1
|
Blinch J, Nourollahimoghadam M, Trovinger C, Roncesvalles MN, Hart MA, Chua R. Limb-Target Control Increases With Effective Index of Difficulty. J Mot Behav 2025:1-14. [PMID: 39912872 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2025.2461085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2024] [Revised: 01/10/2025] [Accepted: 01/24/2025] [Indexed: 02/07/2025]
Abstract
Researchers have investigated the sensorimotor mechanisms that result in Fitts' law. One approach has been to analyse movement trajectories during Fitts' tasks to reveal the processes that occur during movement preparation and execution. We used trajectory analysis in the current study to investigate how limb-target control contributed to Fitts' law during the transition from ballistic movements to movements with online control. Twenty-five participants made discrete reaching movements in seven conditions with indexes of difficulty that ranged from one to seven. There were strong linear relationships between index of difficulty, effective index of difficulty and movement time. Trajectory analysis suggested that the easiest condition had movements that were mostly ballistic. There was enough time for visual-based online corrections, but the condition was probably too easy to require limb-target control. Trajectory analysis also suggested that there was an increased reliance on limb-target control as the index of difficulty increased. In conclusion, there was a strong linear relationship between effective index of difficulty and movement time even with conditions that ranged from mostly ballistic to a high degree of limb-target control. We suggest that there is a direct relationship between effective index of difficulty and degree of limb-target control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jarrod Blinch
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
| | - Maryam Nourollahimoghadam
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
- Department of Kinesiology and Educational Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA, USA
| | - Coby Trovinger
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
| | | | - Melanie A Hart
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
- School of Health Professions, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
| | - Romeo Chua
- School of Kinesiology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Owen R, Wakefield CJ, Roberts JW. Online corrections can occur within movement imagery: An investigation of the motor-cognitive model. Hum Mov Sci 2024; 95:103222. [PMID: 38696913 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2024.103222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2024] [Revised: 04/04/2024] [Accepted: 04/24/2024] [Indexed: 05/04/2024]
Abstract
The motor-cognitive model proposes that movement imagery additionally requires conscious monitoring owing to an absence of veridical online sensory feedback. Therefore, it is predicted that there would be a comparatively limited ability for individuals to update or correct movement imagery as they could within execution. To investigate, participants executed and imagined target-directed aiming movements featuring either an unexpected target perturbation (Exp. 1) or removal of visual sensory feedback (Exp. 2). The results of both experiments indicated that the time-course of executed and imagined movements was equally influenced by each of these online visual manipulations. Thus, contrary to some of the tenets of the motor-cognitive model, movement imagery holds the capacity to interpolate online corrections despite the absence of veridical sensory feedback. The further theoretical implications of these findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robin Owen
- Liverpool Hope University, School of Health and Sport Sciences, Hope Park, Liverpool L16 9JD, UK
| | - Caroline J Wakefield
- Liverpool Hope University, School of Health and Sport Sciences, Hope Park, Liverpool L16 9JD, UK
| | - James W Roberts
- Liverpool John Moores University, Research Institute of Sport & Exercise Sciences (RISES), Brain & Behaviour Research Group, Tom Reilly Building, Byrom Street, Liverpool L3 5AF, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Roberts JW, Burkitt JJ, Elliott D. The type 1 submovement conundrum: an investigation into the function of velocity zero-crossings within two-component aiming movements. Exp Brain Res 2024:10.1007/s00221-024-06784-0. [PMID: 38329516 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-024-06784-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2023] [Accepted: 01/15/2024] [Indexed: 02/09/2024]
Abstract
In rapid manual aiming, traditional wisdom would have it that two components manifest from feedback-based processes, where error accumulated within the primary submovement can be corrected within the secondary submovement courtesy of online sensory feedback. In some aiming contexts, there are more type 1 submovements (overshooting) compared to types 2 and 3 submovements (undershooting), particularly for more rapid movements. These particular submovements have also been attributed to a mechanical artefact involving movement termination and stabilisation. Hence, the goal of our study was to more closely examine the function of type 1 submovements by revisiting some of our previous datasets. We categorised these submovements according to whether the secondary submovement moved the limb closer (functional), or not (non-functional), to the target. Overall, there were both functional and non-functional submovements with a significantly higher proportion for the former. The displacement at the primary and secondary submovements, and negative velocity peak were significantly greater in the functional compared to non-functional. The influence of submovement type on other movement characteristics, including movement time, was somewhat less clear. These findings indicate that the majority of type 1 submovements are related to intended feedforward- and/or feedback-based processes, although there are a portion that can be attributed an indirect manifestation of a mechanical artefact. As a result, we suggest that submovements should be further categorised by their error-reducing function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James W Roberts
- Brain and Behaviour Research Group, Research Institute of Sport and Exercise Sciences (RISES), Liverpool John Moores University, Tom Reilly Building, Byrom Street, Liverpool, L3 5AF, UK.
