1
|
Wiegand I, van Pouderoijen M, Oosterman JM, Deckers K, Horstmann G. Contributions of distractor dwelling, skipping, and revisiting to age differences in visual search. Sci Rep 2025; 15:1801. [PMID: 39805864 PMCID: PMC11730621 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-83532-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2024] [Accepted: 12/16/2024] [Indexed: 01/16/2025] Open
Abstract
Visual search becomes slower with aging, particularly when targets are difficult to discriminate from distractors. Multiple distractor rejection processes may contribute independently to slower search times: dwelling on, skipping of, and revisiting of distractors, measurable by eye-tracking. The present study investigated how age affects each of the distractor rejection processes, and how these contribute to the final search times in difficult (inefficient) visual search. In a sample of Dutch healthy adults (19-85 years), we measured reaction times and eye-movements during a target present/absent visual search task, with varying target-distractor similarity and visual set size. We found that older age was associated with longer dwelling and more revisiting of distractors, while skipping was unaffected by age. This suggests that increased processing time and reduced visuo-spatial memory for visited distractor locations contribute to age-related decline in visual search. Furthermore, independently of age, dwelling and revisiting contributed stronger to search times than skipping of distractors. In conclusion, under conditions of poor guidance, dwelling and revisiting have a major contribution to search times and age-related slowing in difficult visual search, while skipping is largely negligible.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iris Wiegand
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Postbus 9102, 6500 HC, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | - Mariska van Pouderoijen
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Postbus 9102, 6500 HC, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Alzheimer Centrum Limburg, Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNs), Maastricht University, 6200 MD, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Joukje M Oosterman
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Postbus 9102, 6500 HC, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Kay Deckers
- Alzheimer Centrum Limburg, Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNs), Maastricht University, 6200 MD, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Gernot Horstmann
- Department of Psychology, Bielefeld University, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Ben Itzhak N, Stijnen L, Kostkova K, Laenen A, Jansen B, Ortibus E. The effectiveness of an individualised and adaptive game-based rehabilitation, iVision, on visual perception in cerebral visual impairment: A triple-blind randomised controlled trial. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2024; 156:104899. [PMID: 39719804 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2024.104899] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2024] [Revised: 10/30/2024] [Accepted: 12/13/2024] [Indexed: 12/26/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Cerebral visual impairment (CVI) can negatively affect a child's functioning, emphasising the need for interventions to improve visual perception (VP), potentially translating into improved health-related quality of life (HRQOL). AIMS Assessing the effectiveness of an adaptive individualised game-based rehabilitation, iVision, on VP, visual function, functional vision, and HRQOL. METHODS AND PROCEDURES Seventy-three children with CVI (3-12 performance age) were randomised into the adaptive individualised or the non-adaptive non-individualised group (3 sessions/week; 12 weeks). Primary outcome was change score (post-intervention - pre-intervention) of the lowest VP dimension. Key secondary outcomes included change score (post-intervention - pre-intervention) of visual function (reaction time to fixation in a preferential looking eye-tracking paradigm), functional vision (success rate in the adapted virtual toy box paradigm; total Flemish CVI questionnaire score), HRQOL (total scale score of the paediatric quality of life inventory 4.0 child self-report), and the lowest VP dimension change score (short-term follow-up - pre-intervention). OUTCOMES AND RESULTS Both groups significantly improved on the primary outcome, maintaining at short-term. Between-group differences were not significant. No significant effect was found for other key secondary outcomes. Exploratory analyses revealed VP dimension improvements and clinically meaningful HRQOL improvements. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Although children with CVI improved their VP and to some extent HRQOL, no differences were found between the groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N Ben Itzhak
- Department of Development and Regeneration, University of Leuven (KU Leuven), Leuven, Belgium; KU Leuven Child and Youth Institute (L-C&Y), Leuven, Belgium
