1
|
Moses J, Moon HD. Neuroendocrine hyperplasia in mucinous borderline ovarian tumour. Pathology 2023; 55:571-573. [PMID: 36759285 DOI: 10.1016/j.pathol.2022.10.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2022] [Revised: 10/10/2022] [Accepted: 10/20/2022] [Indexed: 01/21/2023]
|
2
|
Punnoose C, Moses J. Mesonephric-like carcinoma of the ovary and endometrium: A report of three cases. Pathology 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.pathol.2021.12.183] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
3
|
Shafi SQ, Brown S, Khaw RA, Hirniak J, Burke JR, Giwa L, Marson L, Hill A, Lobo D, Glasbey JC, McLean KA, Patel T, Liu G, Singal A, Nam R, Kathiravelupillai A, Chia WL, Ooi SZY, Matthews M, Ponniah SH, Komor J, Heyes A, Tushingham S, Hettiarachchi DS, K T, Gaier S, Jordan C, Joyce A, Johnston E, Valentine K, Nagassima K, Reis RD, O'Sullivan M, Tittawella A, Geary E, Thorpe C, Jalal AHB, Georgi M, Mergo A, Ramsay E, Sheikh J, Ashok A, Lee KS, Risquet R, Kathiravelupillai S, Chia D, Al Majid S, Matloob Ahmad AE, Hounat A, Shafi S, Wang J, Cambridge WA, Kawar L, Maseland T, Sharma K, Moses J, Patsalides MA, Brown S, Jaffer A, Feeney K, Richardson G, Joseph JP, Argus L, Sara X, Antypas A, de Andres Crespo M, Daly E, Abraha S. Medical student engagement with surgery and research during the COVID-19 pandemic: Supporting the future workforce for post-pandemic surgical recovery. Int J Surg 2021; 95:106105. [PMID: 34597820 PMCID: PMC8479464 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2021.106105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2021] [Accepted: 09/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
4
|
Stirk S, Moses J, McCulloch A, Phillips S. P242 Ageing with cystic fibrosis: how do older adults with cystic fibrosis adapt to change? J Cyst Fibros 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/s1569-1993(21)01267-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
5
|
Stirk S, Moses J. P251 The experience of adults with cystic fibrosis engaging in a complex health management regime: a meta-ethnography. J Cyst Fibros 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/s1569-1993(21)01276-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
6
|
Stewart CJR, Moses J. NKX3.1 expression in cervical 'adenoid basal cell carcinoma': another gynaecological lesion with prostatic differentiation? Pathology 2020; 53:193-198. [PMID: 33032811 DOI: 10.1016/j.pathol.2020.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2020] [Revised: 07/07/2020] [Accepted: 07/20/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Adenoid basal cell carcinoma (ABC) is considered a rare cervical neoplasm which when present in 'pure' form, uniquely amongst apparently malignant cervical tumours, has never been reported to metastasise or lead to fatal patient outcome. We recently encountered a case of ABC that was morphologically reminiscent of prostatic differentiation, more specifically basal cell hyperplasia of the prostate. Immunohistochemistry was strongly positive for the prostate related marker NKX3.1 in the glandular cells, but there was no expression of prostate specific antigen (PSA) or prostatic acid phosphatase (PAP). However, subsequent review of five additional cervical ABCs demonstrated focal PAP expression in two of four tested cases, and all were NKX3.1 positive. NKX3.1 expression was also demonstrated in the glandular epithelium of 10 additional gynaecological lesions considered to show prostatic differentiation including five cases of cervical ectopic prostatic tissue, three ovarian teratomas with prostatic differentiation, and two vaginal tubulosquamous polyps. We suggest that some lesions traditionally classified as ABC may in fact represent a variant of prostatic differentiation within the cervix, possibly analogous to basal cell hyperplasia of the prostate.
Collapse
|
7
|
Doubell J, Kyriakakis C, Weich H, Herbst P, Pecoraro A, Moses J, Griffiths B, Snyman HW, Kabwe L, Du Toit R, Joubert L, Hassan K, Doubell A. P6518Radial artery dilatation to improve access and lower complication rates during coronary angiography (RADIAL): a randomized controlled trial. Eur Heart J 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehz746.1108] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Background
Transradial catheterization has become the preferred access site for coronary angiography. The transradial approach is however not without challenges and complications. Cannulation is technically challenging and may require multiple cannulation attempts or access may fail. Local access site complications may occur postprocedurally.
Purpose
To explore the use of prolonged occlusion flow mediated dilatation (PO-FMD) to dilate the radial artery prior to cannulation to reduce puncture attempts, increase cannulation success and reduce access site complications in transradial coronary angiography.