- School of Health Sciences, Psychology, Action and Learning of Movement (PALM) Laboratory, Liverpool Hope University, Hope Park, Liverpool, L16 9JD, UK.
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, ON, L8S 4K1, Canada.
| | - James J Burkitt
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, ON, L8S 4K1, Canada
| | - Digby Elliott
- Brain and Behaviour Research Group, Research Institute of Sport and Exercise Sciences (RISES), Liverpool John Moores University, Tom Reilly Building, Byrom Street, Liverpool, L3 5AF, UK
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, ON, L8S 4K1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Roberts JW, Maiden J, Bennett SJ. Impact of simulated target blur on the preparation and execution of aiming movements. VISUAL COGNITION 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2140730] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- James W. Roberts
- Liverpool Hope University, Psychology, Action and Learning of Movement (PALM) Laboratory, School of Health Sciences, Liverpool, UK
| | - James Maiden
- Liverpool Hope University, Psychology, Action and Learning of Movement (PALM) Laboratory, School of Health Sciences, Liverpool, UK
| | - Simon J. Bennett
- Liverpool John Moores University, Research Institute of Sport & Exercise Sciences, Brain & Behaviour Research Group, Liverpool, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Loria T, Tan M, de Grosbois J, Huang A, Thaut MH. Temporospatial Alterations in Upper-Limb and Mallet Control Underlie Motor Learning in Marimba Performance. Front Psychol 2022; 13:834869. [PMID: 35222211 PMCID: PMC8866314 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.834869] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2021] [Accepted: 01/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Sound-producing movements in percussion performance require a high degree of fine motor control. However, there remains a relatively limited empirical understanding of how performance level abilities develop in percussion performance in general, and marimba performance specifically. To address this issue, nine percussionists performed individualised excerpts on marimba within three testing sessions spaced 29 days apart to assess early, intermediate, and late stages of motor learning. Motor learning was quantified via analyses of both the temporal control of mallet movements, and the spatial variability of upper-limb movements. The results showed that temporal control of mallet movements was greater in the intermediate compared to the early learning session, with no significant additional improvements revealed in the late learning session. In addition, spatial variability in the left and right elbows decreased within the intermediate compared to the early learning session. The results suggest that temporal control of mallet movements may be driven by reductions in spatial variability of elbow movements specifically. As a result, this study provides novel evidence for kinematic mechanisms underlying motor learning in percussion which can be applied towards enhancing musical training.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tristan Loria
- Music and Health Research Collaboratory (MaHRC), Faculty of Music, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Melissa Tan
- Music and Health Research Collaboratory (MaHRC), Faculty of Music, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - John de Grosbois
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Aiyun Huang
- Faculty of Music, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Michael H Thaut
- Music and Health Research Collaboratory (MaHRC), Faculty of Music, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Elliott D, Lyons J, Hayes SJ, Burkitt JJ, Hansen S, Grierson LEM, Foster NC, Roberts JW, Bennett SJ. The multiple process model of goal-directed aiming/reaching: insights on limb control from various special populations. Exp Brain Res 2020; 238:2685-2699. [PMID: 33079207 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05952-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2019] [Accepted: 10/08/2020] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
Several years ago, our research group forwarded a model of goal-directed reaching and aiming that describes the processes involved in the optimization of speed, accuracy, and energy expenditure Elliott et al. (Psychol Bull 136:1023-1044, 2010). One of the main features of the model is the distinction between early impulse control, which is based on a comparison of expected to perceived sensory consequences, and late limb-target control that involves a spatial comparison of limb and target position. Our model also emphasizes the importance of strategic behaviors that limit the opportunity for worst-case or inefficient outcomes. In the 2010 paper, we included a section on how our model can be used to understand atypical aiming/reaching movements in a number of special populations. In light of a recent empirical and theoretical update of our model Elliott et al. (Neurosci Biobehav Rev 72:95-110, 2017), here we consider contemporary motor control work involving typical aging, Down syndrome, autism spectrum disorder, and tetraplegia with tendon-transfer surgery. We outline how atypical limb control can be viewed within the context of the multiple-process model of goal-directed reaching and aiming, and discuss the underlying perceptual-motor impairment that results in the adaptive solution developed by the specific group.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Digby Elliott
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, L8S 4K1, Canada.