| | - L Stijnen
- Department of Development and Regeneration, University of Leuven (KU Leuven), Leuven, Belgium
| | - K Kostkova
- Department of Electronics and Informatics (ETRO), Vrije Universiteit Brussel (VUB), Brussels, Belgium; imec, Leuven, Belgium
| | - A Laenen
- Leuven Biostatistics and Statistical Bioinformatics Centre (L-BioStat), University of Leuven (KU Leuven), Leuven, Belgium
| | - B Jansen
- Department of Electronics and Informatics (ETRO), Vrije Universiteit Brussel (VUB), Brussels, Belgium; imec, Leuven, Belgium
| | - E Ortibus
- Department of Development and Regeneration, University of Leuven (KU Leuven), Leuven, Belgium; KU Leuven Child and Youth Institute (L-C&Y), Leuven, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Nikolaev AR, Meghanathan RN, van Leeuwen C. Refixation behavior in naturalistic viewing: Methods, mechanisms, and neural correlates. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024:10.3758/s13414-023-02836-9. [PMID: 38169029 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02836-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/17/2023] [Indexed: 01/05/2024]
Abstract
When freely viewing a scene, the eyes often return to previously visited locations. By tracking eye movements and coregistering eye movements and EEG, such refixations are shown to have multiple roles: repairing insufficient encoding from precursor fixations, supporting ongoing viewing by resampling relevant locations prioritized by precursor fixations, and aiding the construction of memory representations. All these functions of refixation behavior are understood to be underpinned by three oculomotor and cognitive systems and their associated brain structures. First, immediate saccade planning prior to refixations involves attentional selection of candidate locations to revisit. This process is likely supported by the dorsal attentional network. Second, visual working memory, involved in maintaining task-related information, is likely supported by the visual cortex. Third, higher-order relevance of scene locations, which depends on general knowledge and understanding of scene meaning, is likely supported by the hippocampal memory system. Working together, these structures bring about viewing behavior that balances exploring previously unvisited areas of a scene with exploiting visited areas through refixations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrey R Nikolaev
- Department of Psychology, Lund University, Box 213, 22100, Lund, Sweden.
- Brain & Cognition Research Unit, KU Leuven-University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.
| | | | - Cees van Leeuwen
- Brain & Cognition Research Unit, KU Leuven-University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
- Center for Cognitive Science, Rheinland-Pfälzische Technische Universität Kaiserslautern-Landau, Kaiserslautern, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Becker SI, Grubert A, Horstmann G, Ansorge U. Which processes dominate visual search: Bottom-up feature contrast, top-down tuning or trial history? Cognition 2023; 236:105420. [PMID: 36905828 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105420] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2022] [Revised: 02/21/2023] [Accepted: 02/22/2023] [Indexed: 03/11/2023]
Abstract
Previous research has identified three mechanisms that guide visual attention: bottom-up feature contrasts, top-down tuning, and the trial history (e.g., priming effects). However, only few studies have simultaneously examined all three mechanisms. Hence, it is currently unclear how they interact or which mechanisms dominate over others. With respect to local feature contrasts, it has been claimed that a pop-out target can only be selected immediately in dense displays when the target has a high local feature contrast, but not when the displays are sparse, which leads to an inverse set-size effect. The present study critically evaluated this view by systematically varying local feature contrasts (i.e., set size), top-down knowledge, and the trial history in pop-out search. We used eye tracking to distinguish between early selection and later identification-related processes. The results revealed that early visual selection was mainly dominated by top-down knowledge and the trial history: When attention was biased to the target feature, either by valid pre-cueing (top-down) or automatic priming, the target could be localised immediately, regardless of display density. Bottom-up feature contrasts only modulated selection when the target was unknown and attention was biased to the non-targets. We also replicated the often-reported finding of reliable feature contrast effects in the mean RTs, but showed that these were due to later, target identification processes (e.g., in the target dwell times). Thus, contrary to the prevalent view, bottom-up feature contrasts in dense displays do not seem to directly guide attention, but only facilitate nontarget rejection, probably by facilitating nontarget grouping.