Methods
1156 patients undergoing transradial coronary angiography were randomized into PO-FMD and sham PO-FMD groups. PO-FMD was achieved by a 10 minute inflation of a blood pressure cuff on the arm to above systolic pressure, followed by deflation with resultant radial artery dilation. In the sham PO-FMD group the cuff was not inflated. The operators were blinded to the intervention.
Results
580 patients were randomized to the sham PO-FMD group and 576 to the PO-FMD group. The number of puncture attempts were reduced with the use of PO-FMD, with a median number of attempts of 1 in the PO-FMD group and 2 in the sham PO-FMD group (p<0.001). Cannulation failure was reduced with PO-FMD FMD with cannulation failure rates of 2.7% in the PO-FMD group and 5.8% in the sham PO-FMD group (p=0.01). Radial artery pulsation loss (RAPL) was reduced with PO-FMD with 1.4% in the PO-FMD group and 3.8% in the sham PO-FMD group (p=0.02).
Conclusion
PO-FMD decreases puncture attempts, reduces cannulation failure rates and decreases RAPL during transradial coronary angiography.
Collapse
|
8
|
Guerra A, Moses J, Rivera J, Davis M, Hakinson K. A-65 Verbal Abilities May Influence Performance in Assessment of Verbal Short-Term Memory Tasks. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acz034.65] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Objective
Examine whether verbal abilities may help explain the learning strategies people employ when completing a short-term verbal memory task.
Methods
The assessment records of 296 American Veterans with diverse neuropsychiatric conditions were analyzed using Exploratory Factor Analyses. There were no exclusion criteria. All participants completed the Benton Serial Digit Learning Test – 9 Digits (SDL-9) and Visual Naming (VisNam), Sentence Repetition (SenRep), Controlled Word Association (COWA), and Token Tests of the Multilingual Aphasia Examination (MAE). Individual assessment instruments were factored using Principal Component Analyses (PCA). A three-factor solution of the SDL-9 was co-factored with the verbal components of the MAE to identify common sources of variance.
Results
A three-factor solution of the SDL-9 separated trials into three overlapping factors consisting of early (SDL-9_Early), middle (SDL-9_Middle), and late (SDL-9_Late) trials. Co-factoring the three new scales with the verbal components of the MAE produced a four-factor model explaining 67.85% of the shared variance: 1) SenRep loaded with SDL-9_Early, 2) COWAT loaded with SDL-9_Middle and SDL-9_Late, 3) Token loaded with SDL-9_Late, and 4) Vis Nam loaded with SDL-9_Late.
Conclusions
The results suggest that individuals may engage verbal abilities differently as they progress from simpler to more difficult verbal short-term memory tasks. It appears performance in early trials is mostly associated with rote repetition and performance on middle trials is mostly associated with verbal fluency, while performance on the late trials is associated with a combination of verbal fluency, auditory comprehension, and conceptual organization/naming. This may therefore indicate a shift in learning strategy to meet increased cognitive demands.
Collapse
|
9
|
Hakinson K, Moses J, RIvera J, Guerra A, Davis M, Greenman K. A-49 Visual Memory Errors and Intelligence: The Role of Verbal Mediation. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acz034.49] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Objective
Examine the relationship of verbal mediation with visual memory errors and intelligence to understand the role of spoken language on other assessment measures.
Method
Assessment records were obtained from a Veteran Affairs clinic for veterans (n=100) with diverse neuropsychiatric conditions who completed the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, third edition (WAIS-III), Multilingual Aphasia Examination (MAE), and Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT). A Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was used to examine the interrelationship among these assessments. The components of spoken language, types of errors on the BVRT, and the four factors of the WAIS-III were factored using the PCA to identify common sources of variance.
Results
A principal component analysis revealed a six-factor model explaining 68.16% of the shared variance among the WAIS-III factors, MAE components, and BVRT Errors. Omission errors loaded with Processing Speed and Controlled Word Association. Distortions and size errors loaded with Perceptual Organization. Size errors also loaded with Verbal Comprehension and Visual Naming. Misplacements loaded with Working Memory and Sentence Repetition. Misplacements, perseverations, and omissions loaded with the Token Test (a measure associated with auditory comprehension). Rotation errors loaded with Perceptual Organization.
Conclusions
Results indicated significant shared variance between visual memory errors, spoken language, and intelligence factors. This suggests that spoken language is involved in the process of visual memory, and deficits in spoken language may result in increased errors on visual memory tasks. Therefore, treatment recommendations for visual memory difficulties should take into consideration verbal capabilities and intelligence factors to better individualize treatment.
Collapse
|
10
|
Gukasyan M, Moses J, Greenman K. A-44 Categorical Errors on the Benton Visual Retention Test are Systemically Related to Specific Factorial Components of Intelligence. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acz034.44] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Objective
We investigated the factorial relationship of the six categories of memory errors of the BVRT to the four factorial variables of the WAIS to determine the relationship between cognitive and nonverbal memory variables.
