51
|
Thierry KL, Lamb ME, Pipe ME, Spence MJ. The flexibility of source-monitoring training: Reducing young children's source confusions. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2009. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1574] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
52
|
The susceptibility of young preschoolers to source similarity effects: confusing story or video events with reality. J Exp Child Psychol 2009; 102:392-407. [PMID: 19217122 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.12.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2008] [Revised: 12/23/2008] [Accepted: 12/31/2008] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
This study examined children's tendency to confuse events that varied in source similarity, which was manipulated using different media of event presentation. In Experiment 1, children in two age groups (3- and 4-year-olds and 5- and 6-year-olds) experienced a live presentation of an event, and another event was either heard from a story (low similarity group) or seen on a video (high similarity group). Immediately afterward, the children were asked to monitor the source of the events. The children in the low similarity group produced higher source discrimination scores than did the children in the high similarity group. Overall, the older children were better at source monitoring than were the younger children. In Experiment 2, the procedure was replicated except that the children's source monitoring was tested after a 4-day delay. When attributing the source of the story or video events, both 3- and 4-year-olds and 5- and 6-year-olds in the low similarity group produced more accurate story or video attributions than did their age mates in the high similarity group. However, when attributing the source of the live events, only the 3- and 4-year-olds evidenced this effect of source similarity. The 5- and 6-year-olds in both the low and high similarity groups performed at ceiling levels for live discriminations.
Collapse
|
53
|
Otgaar H, Candel I, Merckelbach H, Wade KA. Abducted by a UFO: prevalence information affects young children's false memories for an implausible event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2009. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1445] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
|
54
|
Drivdahl SB, Zaragoza MS, Learned DM. The role of emotional elaboration in the creation of false memories. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2009. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1446] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
55
|
Abstract
A crucial issue in the study of eyewitness memory concerns effects of repeated interviews on children's memory accuracy. There is growing belief that exposure to repeated interviews causes increased errors. In some situations, it may. Yet, several studies reveal increased accuracy with repeated interviewing, even when the interviews include misleading questions. We review repeated-interview research in relation to event veracity, interviewer bias, and delay. We conclude that when and how children are interviewed is at least as important for their accuracy as is how many times they are interviewed.
Collapse
|
56
|
Schaaf JM, Alexander KW, Goodman GS. Children’s false memory and true disclosure in the face of repeated questions. J Exp Child Psychol 2008; 100:157-85. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2007] [Accepted: 09/14/2007] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
57
|
Otgaar H, Candel I, Merckelbach H. Children's false memories: easier to elicit for a negative than for a neutral event. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008; 128:350-4. [PMID: 18462700 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2007] [Revised: 03/12/2008] [Accepted: 03/14/2008] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study examined the role of valence in the development of children's implanted false memories. Seventy-six 7-year-old children listened to two true and one false narrative. The false narrative was either neutral ("moving to another classroom") or emotional negative ("being accused by the teacher for copying off your neighbor"). In addition, half of the children were presented with their class photograph while listening to the narratives. During two interviews, children recalled as many details as possible from the true and false events. Results showed that the negative event elicited more false memories than the neutral event. The presentation of a true photograph did not promote the development of false memories.