Collapse
|
5
|
Mirror blindness: Our failure to recognize the target in search for mirror-reversed shapes. Atten Percept Psychophys 2023; 85:418-437. [PMID: 36653521 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-022-02641-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/13/2022] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
It is well known that visual search for a mirror target (i.e., a horizontally flipped item) is more difficult than search for other-oriented items (e.g., vertically flipped items). Previous studies have typically attributed costs of mirror search to early, attention-guiding processes but could not rule out contributions from later processes. In the present study we used eye tracking to distinguish between early, attention-guiding processes and later target identification processes. The results of four experiments revealed a marked human weakness in identifying mirror targets: Observers appear to frequently fail to classify a mirror target as a target on first fixation and to continue with search even after having directly looked at the target. Awareness measures corroborated that the location of a mirror target could not be reported above chance level after it had been fixated once. This mirror blindness effect explained a large proportion (45-87%) of the overall costs of mirror search, suggesting that part of the difficulties with mirror search are rooted in later, object identification processes (not attentional guidance). Mirror blindness was significantly reduced but not completely eliminated when both the target and non-targets were held constant, which shows that perfect top-down knowledge can reduce mirror blindness, without completely eliminating it. The finding that non-target certainty reduced mirror blindness suggests that object identification is not solely achieved by comparing a selected item to a target template. These results demonstrate that templates that guide search toward targets are not identical to the templates used to conclusively identify those targets.
Collapse
|
6
|
Ernst D, Wolfe JM. How fixation durations are affected by search difficulty manipulations. VISUAL COGNITION 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2063465] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Ernst
- Brigham & Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Jeremy M. Wolfe
- Brigham & Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Schmidt S, Zlatkin-Troitschanskaia O, Roeper J, Klose V, Weber M, Bültmann AK, Brückner S. Undergraduate Students' Critical Online Reasoning-Process Mining Analysis. Front Psychol 2020; 11:576273. [PMID: 33424686 PMCID: PMC7793971 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.576273] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2020] [Accepted: 11/02/2020] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
To successfully learn using open Internet resources, students must be able to critically search, evaluate and select online information, and verify sources. Defined as critical online reasoning (COR), this construct is operationalized on two levels in our study: (1) the student level using the newly developed Critical Online Reasoning Assessment (CORA), and (2) the online information processing level using event log data, including gaze durations and fixations. The written responses of 32 students for one CORA task were scored by three independent raters. The resulting score was operationalized as "task performance," whereas the gaze fixations and durations were defined as indicators of "process performance." Following a person-oriented approach, we conducted a process mining (PM) analysis, as well as a latent class analysis (LCA) to test whether-following the dual-process theory-the undergraduates could be distinguished into two groups based on both their process and task performance. Using PM, the process performance of all 32 students was visualized and compared, indicating two distinct response process patterns. One group of students (11), defined as "strategic information processers," processed online information more comprehensively, as well as more efficiently, which was also reflected in their higher task scores. In contrast, the distributions of the process performance variables for the other group (21), defined as "avoidance information processers," indicated a poorer process performance, which was also reflected in their lower task scores. In the LCA, where two student groups were empirically distinguished by combining the process performance indicators and the task score as a joint discriminant criterion, we confirmed these two COR profiles, which were reflected in high vs. low process and task performances. The estimated parameters indicated that high-performing students were significantly more efficient at conducting strategic information processing, as reflected in their higher process performance. These findings are so far based on quantitative analyses using event log data. To enable a more differentiated analysis of students' visual attention dynamics, more in-depth qualitative research of the identified student profiles in terms of COR will be required.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susanne Schmidt
- Department of Business and Economics Education, Johannes Gutenberg University, Mainz, Germany
| | | | - Jochen Roeper
- Department of Neurophysiology, University Hospital of the Goethe University, Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Verena Klose
- Department of Neurophysiology, University Hospital of the Goethe University, Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Maruschka Weber
- Department of Neurophysiology, University Hospital of the Goethe University, Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Ann-Kathrin Bültmann
- Department of Business and Economics Education, Johannes Gutenberg University, Mainz, Germany
| | - Sebastian Brückner
- Department of Business and Economics Education, Johannes Gutenberg University, Mainz, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Wolfe JM. Major issues in the study of visual search: Part 2 of "40 Years of Feature Integration: Special Issue in Memory of Anne Treisman". Atten Percept Psychophys 2020; 82:383-393. [PMID: 32291612 PMCID: PMC7250731 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-020-02022-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jeremy M Wolfe
- Ophthalmology & Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
- Visual Attention Lab, Department of Surgery, Brigham & Women's Hospital, 65 Landsdowne St, 4th Floor, Cambridge, MA, 02139, USA.
| |
Collapse
|