Methods
A sample of 134 diagnostically mixed ambulatory American Veteran patients with a wide variety of mixed neuropsychiatric diagnoses and with or without general medical problems who had completed the WAIS-3, and the BVRT were examined. There were no demographic or diagnostic exclusion criteria.
Results
The 6 types of BVRT memory errors (omissions, distortions, perseverations, rotations, misplacements, and size errors) were factored using principal component analysis. The four WAIS 3 and six BVRT components were jointly factored to examine for systematic relationships between memory and cognitive domains. The analysis identified specific factorial relationships of BVRT error type to each of the four factorial components of the WAIS. POI was related to rotation errors, VCI was related to size errors, PSI specifically related to omissions and WMI to distortions. Misplacement and perseveration errors were related to each other but not to factorial constructs of the WAIS.
Conclusions
There are specific and robust relationships among BVRT errors and dimensional cognitive variables.
Collapse
|
11
|
Davis M, Moses J, Rivera J, Guerra A, Hakinson K. A-53 Exploring the Relationship between Spoken Language and Verbal Short-term Memory Assessment Tasks. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acz034.53] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Objective
Examine whether performance on spoken language assessment measures may be associated with performance at different phases of verbal learning and recall tasks.
Method
The assessment records of 222 American Veterans with diverse neuropsychiatric conditions were analyzed using Exploratory Factor Analyses. There were no exclusion criteria. All participants completed the Visual Naming (VisNam), Sentence Repetition (SenRep), Controlled Word Association (COWA), and Token Tests of the Multilingual Aphasia Examination (MAE), and Benton Serial Digit Learning Test – 8 Digits (SDL8). Individual assessment instruments were factored using Principal Component Analyses (PCA). A three-factor solution of the SDL-8 was co-factored with the spoken language components of the MAE to identify common sources of variance.
Results
A three-factor solution of the SDL8 separated trials into three overlapping factors consisting of early (SDL8_Early), middle (SDL8_Middle), and late (SDL8_Late) trials. Co-factoring the three new scales with the verbal components of the MAE produced a five-factor model explaining 84.563% of the shared variance: 1) SDL8_Early loaded with SenRep, 2) SDL8_Middle loaded with SenRep, 3) SDL8_Late loaded with Token, 4) SDL8_Late loaded with COWA, and 5) VisNam alone formed the fifth factor.
Conclusions
The results suggest that rote repetition is largely associated with early trials and slightly associated with middle trials, while late trials are largely associated with auditory comprehension and slightly associated with verbal fluency. This may be indicative of a shift in use of spoken language abilities to accommodate increasing levels of complexity in presented verbal short-term memory tasks and thus reflective of a change on learning strategy to optimize performance.
Collapse
|
12
|
Gukasyan M, Bhowmick C, Moses J. A-43 Factorial Relationships Among Copy and Memory Trials of the Benton Visual Retention Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acz034.43] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Objective
We investigated the factorial relationships among categorical error groups on the Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT) copy and memory trials.
Methods
A sample of 523 ambulatory American Veteran patients who presented for clinical evaluation with a wide variety of mixed neuropsychiatric diagnoses and general medical diagnoses were studied. There were no demographic or diagnostic exclusion criteria.
Results
Frequency summary scores for the six types of BVRT errors (omission, misplacement, size, distortion, perseveration, and rotation errors) were factored jointly by means of principal component analysis. Omission, misplacement, and size errors grouped factorially across copy and memory domains by error type. Results showed the factorial relationships are primarily defined by the type of error. Omission, size, and misplacement errors were grouped together regardless of whether they occurred on copy or memory trials. Rotation, distortion, and perseveration errors were factorially grouped on both the copy and memory trials, but the groupings of these similar error groups formed independent factors for the copy and memory trials. The copy error factor explained the most variance and the memory error factor o explained the least variance.
Conclusions
Omission, size, and misplacement errors on the BVRT copy and memory trials appear to be due to similar encoding process errors. Distortion, rotation, and perseveration errors on the BVRT copy and memory trials are related to each other within each trial type but different cognitive processes account for errors of this kind on the copy and memory trials.
Collapse
|
13
|
Gukasyan M, Moses J. A-45 Systematic Relationships of the Benton Visual Retention Test to Intelligence, Language and Demographics. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acz034.45] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Objective
We investigated the factorial relationships of WAIS 3, BVRT, Visual Naming and demographic variables to better understand factorial relationships among those variables.
Methods
A sample of 126 ambulatory American Veteran patients who presented for clinical evaluation with a wide variety of mixed neuropsychiatric diagnoses and general medical diagnoses were studied. There were no demographic or diagnostic exclusion criteria.