Collapse
|
58
|
Principe GF, Smith E. Seeing Things Unseen: Fantasy Beliefs and False Reports. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2008. [DOI: 10.1080/15248370701836618] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
59
|
Ghetti S, Lyons KE, Lazzarin F, Cornoldi C. The development of metamemory monitoring during retrieval: The case of memory strength and memory absence. J Exp Child Psychol 2008; 99:157-81. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.11.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2007] [Revised: 11/26/2007] [Accepted: 11/27/2007] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
60
|
Connolly DA, Price HL, Lavoie JAA, Gordon HM. Perceptions and predictors of children's credibility of a unique event and an instance of a repeated event. LAW AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2008; 32:92-112. [PMID: 17253152 DOI: 10.1007/s10979-006-9083-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2006] [Accepted: 12/26/2006] [Indexed: 05/13/2023]
Abstract
Perceptions of children's credibility were studied in two experiments wherein participants watched a videotape of a 4- to 5- or a 6- to 7-year old child report details of a play session that had been experienced once (single-event) or was the last in a series of four similar play sessions (repeat-event). The child's report was classified as high or low accurate. In Experiments 1 and 2, reports of repeat-event children were judged to be less believable on several measures. In Experiment 1, younger children were viewed as less credible than older children. In both experiments, neither undergraduates nor community members correctly discriminated between high- and low-accurate reports. Content analysis in Study 3 revealed the relationship between age and event frequency and children's credibility ratings was mediated by the internal consistency of children's reports. Recent research on children's reports of instances of repeated events has identified several challenges facing children who report repeated abuse. These data bring to light another potential difficulty for these children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Deborah A Connolly
- Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, 8888 University Dr., Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada V5A 1S6.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
61
|
Principe GF, Smith E. The tooth, the whole tooth and nothing but the tooth: how belief in the Tooth Fairy can engender false memories. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2008. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1402] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
62
|
Strange D, Hayne H, Garry M. A photo, a suggestion, a false memory. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2008. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1390] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
63
|
Abstract
In two experiments, congruence of postevent information was manipulated in order to explore its role in the misinformation effect. Congruence of a detail was empirically defined as its compatibility (or match) with a concrete event. Based on this idea it was predicted that a congruent suggested detail would be more easily accepted than an incongruent one. In Experiments 1 and 2 two factors(congruence and truth value ) were manipulated within-subjects, and a two-alternative forced-choice recognition test was used followed by phenomenological judgements. Furthermore, in the second experiment participants were asked to describe four critical items (two seen and two suggested details)to explore differences and similarities between real and unreal memories. Both experiments clearly showed that the congruence of false information caused a robust misinformation effect, so that congruent information was much more accepted than false incongruent information. Furthermore, congruence increased the descriptive and phenomenological similarities between perceived and suggested memories, thus contributing to the misleading effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nieves Pérez-Mata
- Departamento de Psicología Básica, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, C/ Ivan Pavlov, No 6, 28049 Madrid, Spain.
| | | |
Collapse
|
64
|
Faller KC. Coaching children about sexual abuse: a pilot study of professionals' perceptions. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2007; 31:947-59. [PMID: 17870160 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2005] [Revised: 01/07/2007] [Accepted: 05/10/2007] [Indexed: 05/17/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Kathleen Coulborn Faller
- School of Social Work, Family Assessment Clinic, University of Michigan, 1080 S. University Avenue, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1066, USA
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Quas JA, Malloy LC, Melinder A, Goodman GS, D'Mello M, Schaaf J. Developmental differences in the effects of repeated interviews and interviewer bias on young children's event memory and false reports. Dev Psychol 2007; 43:823-37. [PMID: 17605517 PMCID: PMC2913698 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.823] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The present study investigated developmental differences in the effects of repeated interviews and interviewer bias on children's memory and suggestibility. Three- and 5-year-olds were singly or repeatedly interviewed about a play event by a highly biased or control interviewer. Children interviewed once by the biased interviewer after a long delay made the most errors. Children interviewed repeatedly, regardless of interviewer bias, were more accurate and less likely to falsely claim that they played with a man. In free recall, among children questioned once after a long delay by the biased interviewer, 5-year-olds were more likely than were 3-year-olds to claim falsely that they played with a man. However, in response to direct questions, 3-year-olds were more easily manipulated into implying that they played with him. Findings suggest that interviewer bias is particularly problematic when children's memory has weakened. In contrast, repeated interviews that occur a short time after a to-be-remembered event do not necessarily increase children's errors, even when interviews include misleading questions and interviewer bias. Implications for developmental differences in memory and suggestibility are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jodi A Quas
- Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-7085, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
66
|
Ceci SJ, Kulkofsky S, Klemfuss JZ, Sweeney CD, Bruck M. Unwarranted Assumptions about Children's Testimonial Accuracy. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2007; 3:311-28. [PMID: 17716058 DOI: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
We examine eight unwarranted assumptions made by expert witnesses, forensic interviewers, and legal scholars about the reliability of children's eyewitness reports. The first four assumptions modify some central beliefs about the nature of suggestive interviews, age-related differences in resistance to suggestion, and thresholds necessary to produce tainted reports. The fifth unwarranted assumption involves the influence of both individual and interviewer factors in determining children's suggestibility. The sixth unwarranted assumption concerns the claim that suggested reports are detectable. The seventh unwarranted assumption concerns new findings about how children deny, disclose, and/or recant their abuse. Finally, we examine unwarranted statements about the value of science to the forensic arena. It is important not only for researchers but also expert witnesses and court-appointed psychologists to be aware of these unwarranted assumptions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen J Ceci
- Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
67
|
Hershkowitz I, Fisher S, Lamb ME, Horowitz D. Improving credibility assessment in child sexual abuse allegations: the role of the NICHD investigative interview protocol. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2007; 31:99-110. [PMID: 17316794 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2004] [Revised: 09/12/2006] [Accepted: 09/30/2006] [Indexed: 05/14/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES The study was designed to explore whether the credibility of children's statements regarding their alleged experiences of child sexual abuse could be assessed in a more valid and reliable way when investigative interviews were conducted using the NICHD protocol rather than in an unstructured manner. METHODS Forty-two experienced Israeli youth investigators each assessed the credibility of allegations of sexual abuse made by alleged victims of sexual abuse when interviewed either with or without the protocol. Half of the alleged incidents were judged likely to have happened ("plausible") on the basis of independent evidence, while half were deemed unlikely to have happened ("implausible"). RESULTS More non-protocol than protocol interviews were rated as "No judgment possible" rather than either credible or incredible. Allegations made in protocol interviews were more accurately rated as credible or incredible when they were either plausible or implausible, respectively, than those made in non-protocol statements. Levels of inter-rater reliability were also higher when protocol interviews were rated. The differences were significant only for plausible cases, however. CONCLUSIONS The use of the NICHD protocol facilitated the assessment of credibility by child investigators although incredible allegations (those describing incidents that were unlikely to have happened) remained difficult to detect, even when the protocol was used.
Collapse
|
68
|
Ceci SJ, Bruck M. Children's suggestibility: characteristics and mechanisms. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2006; 34:247-81. [PMID: 17120807 DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2407(06)80009-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen J Ceci
- Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
69
|
Lane SM. Dividing attention during a witnessed event increases eyewitness suggestibility. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2006. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
70
|
Crossman AM, Lewis M. Adults' ability to detect children's lying. BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES & THE LAW 2006; 24:703-15. [PMID: 17016813 DOI: 10.1002/bsl.731] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/12/2023]
Abstract
Adults are poor deception detectors when examining lies told by adults, on average. However, there are some adults who are better at detecting lies than others. Children learn to lie at a very young age, a behavior that is socialized by parents. Yet, less is known about the ability to detect children's lies, particularly with regard to individual differences in the ability to detect this deception. The current study explored adult raters' ability to discern honesty in children who lied or told the truth about committing a misdeed. Results showed that adults are no better at detecting children's lies than they are with adult lies. In particular, adults were very poor at identifying children's honest statements. However, individual differences did emerge, suggesting that the ability to detect lying in children might be facilitated by relevant experience working with children. Implications for legal and mental health contexts are discussed.
Collapse
|
71
|
Faller KC. False accusations of child maltreatment: a contested issue. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2005; 29:1327-31. [PMID: 16293308 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.10.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Kathleen Coulborn Faller
- University of Michigan, School of Social Work, Family Assessment Clinic, 1080 S. University Avenue, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
2005 Award Winners: Distinguished Professional Contributions. AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2005. [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.60.8.869a] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
73
|
Quas JA, Thompson WC, Alison K, Stewart C. Do jurors "know" what isn't so about child witnesses? LAW AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2005; 29:425-56. [PMID: 16133948 DOI: 10.1007/s10979-005-5523-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/04/2023]
Abstract
Are expert witnesses needed in child sexual abuse cases to educate jurors about children's memory, suggestibility, and reactions to abuse, or do jurors already know what such experts could tell them? To cast light on this question, we surveyed jurors and jury-eligible college students and compared their beliefs with what is known via scientific research regarding children's memory and ability to testify, reactions to interrogation, and reactions to sexual abuse. We also asked participants to infer results of four widely cited studies of children's suggestibility. Participants' beliefs were consistent with findings from research on some issues (e.g., that children can be led to claim that false events occurred) but diverged from the scientific consensus on other issues (e.g., whether children can remember painful events in infancy). Similarly, participants sometimes overestimated and sometimes underestimated the level of suggestibility observed in empirical studies. Individual differences in accuracy were related to participants' gender, education and ethnicity, and there was considerable disagreement among participants on many questions. Implications of findings for the admissibility of expert testimony in child abuse cases are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jodi A Quas
- Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-7085, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
74
|
Roebers CM, Schwarz S, Neumann R. Social influence and children's event recall and suggestibility. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1080/17405620444000274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
75
|
Cordón IM, Saetermoe CL, Goodman GS. Facilitating children's accurate responses: conversational rules and interview style. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1090] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
76
|
Herman S. Improving decision making in forensic child sexual abuse evaluations. LAW AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2005; 29:87-120. [PMID: 15865333 DOI: 10.1007/s10979-005-1400-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/02/2023]
Abstract
Mental health professionals can assist legal decision makers in cases of allegations of child sexual abuse by collecting data using forensic interviews, psychological testing, and record reviews, and by summarizing relevant findings from social science research. Significant controversy surrounds another key task performed by mental health professionals in most child sexual abuse evaluations, i.e., deciding whether or not to substantiate unconfirmed abuse allegations. The available evidence indicates that, on the whole, these substantiation decisions currently lack adequate psychometric reliability and validity: an analysis of empirical research findings leads to the conclusion that at least 24% of all of these decisions are either false positive or false negative errors. Surprisingly, a reanalysis of existing research also indicates that it may be possible to develop reliable, objective procedures to improve the consistency and quality of decision making in this domain. A preliminary, empirically-grounded procedure for making substantiation decisions is proposed.