- Brain and Behaviour Laboratory, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK.
| | - James Lyons
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, L8S 4K1, Canada
| | - Spencer J Hayes
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, University College London, London, UK
| | | | - Steve Hansen
- School of Physical and Health Education, Nipissing University, North Bay, ON, Canada
| | - Lawrence E M Grierson
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, L8S 4K1, Canada
- Department of Family Medicine, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
| | - Nathan C Foster
- Cognition, Motion and Neuroscience Unit, Fondazione Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
| | - James W Roberts
- Brain and Behaviour Laboratory, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| | - Simon J Bennett
- Brain and Behaviour Laboratory, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Abstract
Reaching trajectories have provided a unique tool to observe changes in internal cognitive decisions. Furthermore, technological advances have made devices for measuring reach movements more accessible and researchers have recognized that various populations including children, elderly populations, and non-human primates can easily execute simple movements as responses. As a result, devices such as a three-dimensional (3D) reach tracker, a stylus, or a computer-mouse have been increasingly utilized to study cognitive processes. However, although the specific type of tracking device that a researcher uses may impact behavior due to the constraints it places on movements, most researchers in these fields are unaware of this potential issue. Here, we examined the potential behavioral impact of using each of these three devices. To induce re-directed movements that mimic the movements that often occur following changes in cognitive states, we used a double-step task in which displacement of an initial target location requires participants to quickly re-direct their movement. We found that reach movement parameters were largely comparable across the three devices. However, hand movements measured by a 3D reach tracker showed earlier reach initiation latencies (relative to stylus movements) and more curved movement trajectories (relative to both mouse and stylus movements). Reach movements were also re-directed following target displacement more rapidly. Thus, 3D reach trackers may be ideal for observing fast, subtle changes in internal decision-making processes compared to other devices. Taken together, this study provides a useful reference for comparing and implementing reaching studies to examine human cognition.
Collapse
|
8
|
Ten Dam E, Helder HM, van der Laan BFAM, Feijen RA, Korsten-Meijer AGW. The effect of three-dimensional visualisation on performance in endoscopic sinus surgery: A clinical training study using surgical navigation for movement analysis in a randomised crossover design. Clin Otolaryngol 2020; 45:211-220. [PMID: 31846558 PMCID: PMC7027512 DOI: 10.1111/coa.13494] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2019] [Revised: 11/06/2019] [Accepted: 12/12/2019] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Endoscopic imaging techniques and endoscopic endonasal surgery (EES) expertise have evolved rapidly. Only few studies have assessed the effect of three-dimensional (3D) endoscopy on endoscopic sinus surgery (ESS). The present study aimed to objectively and subjectively assess the additional value of 3D high-definition (HD) endoscopy in ESS. DESIGN A randomized crossover study of endoscopic surgery performance, using five ESS tasks of varying complexity, performed on Thiel embalmed human specimens. SETTING Simulated surgical environment. PARTICIPANTS Thirty participants, inexperienced in ESS. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Performance was assessed using video imaging, surgical navigation and questionnaires. Main outcome measures were as follows: efficiency (defined by time to task completion), distance covered inside the nose, average velocity towards target, accuracy (measured by error rate), and subjective assessment of endoscope characteristics. RESULTS During ESS tasks, both efficiency and accuracy did not differ significantly between 2D HD and 3D HD endoscopy. Subjectively, imaging characteristics of the 3D HD endoscope were rated significantly better. CONCLUSIONS ESS performance of inexperienced participants was not significantly improved by the use of 3D HD endoscopy during ESS tasks, although imaging characteristics of the 3D HD endoscope were rated significantly better. Surgical field characteristics and surgical techniques are likely to influence any additional value of 3D HD endoscopy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ellen Ten Dam
