large that they qualitatively or quantitatively affect learning in any real sense. In contrast, there is now abundant evidence to suggest that variation in spoken language abilities have a direct impact on memory span and perhaps on working memory more generally. These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for the education of students who are deaf or hard of hearing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Marschark
- National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of Technology, New York, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Novack LL, Bonvillian JD. Word recall in deaf students: the effects of different coding strategies. Percept Mot Skills 1996; 83:627-39. [PMID: 8902042 DOI: 10.2466/pms.1996.83.2.627] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
This study examined whether instructions to use specific word coding strategies affected deaf students immediate and delayed final free recall of English word lists. Both the word-coding strategy and the visual imagery value of the words were important factors in word recall. 44 deaf students participated. Those who received instructions to produce the sign language equivalent of each stimulus word tended to recall more words over all than those students instructed to fingerspell each word or those instructed to form a sign language sentence that included the stimulus word. Stimulus words rated high in imagery value were recalled more frequently than words with low imagery values across coding strategies and in both immediate and delayed memory. In addition, analyses of serial position indicated pronounced primary and recency effects in immediate recall of words and a primacy effect in delayed final recall. These findings are discussed in relation to current conceptualizations of memory and language processing in deaf students.
Collapse
|
7
|
Hanson VL. Recall of order information by deaf signers: phonetic coding in temporal order recall. Mem Cognit 1990; 18:604-10. [PMID: 2266862 DOI: 10.3758/bf03197103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
To examine the claim that phonetic coding plays a special role in temporal order recall, deaf and hearing college students were tested on their recall of temporal and spatial order information at two delay intervals. The deaf subjects were all native signers of American Sign Language. The results indicated that both the deaf and hearing subjects used phonetic coding in short-term temporal recall, and visual coding in spatial recall. There was no evidence of manual or visual coding among either the hearing or the deaf subjects in the temporal order recall task. The use of phonetic coding for temporal recall is consistent with the hypothesis that recall of temporal order information is facilitated by a phonetic code.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- V L Hanson
- IBM Researach Division, Thomas J. Watson Research Center, Yorktown Heights, New York 10598
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Campbell R, Wright H. Deafness and immediate memory for pictures: dissociations between "inner speech" and the "inner ear"? J Exp Child Psychol 1990; 50:259-86. [PMID: 2258691 DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90042-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Born-deaf, orally trained youngsters were examined on two tasks of immediate memory for pictures of objects. The aim was to investigate the extent of speech coding for pictures in immediate memory in a developmental context. The deaf, unlike young hearing children, did not use picture-name rhyme spontaneously as a cue to recall in a paired association task. Nevertheless, they were just as sensitive as reading age-matched hearing controls to spoken word length in recalling pictures by name. This might mean that the deaf use articulatory rehearsal in some immediate memory tasks, but this leads to a paradoxical conclusion. What could "inner speech" in the deaf be for, if it fails to affect their "inner ear" by inducing rhyme sensitivity in the paired associate task? This paradox is discussed in relation to distinctions between covert and overt use of memory cues in the paired recall task and to possible sources of the word length effect in young hearing (8-9 years old) and deaf subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Campbell
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, London, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Hanson VL, Lichtenstein EH. Short-term memory coding by deaf signers: the primary language coding hypothesis reconsidered. Cogn Psychol 1990; 22:211-24. [PMID: 2331856 DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(90)90016-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Shand (Cognitive Psychology, 1982, 14, 1-12) hypothesized that strong reliance on a phonetic code by hearing individuals in short-term memory situations reflects their primary language experience. As support for this proposal, Shand reported an experiment in which deaf signers' recall of lists of printed English words was poorer when the American Sign Language translations of those words were structurally similar than when they were structurally unrelated. He interpreted this result as evidence that the deaf subjects were recoding the printed words into sign, reflecting their primary language experience. This primary language interpretation is challenged in the present article first by an experiment in which a group of hearing subjects showed a similar recall pattern on Shand's lists of words, and second by a review of the literature on short-term memory studies with deaf subjects. The literature survey reveals that whether or not deaf signers recode into sign depends on a variety of task and subject factors, and that, contrary to the primary language hypothesis, deaf signers may recode into a phonetic code in short-term recall.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- V L Hanson
- IBM Research Division, Thomas J. Watson Research Center, Yorktown Heights, New York
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Spencer S, Dale JA, Klions HL. Deaf versus Hearing Subjects’ Recall of Words on a Distraction Task as a Function of the Signability of the Words. Percept Mot Skills 1989; 69:1043-7. [PMID: 2608384 DOI: 10.1177/00315125890693-161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Subjects, including 17 deaf and 10 hearing students in Grades 11 and 12, completed a test for memory of lists of 6 words (presented visually for 10 sec.). Subjects recalled the words in writing after a distracting task of adding pairs of digits for 10 sec. Word lists are categorized as signable with a single sign, compound or combination of signs, or finger-spelling signs only. Hearing subjects recalled significantly more words in each category than did deaf subjects. Deaf subjects recalled significantly more single-signed words than either of the other two categories. Deaf subjects did not recall more compound/combination signed words than words that could only be finger-spelled. Hearing subjects also recalled significantly more single-signed words than either of the other two categories and were not superior in either of the other categories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Spencer