Collapse
|
77
|
Abstract
The scientific study of child witnesses has influenced both developmental science and jurisprudence concerning children. Focusing on the author's own studies, 4 categories of research are briefly reviewed: (a) children's eyewitness memory and suggestibility; (b) memory for traumatic events in childhood; (c) disclosure of child sexual abuse; and (d) experiences of child victim/witnesses within the legal system. Implications for psychology and for legal practice are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gail S Goodman
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Thomsen Y, Berntsen D. Knowing that I didn't know: preschoolers' understanding of their own false belief is a predictor of assents to fictitious events. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
79
|
|
80
|
Thierry KL, Lamb ME, Orbach Y, Pipe ME. Developmental Differences in the Function and Use of Anatomical Dolls During Interviews With Alleged Sexual Abuse Victims. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005; 73:1125-34. [PMID: 16392985 DOI: 10.1037/0022-006x.73.6.1125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The impact of anatomical dolls on reports provided by 3- to 12-year-old alleged sexual abuse victims (N = 178) was examined. Children produced as many details in response to open-ended invitations with and without the dolls. In response to directive questions, the 3- to 6-year-olds were more likely to re-enact behaviorally than to report verbally, whereas the 7- to 12-year-olds produced more verbal details than enactments when using the dolls. With the dolls, the younger children were more likely than the older children to play suggestively and to contradict details provided without the dolls, whereas the older children were more likely to provide details that were consistent. Children in both age groups produced proportionally more fantastic details with the dolls than without the dolls.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen L Thierry
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
81
|
Schreiber N, Parker JF. Inviting witnesses to speculate: effects of age and interaction on children's recall. J Exp Child Psychol 2004; 89:31-52. [PMID: 15336917 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2003] [Revised: 03/23/2004] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Inviting speculation has been found to increase children's false recall. In this study, kindergartners and third graders saw a clown perform actions alone or in interaction with a child. Two weeks later, the speculation group recalled all actions and was asked to speculate on half the actions. The control group recalled all actions without speculating. Four weeks after the show, all children recalled all actions again. The speculation group gave more false answers to the speculated items than the control group. Surprisingly, older children tended to report as many if not more false responses than younger children, regardless of speculation. In the speculation group, there were fewer false answers for interactions than actions, but false answers did not differ across observation types in the control group. Finally, speculation did not affect free and cued recall differentially.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nadja Schreiber
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
82
|
Miles KL, Powell MB, Stokes MA. A comparison of the effectiveness of two suggestibility paradigms in predicting preschoolers' tendency to report a non-experienced event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
83
|
Gilstrap LL. A Missing Link in Suggestibility Research: What Is Known About the Behavior of Field Interviewers in Unstructured Interviews With Young Children? ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2004; 10:13-24. [PMID: 15053699 DOI: 10.1037/1076-898x.10.1.13] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Despite suggestibility researchers' focus on adult behaviors that distort children's reports, whether behaviors examined in experimental work are used in the field is unknown. The current study presents a mutually exclusive and exhaustive hierarchical coding system that reflects interview questioning behaviors of concern in experimental work. The study examined 80 unstructured interviews conducted by 41 field interviewers with 40 children ages 3 to 7 about known events. Data on the use of leading and neutral questions are presented and include distinctions between accurate and inaccurate suggested information. In addition, analyses show that interviewers are consistent in their style of questioning and that a preinterview measure of interviewers' preference for a qualitative versus a quantitative interviewing style predicted the introduction of novel information into the interview.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Livia L Gilstrap
- University of Colorado at Colorado Springs, Colorado Springs, CO, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
84
|
Erdmann K, Volbert R, Böhm C. Children report suggested events even when interviewed in a non-suggestive manner: what are its implications for credibility assessment? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
85
|
Ceci SJ. Cast in Six Ponds and You'll Reel in Something: Looking Back on 25 Years of Research. AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2003; 58:855-864. [PMID: 14609372 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.58.11.855] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The author describes his program of research over the past 25 years. This research falls into 6 areas that are interdependent and that inform each other. The overall program is guided by 3 bioecological principles that posit the need for proximal processes and motivation to actualize biological potential. The author presents examples of experiments that fall into each of the 6 areas and show that human potential is highly contextualized and that, consequently, the same person who fails at a task in one domain is often able to succeed at it in a different domain.
