1
|
Pontecorvo C, Fasulo A, Sterponi L. Mutual Apprentices: The Making of Parenthood and Childhood in Family Dinner Conversations. Hum Dev 2001. [DOI: 10.1159/000046155] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
|
24 |
105 |
2
|
|
|
15 |
83 |
3
|
Pontecorvo C, Fasulo A. Planning a Typical Italian Meal: A Family Reflection on Culture. CULTURE & PSYCHOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/1354067x9953004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The paper addresses the issue of cultural descriptions as they are perceived and used within mundane conversation. We analyze a discussion of an Italian family about a future formal occasion (a party) in a foreign country (Austria), with foreign participants, in which they shall produce a typically Italian meal. The analysis shows how cultural descriptions are both a resource and a constraint when they must orient a practical activity which must be publicly acknowledged for its cultural typicality. Discrepancies are highlighted between cultural descriptions and ordinary practices, but it is also shown how culture (or ‘cultural preferences’) gets produced, at a less explicit level, within discursive practices, through turn-taking filtering, sequential architecture and selection of differentiated addressees. The socializing import of the discursive situation for the younger participants is also discussed, with reference to the relevant conversational devices.
Collapse
|
|
9 |
34 |
4
|
Staiti P, Poltarzewski Z, Alderucci V, Maggio G, Giordano N, Fasulo A. Influence of electrodic properties on water management in a solid polymer electrolyte fuel cell. J APPL ELECTROCHEM 1992. [DOI: 10.1007/bf01092616] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
|
33 |
31 |
5
|
Fantasia V, De Jaegher H, Fasulo A. We can work it out: an enactive look at cooperation. Front Psychol 2014; 5:874. [PMID: 25152745 PMCID: PMC4126490 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00874] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2014] [Accepted: 07/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The past years have seen an increasing debate on cooperation and its unique human character. Philosophers and psychologists have proposed that cooperative activities are characterized by shared goals to which participants are committed through the ability to understand each other's intentions. Despite its popularity, some serious issues arise with this approach to cooperation. First, one may challenge the assumption that high-level mental processes are necessary for engaging in acting cooperatively. If they are, then how do agents that do not possess such ability (preverbal children, or children with autism who are often claimed to be mind-blind) engage in cooperative exchanges, as the evidence suggests? Secondly, to define cooperation as the result of two de-contextualized minds reading each other's intentions may fail to fully acknowledge the complexity of situated, interactional dynamics and the interplay of variables such as the participants' relational and personal history and experience. In this paper we challenge such accounts of cooperation, calling for an embodied approach that sees cooperation not only as an individual attitude toward the other, but also as a property of interaction processes. Taking an enactive perspective, we argue that cooperation is an intrinsic part of any interaction, and that there can be cooperative interaction before complex communicative abilities are achieved. The issue then is not whether one is able or not to read the other's intentions, but what it takes to participate in joint action. From this basic account, it should be possible to build up more complex forms of cooperation as needed. Addressing the study of cooperation in these terms may enhance our understanding of human social development, and foster our knowledge of different ways of engaging with others, as in the case of autism.
Collapse
|
research-article |
11 |
25 |
6
|
Fantasia V, Fasulo A, Costall A, López B. Changing the game: exploring infants' participation in early play routines. Front Psychol 2014; 5:522. [PMID: 24936192 PMCID: PMC4047965 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00522] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2014] [Accepted: 05/12/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Play has proved to have a central role in children's development, most notably in rule learning (Piaget, 1965; Sutton-Smith, 1979) and negotiation of roles and goals (Garvey, 1974; Bruner et al., 1976). Yet very little research has been done on early play. The present study focuses on early social games, i.e., vocal-kinetic play routines that mothers use to interact with infants from very early on. We explored 3-month-old infants and their mothers performing a routine game first in the usual way, then in two violated conditions: without gestures and without sound. The aim of the study is to investigate infants' participation and expectations in the game and whether this participation is affected by changes in the multimodal format of the game. Infants' facial expressions, gaze, and body movements were coded to measure levels of engagement and affective state across the three conditions. Results showed a significant decrease in Limbs Movements and expressions of Positive Affect, an increase in Gaze Away and in Stunned Expression when the game structure was violated. These results indicate that the violated game conditions were experienced as less engaging, either because of an unexpected break in the established joint routine, or simply because they were weaker versions of the same game. Overall, our results suggest that structured, multimodal play routines may constitute interactional contexts that only work as integrated units of auditory and motor resources, representing early communicative contexts which prepare the ground for later, more complex multimodal interactions, such as verbal exchanges.