Results
Our first analysis demonstrated robust independent relationships of age to a late occurring BVRT item group and education to an early occurring BVRT item group. A two factor solution for the items of the multilingual aphasia exam visual naming subtest from previous research showed systematic and robust relationships of one visual naming factor to VCI and the other visual naming factor to PSI only. Factor scales were computed to represent the orthogonal factors for the new variables in each of these analyses. Factor scales from the first two analyses were factored together to produce a four-part solution that explained 86% of the variance. POI was related to the late BVRT item group and age. The early BVRT item grouping was related to educational level, VCI, PSI and both visual naming components. WMI was independent of demographic, linguistic and BVRT variables.
Conclusions
There are factorial relationships between factorial components of nonverbal memory, intelligence, naming and demographic variables.
Collapse
|
14
|
Rivera J, Moses J, Davis M, Guerra A, Hakinson K. A-52 An Exploratory Factor Analysis Investigation of the Role of Verbal Mediation in the Interaction between Intelligence and Visual Memory Tasks. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acz034.52] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Objective
Examine whether verbal mediation may play a role in the interaction between visual memory tasks and the four-factor model of intelligence as operationalized by standard neuropsychological assessment instruments.
Method
The assessment records of 101 American Veterans with diverse neuropsychiatric conditions were examined using Exploratory Factor and Principal Component Analyses (EFA and PCA respectively). There were no exclusion criteria. All participants completed the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, third edition (WAIS-III), Benton’s Visual Retention Test (BVRT), and Multilingual Aphasia Examination (MAE). Individual assessment instruments were factored using PCA. The factor solution of the BVRT was co-factored with the scales of the WAIS-III, then the resulting factor scales were again factored with the verbal components of the MAE to identify common sources of variance.
Results
A three-step analysis revealed a four-factor model explaining 69.44% of the shared variance: 1) Items 1-4 of the BVRT (BVRT-E) loaded with Verbal Comprehension and Visual Naming. 2) BVRT-E also loaded with Processing Speed and Controlled Word Association. 3) Items 5-10 of the BVRT (BVRT-L) loaded with Perceptual Organization and the Token Test. 4) Working Memory loaded with Sentence Repetition on a fourth factor.
Conclusions
The results indicate a strong relationship between assessed performance on visual memory tasks and performance on measures based on the four-factor model of intelligence. The results also appear to support the idea that verbal mediation plays a role in the interaction between visual memory and intelligence, particularly when comparing performance on simple versus more complex visual memory tasks.
Collapse
|
15
|
Husna ASA, Geeta IB, Sha SS, Muhammed A, Moses J. In silico evaluation of the efficacies of two different medicaments against Enterococcus faecalis. ENDODONTOLOGY 2019. [DOI: 10.4103/endo.endo_88_18] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
|
16
|
Greenman K, Moses J. B - 29An Exploratory Factor Analysis of Verbal Mediation in Visual Assessment Tasks. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2018. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acy061.105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
17
|
Rivera J, Moses J, Greenman K, Gukasyan M, Newman N. B - 33Verbal and Nonverbal Intelligence Components Mediate Aspects of Visual Memory and Perception. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2018. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acy061.109] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
|
18
|
Doubell J, Kyriakakis C, Weich H, Herbst P, Pecoraro A, Griffiths B, Snyman HW, Moses J, Kabwe L, Du Toit R, Joubert L, Hassan K, Doubell A. P5519Radial artery dilatation to improve access and lower complication rates during coronary angiography (RADIAL): a randomized controlled trial. Eur Heart J 2018. [DOI: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehy566.p5519] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
19
|
Seo B, Aaron H, Moses J. C-45Specific Attention Span, Working Memory and Spoken Language Components Mediate Rote Auditory Learning and Recall Ability. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2017. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acx076.212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
20
|
Sakhai S, Moses J, Smith J, Caffrey S. C-57Factorial Construct Validation of an Interactive Model of Visual Perception and Visual Memory with Verbal and Nonverbal Intelligence. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2017. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acx076.224] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
|
21
|
Gukasyan M, Moses J, Lai J. C-50Factorial Relationships of Visual Pattern Recognition to Intelligence and Spoken Language. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2017. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acx076.217] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
22
|
Davis B, Moses J, Aaron H. C-41Specific Spoken Language Components Mediate Each Stage of Rote Auditory Learning. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2017. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acx076.208] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
23
|
Aaron H, Moses J, Davis B, Thimpson D, Eng M. C-60Cognitive Mediation Strategies of Benton's Visual Form Discrimination Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2016. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acw043.209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
24
|
Smith J, Moses J. B-64Spoken Language and WAIS-III Factorial Components Are Dimensionally and Specifically Related to Stages of Serial Digit Learning Test Performance. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2016. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acw043.139] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
25
|
Davis B, Moses J, Aaron H, Eng M, Thompson D. B-60Verbal and Non-Verbal Mediation Strategies of Independent Components of Visual Naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2016. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acw043.135] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|