- Department of Psychology, Allegheny College, Meadville, PA 16335
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Spencer S, Dale JA, Klions HL. Deaf versus hearing subjects' recall of words on a distraction task as a function of the signability of the words. Percept Mot Skills 1989. [PMID: 2608384 DOI: 10.2466/pms.1989.69.3.1043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Subjects, including 17 deaf and 10 hearing students in Grades 11 and 12, completed a test for memory of lists of 6 words (presented visually for 10 sec.). Subjects recalled the words in writing after a distracting task of adding pairs of digits for 10 sec. Word lists are categorized as signable with a single sign, compound or combination of signs, or finger-spelling signs only. Hearing subjects recalled significantly more words in each category than did deaf subjects. Deaf subjects recalled significantly more single-signed words than either of the other two categories. Deaf subjects did not recall more compound/combination signed words than words that could only be finger-spelled. Hearing subjects also recalled significantly more single-signed words than either of the other two categories and were not superior in either of the other categories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Spencer
- Department of Psychology, Allegheny College, Meadville, PA 16335
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Manion IG, Bucher B. Generalization of a sign language rehearsal strategy in mentally retarded and hearing deficient children. APPLIED RESEARCH IN MENTAL RETARDATION 1986; 7:133-48. [PMID: 3729380 DOI: 10.1016/0270-3092(86)90001-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
Abstract
Memory deficits are common in mentally retarded individuals who lack verbal skills. Training in manual signing may be of value. In this study we trained a sign language rehearsal strategy so that the strategy would be used with no instructions to perform it; and assessed whether the strategy would transfer across tasks, and how to train for transfer. Subjects were five essentially nonverbal severely mentally retarded children and three essentially nonverbal deaf children. Each was trained to use a sign-rehearsal strategy in one of two memory tasks. Use of the strategy showed marked increases when children were instructed to rehearse, but showed less change when no instructions were given. Generalization to uninstructed trials was then achieved by gradually delaying the instructions. After this training, two of three children trained in a more complex task transferred uninstructed sign-rehearsal to a less complex task, but children trained in the simpler task did not generalize to the complex task.
Collapse
|
13
|
Hanson VL, Liberman IY, Shankweiler D. Linguistic coding by deaf children in relation to beginning reading success. J Exp Child Psychol 1984; 37:378-93. [PMID: 6726116 DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90010-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/21/2023]
Abstract
The coding of printed letters in a task of consonant recall was examined in relation to the level of success of prelingually and profoundly deaf children (median age 8.75 years) in beginning reading. As determined by recall errors, the deaf children who were classified as good readers appeared to use both speech and fingerspelling (manual) codes in short-term retention of printed letters. In contrast, deaf children classified as poor readers did not show influence of either of these linguistically based codes in recall. Thus, the success of deaf children in beginning reading, like that of hearing children, appears to be related to the ability to establish and make use of linguistically recoded representations of the language. Neither group showed evidence of dependence on visual cues for recall.
Collapse
|
14
|
Bonvillian JD. Effects of signability and imagery on word recall of deaf and hearing students. Percept Mot Skills 1983; 56:775-91. [PMID: 6877965 DOI: 10.2466/pms.1983.56.3.775] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
This study examined 40 deaf and 20 hearing students' free recall of visually presented words varied systematically with respect to signability (i.e., words that could be expressed by a single sign) and visual imagery. Half of the deaf subjects had deaf parents, while the other half had hearing parents. For deaf students, recall was better for words that had sign-language equivalents and high-imagery values. For the hearing students, recall was better for words with high-imagery values, but there was no effect of signability. Over-all, the hearing students recalled significantly more words than the deaf students in both immediate and delayed free-recall conditions. In immediate recall, deaf students with deaf parents reported using a sign-language coding strategy more frequently and recalled more words correctly than deaf students with hearing parents. Serial-position curves indicated several differences in patterns of recall among the groups. These results underline the importance of sign language in the memory and recall of deaf persons.
Collapse
|
15
|
|
16
|
Luftig RL. Increasing probability of sign language learning by severely mentally retarded individuals: a discussion of learner, sign production, and linguistic variables. APPLIED RESEARCH IN MENTAL RETARDATION 1982; 3:81-97. [PMID: 6213202 DOI: 10.1016/0270-3092(82)90060-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
A pervasive problem for educators of the severely mentally retarded is language training. In spite of extensive oral language training, many severely mentally retarded individuals never acquire functional oral language. Many of these clients, however, are able to acquire sign language communication skills. The present article discusses sign language learning in terms of learner attributes, production variables in sign, and the referential concepts which the signs represent. More specifically, it is hypothesized that by taking into account variables such as sign translucency, referential concreteness, learning readiness, and by externally organizing the signs to be learned along visual continuums, the probability of sign learning by severely mentally retarded individuals can be increased.
Collapse
|
17
|
|
18
|
Liben LS. Free recall by deaf and hearing children: semantic clustering and recall in trained and untrained groups. J Exp Child Psychol 1979; 27:105-19. [PMID: 458366 DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(79)90063-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
|
19
|
|