Collapse
|
Journal Article |
11 |
24 |
7
|
Kougiali ZG, Fasulo A, Needs A, Van Laar D. Planting the seeds of change: Directionality in the narrative construction of recovery from addiction. Psychol Health 2017; 32:639-664. [PMID: 28276737 DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1293053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The dominant theoretical perspective that guides treatment evaluations in addiction assumes linearity in the relationship between treatment and outcomes, viewing behaviour change as a 'before and after event'. In this study we aim to examine how the direction of the trajectory of the process from addiction to recovery is constructed in personal narratives of active and recovering users. DESIGN 21 life stories from individuals at different stages of recovery and active use were collected and analysed following the principles of narrative analysis. RESULTS Personal trajectories were constructed in discontinuous, non-linear and long lasting patterns of repeated, and interchangeable, episodes of relapse and abstinence. Relapse appeared to be described as an integral part of a learning process through which knowledge leading to recovery was gradually obtained. CONCLUSION The findings show that long-term recovery is represented as being preceded by periods of discontinuity before change is stabilised. Such periods are presented to be lasting longer than most short-term pre-post intervention designs can capture and suggest the need to rethink how change is defined and measured.
Collapse
|
|
8 |
17 |
8
|
Peraldo M, Fasulo A, Chiappini E, Milio C, Marianelli L. Evaluation of glucose tolerance and insulin secretion in cystic fibrosis patients. HORMONE RESEARCH 2000; 49:65-71. [PMID: 9485173 DOI: 10.1159/000023128] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Blood glucose profile and insulin levels during the oral glucose tolerance test were retrospectively analyzed in 52 cystic fibrosis (CF) patients aged 10-33 years (mean 18 years). Thirty-seven subjects (71.1%) had normal glucose tolerance; 6 patients (11.5%) had impaired glucose tolerance and 9 patients (17.3%), with fasting blood glucose levels below 7.2 mmol/l were classified as diabetic, according to WHO diagnostic criteria. No significant differences in the total amount of secreted insulin (expressed as area under the curve) were detected in CF patients with or without glucose intolerance, but a significant delay in the insulin peak time appeared to be related to a compromised nutritional status. Our data indicate a significantly increased risk of glucose metabolism impairment related to poor nutritional condition (RR 5.40; 95% CI: 1.5-19.7) and worse clinical status (RR 4.27; 95% CI: 1.05-17.2). In particular, glucose tolerance abnormalities were found in 50% of CF patients with an unsatisfactory nutritional condition versus 15.6% of CF patients with good nutritional status. CONCLUSIONS Since CF-related diabetes is often underdiagnosed and associated with deterioration in patients' overall clinical status, it is very important to identify subjects at risk of developing diabetes by strict monitoring of glucose metabolism when deterioration in nutritional and clinical conditions is seen which cannot be otherwise explained.