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology/Head and Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.,Institute for Drug Exploration, Graduate School of Medical Sciences, Groningen University, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Herman M Helder
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology/Head and Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.,Cancer Research Center Groningen, Graduate School of Medical Sciences, Groningen University, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Bernard F A M van der Laan
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology/Head and Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Robert A Feijen
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology/Head and Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Astrid G W Korsten-Meijer
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology/Head and Neck Surgery, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Which Measures of Online Control Are Least Sensitive to Offline Processes? Motor Control 2018; 22:358-376. [PMID: 29486667 DOI: 10.1123/mc.2017-0014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
A major challenge to the measurement of online control is the contamination by offline, planning-based processes. The current study examined the sensitivity of four measures of online control to offline changes in reaching performance induced by prism adaptation and terminal feedback. These measures included the squared Z scores (Z2) of correlations of limb position at 75% movement time versus movement end, variable error, time after peak velocity, and a frequency-domain analysis (pPower). The results indicated that variable error and time after peak velocity were sensitive to the prism adaptation. Furthermore, only the Z2 values were biased by the terminal feedback. Ultimately, the current study has demonstrated the sensitivity of limb kinematic measures to offline control processes and that pPower analyses may yield the most suitable measure of online control.
Collapse
|
10
|
Elliott D, Lyons J, Hayes SJ, Burkitt JJ, Roberts JW, Grierson LE, Hansen S, Bennett SJ. The multiple process model of goal-directed reaching revisited. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 72:95-110. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2016] [Revised: 11/18/2016] [Accepted: 11/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
11
|
Hondzinski JM, Soebbing CM, French AE, Winges SA. Different damping responses explain vertical endpoint error differences between visual conditions. Exp Brain Res 2016; 234:1575-87. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4546-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2015] [Accepted: 12/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
12
|
Abstract
The utilization of sensory information during activities of daily living is ubiquitous both prior to and during movements (i.e., related to planning and online control, respectively). Because of the overlapping nature of online corrective processes, the quantification of feedback utilization has proven difficult. In the present study, we primarily sought to evaluate the utility of a novel analysis in the frequency domain for identifying visuomotor feedback utilization (i.e., online control). A second goal was to compare the sensitivity of the frequency analysis to that of currently utilized measures of online control. Participants completed reaching movements to targets located 27, 30, and 33 cm from a start position. During these reaches, vision of the environment was either provided or withheld. Performance was assessed across contemporary measures of online control. For the novel frequency analysis presented in this study, the acceleration profiles of reaching movements were detrended with a 5th-order polynomial fit, and the proportional power spectra were computed from the residuals of these fits. The results indicated that the use of visual feedback during reaching movements increased the contribution of the 4.68-Hz frequency to the residuals of the acceleration profiles. Comparisons across all measures of online control showed that the most sensitive measure was the squared Fisher transform of the correlation between the positions at 75 % and 100 % of the movement time. However, because such correlational measures can be contaminated by offline control processes, the frequency-domain analysis proposed herein represents a viable and promising alternative to detect changes in online feedback utilization.