Collapse
|
86
|
Finnilä K, Mahlberg N, Santtila P, Sandnabba K, Niemi P. Validity of a test of children's suggestibility for predicting responses to two interview situations differing in their degree of suggestiveness. J Exp Child Psychol 2003; 85:32-49. [PMID: 12742761 DOI: 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00025-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
In the present study the relative contributions of internal and external sources of variation in children's suggestibility in interrogative situations were examined. One hundred and eleven children (48 4- to 5-year-olds and 63 7- to 8-year-olds) were administered a suggestibility test (BTSS) and the most suggestible (N=36) and the least suggestible (N=36) children were randomly assigned to either an interview condition containing several suggestive techniques or to one containing only suggestive questions. The effects of internal sources of variation in suggestibility were compared with the effects of the interview styles on the children's answers. The former did influence the children, but the external sources of variation in suggestibility had a stronger impact. Influences of cognitive, developmental factors could be found, but not when abuse-related questions were asked and high pressured interview methods were used. These findings indicate that individual assessment of suggestibility can be of some assistance when interviewing children, but diminishing suggestive influences in interrogations must be given priority.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katarina Finnilä
- Department of Psychology, Abo Akademi University, The Police College of Finland, Laivurinkatu 37 D 22, Helsinki 00150, Finland.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
87
|
Abstract
Abstract
Theories regarding children's reliability as witnesses suggest that children are more likely to confuse memories from different sources especially when the sources are highly similar. To investigate the developmental aspects of source retrieval, we measured brain electrical activity from children and adults while they retrieved content and source information. Similar brain responses among the age groups were found when participants were asked to retrieve content information. However, retrieval of source information improved with age and was accompanied by different patterns of brain potentials. The results implicate immaturity of frontal lobe structures in children's difficulty in retrieving source information.
Collapse
|
88
|
Lindberg MA, Chapman MT, Samsock D, Thomas SW, Lindberg AW. Comparisons of three different investigative interview techniques with young children. J Genet Psychol 2003; 164:5-28. [PMID: 12693741 DOI: 10.1080/00221320309597500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
After viewing a film of a mother hitting her son, a film not seen by the college student interviewers, children were misinformed about a detail (via exposure to a misleading question) as well as explicitly coached to disclose 3 false details. The children were then interviewed by interviewers who had previously learned 1 of 3 different interviewing procedures: the Yuille Step-Wise Interview developed by J. C. Yuille, R. Hunter, R. Joffe, & J. Zapamiuk (1993); a doll play interview developed by Action for Child Protection Inc. (1994); or the Modified Structured Interview developed for this study. The Modified Structured Interview yielded more "where" information and was better at detecting if coaching had occurred. However, the interviewers were not very good at discriminating suggested versus coached versus correct witnessed information. The authors found that the deeper one digs for memories, the more one uncovers incorrect versus correct items. They concluded that although the Modified Structured Interview was superior to the techniques currently in use, cautions are necessary.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc A Lindberg
- Department of Psychology, Marshall University, Huntington, WV 25755, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Strange D, Garry M, Sutherland R. Drawing out children's false memories. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2003. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
90
|
Powell MB, Jones CH, Campbell C. A comparison of preschoolers' recall of experienced versus non-experienced events across multiple interviews. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2003. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.932] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
91
|
Quas JA, Schaaf JM. Children's memories of experienced and nonexperienced events following repeated interviews. J Exp Child Psychol 2002; 83:304-38. [PMID: 12470963 DOI: 10.1016/s0022-0965(02)00150-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
The present study compared 3- and 5-year-olds' reports of a true or false play interaction following repeated interviews. Final interviews were conducted either by the same researcher or by a new researcher. Age-related improvements in performance were evident. Also, 3-year-olds questioned repeatedly about an entirely false event made more errors in response to specific questions than 3-year-olds questioned repeatedly about false details of a true event. Five-year-olds who were questioned about the false event, however, were particularly accurate when answering questions about never-experienced body touch. Interviewer familiarity was associated with decreases in the amount of narrative detail 5-year-olds provided in free-recall and with increases in 3-year-olds' accuracy in response to direct questions. Both errors and response latency on a cognitive matching task were related to children's suggestibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jodi A Quas
- Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, 3340 Social Ecology II, Irvine, CA 92697-7085, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
92
|
Abstract
Source-monitoring decision processes were manipulated during retrieval while event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. Words were either seen or heard at study, and memory for modality was measured on two separate yes-no source tests. Decision processes were varied across the two tests by asking participants to respond to leading questions. One leading question asked if the items were seen at study, whereas the second question asked if the items were heard at study (cf., Marsh & Hicks, 1998). Behavioral responses indicated that leading questions altered the way in which memory was evaluated to determine the source of information. Varying the decision processes affected frontal--but not parietal ERPs--indicating that frontal ERPs reflect processing that is used to evaluate activated information. Furthermore, left and right frontal ERP activity was affected by the combination of test query and type of source supporting the hypothesis that both the right and left frontal lobes contribute to memory retrieval processes. The pattern of frontal ERP effects supports the hypothesis that activation in right frontal areas reflect basic decision processes that are used to determine source and that the left frontal lobes are recruited when more systematic processing is required by the test context (cf., Nolde, Johnson, & Raye, 1998b).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Andrew Leynes
- Department of Psychology, The College of New Jersey, Ewing, NJ 08628, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
93
|
Esser G, Steigleider P, Lange S, Ihle W, Blanz B, Schmidt MH. Die Validität des autobiographischen Gedächtnisses. KINDHEIT UND ENTWICKLUNG 2002. [DOI: 10.1026//0942-5403.11.4.228] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Die Validität des autobiographischen Gedächtnisses wird kontrovers diskutiert, bislang fehlen prospektive Längsschnittstudien weitgehend. Die vorliegende Studie überprüft die Validität des autobiographischen Gedächtnisses anhand der Daten einer prospektiven epidemiologischen Längsschnittstudie, die in vier Untersuchungswellen 399 achtjährige Kinder bis zum Alter von 25 Jahren begleitete. Im Alter von 25 Jahren wurden die Erinnerungsleistungen der Probanden an Kernfakten, Erziehungsstil der Eltern, eigene psychische Probleme sowie Lebensereignisse aus Kindheit und Jugend mit Hilfe eines voll strukturierten Fragebogens erhoben. Mit Ausnahme der Kernfakten waren die Erinnerungsleistungen durchweg schlecht, intelligente Probanden zeigten insgesamt bessere Erinnerungsleistungen, junge Erwachsene mit psychischen Störungen berichteten vermehrt auch früher nicht vorhandene Symptome. Die retrospektive Erfassung früherer Lebensereignisse, der Beziehung zu den Eltern und psychischer Auffälligkeiten im Rahmen von Risikostudien und klinischen Studien ist sehr bedenklich.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Günter Esser
- Institut für Psychologie und Akademie für Psychotherapie und Interventionsforschung der Universität Potsdam
| | - Petra Steigleider
- Zentralinstitut für Seelische Gesundheit, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters in Mannheim
| | - Sabine Lange
- Institut für Psychologie und Akademie für Psychotherapie und Interventionsforschung der Universität Potsdam
| | - Wolfgang Ihle
- Institut für Psychologie und Akademie für Psychotherapie und Interventionsforschung der Universität Potsdam
| | - Bernd Blanz
- Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrische Klinik der Universität Jena
| | - Martin H. Schmidt
- Zentralinstitut für Seelische Gesundheit, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters in Mannheim
| |
Collapse
|
94
|
Principe GF, Ceci SJ. "I saw it with my own ears": the effects of peer conversations on preschoolers' reports of nonexperienced events. J Exp Child Psychol 2002; 83:1-25. [PMID: 12379416 DOI: 10.1016/s0022-0965(02)00120-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The study was designed to explore the effects of naturally occurring peer interactions and repeated suggestive interviews on preschoolers' (N=96, Meanage=54 months) memories for a personally experienced event, namely a staged archaeological dig. During the dig, one third of the children witnessed two "target" activities. A second third of the children were the classmates of those in the first group, but did not witness the target activities. The remaining children were not the classmates of those who witnessed the target activities, nor did they witness the target activities themselves, and thus served to provide a baseline against which to assess the effects of peer contact. Following the dig, the children were interviewed in either a neutral or suggestive manner on three occasions. Results from a fourth interview by a new examiner revealed that the combination of suggestive interviews and peer exposure led to claims of witnessing the target activities by the classmate group that were comparable to the children who actually did witness these activities. Further, assent rates to misleading questions employing peer pressure and false claims of actually seeing versus merely hearing about the target activities were elevated following opportunities to discuss these activities with peers.