Collapse
|
Clinical Trial |
25 |
15 |
9
|
|
|
38 |
11 |
10
|
Fatigante M, Fasulo A, Pontecorvo C. Life with the alien: role casting and face-saving techniques in family conversation with young children. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1998. [DOI: 10.5070/l492005274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
|
27 |
11 |
11
|
Bonaiuto M, Fasulo A. Rhetorical intentionality attribution: its ontogenesis in ordinary conversation. BRITISH JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 1997; 36 ( Pt 4):511-36. [PMID: 9440215 DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1997.tb01147.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
This paper addresses the assumption that the epistemological dichotomy between reality and its construction is an everyday rhetorical tool children and adults use in their explanations and arguments. Within a discursive psychology perspective, the phenomenon of attribution of rhetorical intentionality (AIR) is theoretically defined developing the notion of prepackaged interest attribution technique (Edwards & Potter, 1992). AIR consists in an explanation of others' discursive moves in terms of a rhetorical-argumentative activity; that is, in terms of a deliberate attempt aimed at construing and presenting reality as a function of the speaker's point of view, perspective and interests, in order to affect the listeners mental state (belief, knowledge, intention) or action. Theoretical links are made to the theory of mind paradigm in the study of intentionality attribution, particularly to the form of life approach which defines intentionality attribution in terms of language games. On this basis, the present study examines the structures and functions of AIR used by children and their parents in everyday conversation. Qualitative discourse analysis of transcripts of 22 dinner conversations from eight Italian families shows explicit and implicit AIR in children from three and a half to seven years of age. Moreover, parents attribute rhetorical intentionality to their children, contributing to their social construction as rhetorical speakers. AIR is used as a rhetorical strategy within disputes and argumentative contexts, often achieving topic closure. Results are discussed with reference to the ontological status of intentionalist talk.
Collapse
|
Comparative Study |
28 |
11 |
12
|
Martinucci ME, Curradi G, Fasulo A, Medici A, Toni S, Osovik G, Lapistkaya E, Sherbitskaya E. Incidence of childhood type 1 diabetes mellitus in Gomel, Belarus. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab 2002; 15:53-7. [PMID: 11822581 DOI: 10.1515/jpem.2002.15.1.53] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The importance of genetic and environmental factors in causing type 1 diabetes mellitus (DM1) in many European countries is unknown. We studied this question by assessing the incidence of the disease in children in Gomel, Belarus, in the years subsequent to the Chernobyl disaster, comparing it to the antecedent period. METHODS We identified all new patients with DM1 under 15 years at onset who presented between 1976 and 1999; we used as denominator the number of children aged 0-14 born in Gomel, to calculate the incidence. All data were validated by using the capture-recapture method. We compared the incidence rate between the periods before and after 1986; we also calculated the average increment of incidence in the latter period (1987-1999). RESULTS In the whole period, 438 new patients presented, with an average crude rate of incidence of 4.6/100,000 but with a net difference between the former (before 1986) and latter (after 1986) periods. The average standard incidence in the latter period was 5.7 (C.I. 4.5-7.0), with a significant increase in the 10-14 year age group. The average yearly increase of incidence in the same period was 8.9%, highly significant (p < 0.001). CONCLUSIONS The results seem to confirm the hypothesis of the influence of environmental pollution subsequent to the Chernobyl accident.
Collapse
|
|
23 |
9 |
13
|
Fantasia V, Markova G, Fasulo A, Costall A, Reddy V. Not Just Being Lifted: Infants are Sensitive to Delay During a Pick-Up Routine. Front Psychol 2016; 6:2065. [PMID: 26834674 PMCID: PMC4718994 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2015] [Accepted: 12/31/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
In the present study we observed whether infants show online adjustments to the mother’s incipient action by looking at their sensitivity to changes as the pick-up unfolded. Twenty-three 3-month-old infants and their mothers were observed in the lab, where mothers were instructed (1) to pick-up their infants as they usually did (normal pick-up), and then (2) to delay the pick-up for 6 s after placing their hands on the infants’ waist (delayed pick-up). In both Normal and Delayed conditions infant’s body tension, affective displays and gaze shifts were coded during three phases: Approach, Contact, and Lift. Additionally, a measure of infants’ head support in terms of head lag at the beginning and end of Lift was computed. Results showed that during normal pick-up infants tensed up their body during the Approach phase and increased their tension during contact, maintaining it through Lift; their head was also supported and in line with their body during Lift. When the pick-up was delayed, infants also tensed their body during Approach, yet this tension did not increase during the Contact phase and was significantly lower at Lift. Their head support was also lower in the Delayed condition and they shifted their gazes away from their mothers’ face more often than in the Normal condition. These results suggest that infants are sensitive to changes of the timing of the pick-up sequence, which in turn may have affected their contribution to the interaction.