Collapse
|
13
|
Kuang S, Gail A. When adaptive control fails: Slow recovery of reduced rapid online control during reaching under reversed vision. Vision Res 2014; 110:155-65. [PMID: 25218421 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2014] [Revised: 08/21/2014] [Accepted: 08/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies have shown that short-term exposure to mirror-reversed visual feedback suppresses rapid online control (ROC) of arm movements in response to a sudden target displacement. Here we tested if the reduced ROC under reversed vision can be observed for natural reaches without target perturbations, i.e. without corrective movements that are driven by visual input perturbation. Second, we ask if such ROC reduction generalizes to movement phases without visual feedback of the hand. Subjects were instructed to perform simple reach movements towards a stationary target position either under normal or physically reversed vision of the hand during the late movement phase. We quantified time-resolved ROC via a coefficient of determination of the reach trajectories over the full course of the movement. As for other measures in previous studies, we found that our perturbation-independent ROC was reduced within a few trials after exposure to reversed visual feedback. The reduced ROC was restricted to late movement phases, and was not observed in early movement phases. We further asked if subjects would be able to re-gain ROC with prolonged exposure to the reversed visual input. ROC gradually and incompletely increased over the course of 400 exposure trials, affecting both early and late movement phases. Our results show that under reversed vision ROC is reduced even for perturbation-independent natural reaches aiming at stationary targets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shenbing Kuang
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China; German Primate Center, Göttingen, Germany; Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Alexander Gail
- German Primate Center, Göttingen, Germany; Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Göttingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Boisgontier MP, Nougier V. Ageing of internal models: from a continuous to an intermittent proprioceptive control of movement. AGE (DORDRECHT, NETHERLANDS) 2013; 35:1339-1355. [PMID: 22639177 PMCID: PMC3705127 DOI: 10.1007/s11357-012-9436-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2012] [Accepted: 05/16/2012] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
To control the sensory-motor system, internal models mimic the transformations between motor commands and sensory signals. The present study proposed to assess the effects of physiological adult ageing on the proprioceptive control of movement and the related internal models. To this aim, one group of young adults and one group of older adults performed an ankle contralateral concurrent matching task in two speed conditions (self-selected and fast). Error, temporal and kinematic variables were used to assess the matching performance. The results demonstrated that older adults used a different mode of control as compared to the young adults and suggested that the internal models of proprioceptive control were altered with ageing. Behavioural expressions of these alterations were dependent upon the considered condition of speed. In the self-selected speed condition, this alteration was expressed through an increased number of corrective sub-movements in older adults as compared to their young peers. This strategy enabled them to reach a level of end-point performance comparable to the young adults' performance. In the fast speed condition, older adults were no more able to compensate for their impaired internal models through additional corrective sub-movements and therefore decreased their proprioceptive control performance. These results provided the basis for a model of proprioceptive control of movement integrating the internal models theory and the continuous and intermittent modes of control. This study also suggested that motor control was affected by the frailty syndrome, i.e. a decreased resistance to stressors, which characterises older adults.
Collapse
|
15
|
Roberts J, Burkitt JJ, Willemse B, Ludzki A, Lyons J, Elliott D, Grierson LEM. The influence of target context and early and late vision on goal-directed reaching. Exp Brain Res 2013; 229:525-32. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3614-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2012] [Accepted: 06/10/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
16
|
Burkitt JJ, Grierson LEM, Staite V, Elliott D, Lyons J. The Impact of Prior Knowledge about Visual Feedback on Motor Performance and Learning. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013. [DOI: 10.4236/ape.2013.31001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
17
|
Robinson MA, Hayes SJ, Bennett SJ, Barton GJ, Elliott D. Sensory-motor equivalence: manual aiming in C6 tetraplegics following musculotendinous transfer surgery at the elbow. Exp Brain Res 2010; 206:81-91. [PMID: 20809244 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-010-2400-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2010] [Accepted: 08/18/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Cervical spinal lesions at C6 result in paralysis of the triceps brachii while leaving deltoid and elbow flexor function intact. We examined the spatial-temporal characteristics of goal-directed aiming movements performed by C6 tetraplegics who had undergone musculotendinous transfer surgery in which the posterior deltoid replaces the triceps as the elbow extensor. On some trials, liquid crystal goggles were used to eliminate vision of the limb and target upon movement initiation. Although tetraplegic participants achieved the same degree of movement accuracy/consistency as control participants, their movement times were longer regardless of whether the movements were made away from (elbow extension) or towards the body (elbow flexion). Longer movement times were related to lower peak velocities, and not the symmetry of the aiming profiles. The tetraplegic participants were no more dependent on visual feedback for limb regulation than control participants. Although the characteristics of the movement trajectories were surprisingly similar, in both vision conditions, tetraplegics required more real and proportional time to reduce spatial variability in the limb's trajectory for elbow extensions. Our results indicate that the sensorimotor system is adaptable and that the representations governing limb control are not muscle specific.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark A Robinson
- Research Institute for Sport and Exercise Sciences, Faculty of Science, Liverpool John Moores University, Tom Reilly Building, Byrom Street, L3 3AF, Liverpool, UK
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|