Collapse
|
95
|
|
96
|
|
97
|
Roberts KP. Children’s ability to distinguish between memories from multiple sources: Implications for the quality and accuracy of eyewitness statements. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2002. [DOI: 10.1016/s0273-2297(02)00005-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
98
|
Goodman GS, Batterman-Faunce JM, Schaaf JM, Kenney R. Nearly 4 years after an event: children's eyewitness memory and adults' perceptions of children's accuracy. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2002; 26:849-884. [PMID: 12363335 DOI: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00354-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The current study examined children's eyewitness memory nearly 4 years after an event and the ability of adults to evaluate such memory. METHOD In Phase 1, 7- and 10-year olds were interviewed about a past event after a nearly 4-year delay. The interview included leading questions relevant to child abuse as well as statements designed to implicate the original confederate. In Phase 2, laypersons and professionals watched a videotaped interview (from Phase 1) that they were misled to believe was from an ongoing abuse investigation. Respondents then rated the child's accuracy and credibility, and the probability that the child had been abused. RESULTS In Phase 1, few significant age differences in memory accuracy were found, perhaps owing in part to small sample size. Although children made a variety of commission errors, none claimed outright to have been abused. Nevertheless, some of the children's answers (e.g., saying that their picture had been taken, or that they had been in a bathtub) might cause concern in a forensic setting. In Phase 2, professional and nonprofessional respondents were unable to reliably estimate the overall accuracy of children's statements. However, respondents were able to reasonably estimate the accuracy of children's answers to abuse questions. Respondents were also more likely to think that 7-year olds compared to 10-year olds had been abused. Professionals were significantly less likely than nonprofessionals to believe that credible evidence of abuse existed. Professionals who indicated personal experience with child abuse or a close relationship with an abuse victim were more likely to rate children as abused. A gender bias to rate boys as more accurate than girls was apparent among laypersons but not professionals. CONCLUSIONS Children were generally resistant to suggestions that abuse occurred during a long-ago generally forgotten event, but some potentially concerning errors were made. Both professionals and non-professionals had difficulty estimating the accuracy of children's reports, but adults were more likely to rate children as accurate if the children answered abuse-related questions correctly. Training and personal experience were associated with adults' ratings of children's reports. Implications for evaluations of child abuse reports are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gail S Goodman
- Department of Psychology, University of California-Davis, 95616, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Epstein MA, Bottoms BL. Explaining the forgetting and recovery of abuse and trauma memories: possible mechanisms. CHILD MALTREATMENT 2002; 7:210-225. [PMID: 12139189 DOI: 10.1177/1077559502007003004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Much attention has been focused on memories of abuse that are allegedly forgotten or repressed then recovered. By retrospectively surveying more than 1,400 college women, the authors investigated (a) the frequency with which temporary forgetting is reported for child sexual abuse experiences as opposed to other childhood abuse and traumas and (b) exactly how victims characterize their forgetting experiences in terms of various competing cognitive mechanisms. Rates of forgetting were similar among victims who experienced sexual abuse, physical abuse, and multiple types of traumas. Victims of other types of childhood traumas (e.g., car accidents) reported less forgetting than victims of childhood sexual abuse or multiple types of trauma. Most victims' characterizations of their forgetting experiences were not indicative of repression in the classic Freudian sense but instead suggested other more common mechanisms, such as directed forgetting and relabeling. The implications of these findings for psychological theory, clinical practice, and law are discussed.
Collapse
|
100
|
|