Collapse
|
Journal Article |
9 |
9 |
14
|
Fasulo A, Shukla J, Bennett S. Find the Hidden Object. Understanding Play in Psychological Assessments. Front Psychol 2017; 8:323. [PMID: 28392771 PMCID: PMC5364837 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00323] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2016] [Accepted: 02/20/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Standardized psychological assessments are extensively used by practitioners to determine rate and level of development in different domains of ability in both typical and atypical children. The younger the children, the more likely the trials will resemble play activities. However, mode of administration, timing and use of objects involved are constrained. The purpose of this study is to explore what kind of play is play in psychological assessments, what are the expectations about children's performance and what are the abilities supporting the test activities. Conversation Analysis (CA) was applied to the videorecording of an interaction between a child and a practitioner during the administration of the Bayley Scale of Infant and Toddler Development, III edition. The analysis focuses on a 2′07″ long sequence relative to the administration of the test item “Find the hidden object” to a 23 months old child with Down syndrome. The analysis of the sequence shows that the assessor promotes the child's engagement by couching the actions required to administer the item in utterances with marked child-directed features. The analysis also shows that the objects constituting the test item did not suggest to the child a unique course of action, leading to the assessor's modeling of the successful sequence. We argue that when a play frame is activated by an interactional partner, the relational aspect of the activity is foregrounded and the co-player becomes a source of cues for ways in which playing can develop. We discuss the assessment interaction as orienting the child toward a right-or-wrong interpretation, leaving the realm of play, which is inherently exploratory and inventive, to enter that of instructional activities. Finally, we argue that the sequential analysis of the interaction and of the mutual sense-making procedures that partners put in place during the administration of an assessment could be used in the design and evaluation of tests for a finer understanding of the abilities involved.
Collapse
|
|
8 |
7 |
15
|
Costantini C, Akehurst L, Reddy V, Fasulo A. Synchrony, Co-Eating and Communication During Complementary Feeding in Early Infancy. INFANCY 2017. [DOI: 10.1111/infa.12220] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
|
8 |
7 |
16
|
Kain ZN, Fasulo A, Rimar S. Establishment of a pediatric surgery center: increasing anesthetic efficiency. J Clin Anesth 1999; 11:540-4. [PMID: 10624636 DOI: 10.1016/s0952-8180(99)00080-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
STUDY OBJECTIVE To examine whether the establishment of dedicated pediatric operating rooms (ORs) staffed exclusively by pediatric anesthesiologists has had a significant impact on anesthetic efficiency during surgery. STUDY DESIGN Before and after design. SETTING General and pediatric operating rooms at Yale-New Haven Hospital. MEASUREMENTS AND MAIN RESULTS Using Operating Room Information System data (1991 to 1997), we examined whether the anesthesia-controlled time, the time it takes for induction and emergence of anesthesia of a selected surgical procedure (tonsillectomy and adenoidectomy), was affected by the change of practice from general to pediatric ORs. The average length of anesthesia induction decreased by 30% (p = 0.0007). Similarly, the average length of emergence from anesthesia decreased by 42% (p = 0.01) and anesthesia-controlled time decreased by 31% (p = 0.0008). Of particular importance is the decrease by 75% in the anesthesia-controlled time range (maximum-minimum). CONCLUSIONS The establishment of dedicated pediatric ORs resulted in significantly shorter anesthesia induction and emergence times. Furthermore, the decreased variability of anesthesia-controlled time may allow for better scheduling of surgical cases and for better surgeon and patient satisfaction.
Collapse
|
|
26 |
5 |
17
|
Costantini C, Harris G, Reddy V, Akehurst L, Fasulo A. Introducing Complementary Foods to Infants: Does Age Really Matter? A Look at Feeding Practices in Two European Communities: British and Italian. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/13575279.2017.1414033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
|
7 |
5 |
18
|
Abstract
Observations about peculiarities in the autistic population concerning type and frequency of references to subjective states, and lack of perspective taking, have been on the whole referred to as the paradox of the autistic self, i.e. a co-presence of ego-centeredness and weak self-referentiality (Lombardo & Baron Cohen 2010). Prevalent approaches in autism ascribe these peculiarities to high order disfunctions caused by neurological factors, such as defective self-encoding processes. Two narratives told by an adult man with Asperger during counselling are examined with Conversation Analysis; the analysis identifies features that may lead to descriptions like the paradox of autistic self, but also reveals competences related to perspective-taking and narrative construction. Drawing on Bruner's narrative theory, as well on recent interactional research on autism and the psychology of self, it is suggested that a relatively limited practice with narrative co-construction might be at the origin of the peculiarities observed. A socio-developmental approach to the understanding of autism not only can provide explanations compatible with first and second person accounts of life with autism, but can also open new paths for researching ways of self-construction that are less reliant on social interaction. The article finally challenges assumptions in psychological research about the ability of humans to access their internal states, and discusses how such assumptions can deter understanding of atypical populations.
Collapse
|
Case Reports |
6 |
5 |
19
|
Fantasia V, Galbusera L, Reck C, Fasulo A. Rethinking Intrusiveness: Exploring the Sequential Organization in Interactions Between Infants and Mothers. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1543. [PMID: 31396121 PMCID: PMC6668658 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2019] [Accepted: 06/18/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
To date, studies investigating maternal postpartum depression (PPD) have mainly focused on identifying failures in interactions of postpartum depressed mothers and their infants, often attributed to single dysfunctional maternal behaviors. Intrusiveness has been identified as a dysfunctional behavior characterizing mothers suffering from PPD. However, this research does not consider the co-constructed and sequential nature of social interactions, in which single behaviors cannot be conceived as isolated or disconnected units. The aim of the work presented in this paper was to explore the interactional dynamics underlying maternal behaviors previously identified as intrusive by mainstream literature on postpartum depression. Through a conversation analytical approach, we analyzed filmed interactions between mothers with and without postpartum depression and their 3-months-old infants. The analyses of 4 selected episodes illustrate similar dyadic activities, yet presenting different levels of mutuality and affective attunement. Results showed two normative features of social interactions that contributed to the different quality in the mutual adjustment of the partners: interactional rhythm and preliminaries. Interactional rhythm refers to the structuring of infants' spontaneous activity into a turn sequence, whereas preliminaries consist of verbal or nonverbal moves that anticipate following action. As evident from our analytical observations, what seems to be hindering the mutual coordination (previously labeled as “intrusive”) is not based on specific individual behaviors but on the absence or violation of such interactional norms. Adopting an interactive and dynamical framework, we shifted the focus from maternal behaviors considered as dysfunctional to observing the unfolding of interactional aspects contributing to better or poorer sequential structuring. We argue that these aspects shape the possibilities for the infant's participation. Finally, we discuss the theoretical and methodological implications of adopting a conversation analytical approach for a better understanding of the relational dynamics related to clinical and non-clinical interactions.
Collapse
|
|
6 |
4 |
20
|
Toni S, Reali MF, Fasulo A, Festini P, Medici A, Martinucci ME. The use of insulin pumps improves the metabolic control in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Arch Dis Child 2004; 89:796-7. [PMID: 15269092 PMCID: PMC1720044 DOI: 10.1136/adc.2003.046805] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
|
letter |
21 |
3 |
21
|
Fasulo A, Zinken J, Zinken K. Asking 'What about' questions in chronic illness self-management meetings. PATIENT EDUCATION AND COUNSELING 2016; 99:917-925. [PMID: 27026389 DOI: 10.1016/j.pec.2016.03.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2015] [Revised: 03/07/2016] [Accepted: 03/13/2016] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES This study investigates 'What about' questions asked by patients in the course of diabetes self-management groups led by nurses, and explores their functions in these empowerment-informed settings. METHODS Conversation Analysis of 24 video-recorded sessions of a Start Insulin Group Programme for patients with type 2 diabetes, in a diabetes centre in the South of England. The groups included 2-7 patients and were led by 5 nurses, all of whom had received training in the empowerment approach. RESULTS The analysis revealed a prevalence of single-unit 'Whatabout X' questions and found that they were used to embed requests for information in current or just closed activities. The nurses always provided the information, but could ask patients to specify the content of the question and collaborate to the answer. CONCLUSION The analysis suggests that the short form of the question may be adapting to the nurses' restraint in giving recommendations or immediate responses to information seeking-questions. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS When healthcare communication practices are shaped in observance to a theoretical approach, such as empowerment, it is recommendable that practitioners monitor not only what they do, but also how patients change their habitual forms of speech in response.
Collapse
|
|
9 |
3 |
22
|
Abstract
In this article I pick up some threads from the contributions in the previous special issue of IPSB dedicated to the future of qualitative psychology, and elaborate on them around two main points. The first is the status of qualitative psychology as a social and institutional category; the second is what we mean by experience. As concerns the first point, I argue that using the label of qualitative psychology may separate us from the rest of psychology, also creating a false impression of homogeneity among qualitative approaches and a false opposition with quantitative methods. Implications for teaching as well as research are discussed. The second issue has to do with experience as the object of qualitative psychology investigations. I propose three ways of formulating experience in research which would prevent naïve assumptions about accessing it directly through language. These are 1) experience as experience of the researcher, 2) experience as situated intersubjectivity, and 3) experience as expression. I discuss how being clearer about definitions of experience and going towards engaged forms of research could safeguard the integrity of both researcher and participants.
Collapse
|
Review |
9 |
1 |
23
|
Fasulo A, Pino M. Sharing knowledge and shaping identities in healthcare interactions. PATIENT EDUCATION AND COUNSELING 2016; 99:875-877. [PMID: 27316516 DOI: 10.1016/j.pec.2016.04.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
|
Editorial |
9 |
1 |
24
|
Shetranjiwalla S, Fasulo A, Rhoden S. Eco-design and tunable structure-properties of chitosan-epoxy-glycerol-silicate biohybrids using integrated crosslinking. Carbohydr Polym 2023; 299:120187. [PMID: 36876802 DOI: 10.1016/j.carbpol.2022.120187] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2022] [Revised: 09/18/2022] [Accepted: 09/29/2022] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Tunable structure-properties were achieved for chitosan-epoxy-glycerol-silicate (CHTGP) biohybrids, eco-designed via integrated amine-epoxy and waterborne sol-gel crosslinking reactions. Medium molecular weight chitosan (CHT), with 83 % degree of deacetylation was prepared by microwave-assisted alkaline deacetylation of chitin. The amine group of chitosan was covalently bonded to the epoxide of 3-glycidoxypropyltrimethoxysilane (G) for further crosslinking with a sol-gel derived glycerol-silicate precursor (P) from 0.5 % to 5 %. The impact of crosslinking density on the structural morphology, thermal, mechanical, moisture-retention and antimicrobial properties of the biohybrids were characterized by FTIR, NMR, SEM, swelling and bacterial inhibition studies and contrasted with a corresponding series (CHTP) without epoxy silane. Water uptake was significantly reduced in all biohybrids with a 12 % window of variation between the two series. Properties observed in biohybrids with only epoxy-amine (CHTG) or sol-gel crosslinking reactions (CHTP), were reversed in the integrated biohybrids (CHTGP) to impart improved thermal and mechanical stability and antibacterial activity.
Collapse
|
|
2 |
1 |
25
|
Zinken KM, Fasulo A, Zinken J. Challenges of putting the patient expert approach into action. DIABETOL STOFFWECHS 2014. [DOI: 10.1055/s-0034-1375109] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
|
11 |
|