51
|
Graham NL, Leonard C, Tang-Wai DF, Black S, Chow TW, Scott CJM, McNeely AA, Masellis M, Rochon E. Lack of Frank Agrammatism in the Nonfluent Agrammatic Variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra 2016; 6:407-423. [PMID: 27790240 PMCID: PMC5075721 DOI: 10.1159/000448944] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Background/Aims Frank agrammatism, defined as the omission and/or substitution of grammatical morphemes with associated grammatical errors, is variably reported in patients with nonfluent variant primary progressive aphasia (nfPPA). This study addressed whether frank agrammatism is typical in agrammatic nfPPA patients when this feature is not required for diagnosis. Method We assessed grammatical production in 9 patients who satisfied current diagnostic criteria. Although the focus was agrammatism, motor speech skills were also evaluated to determine whether dysfluency arose primarily from apraxia of speech (AOS), instead of, or in addition to, agrammatism. Volumetric MRI analyses provided impartial imaging-supported diagnosis. Results The majority of cases exhibited neither frank agrammatism nor AOS. Conclusion There are nfPPA patients with imaging-supported diagnosis and preserved motor speech skills who do not exhibit frank agrammatism, and this may persist beyond the earliest stages of the illness. Because absence of frank agrammatism is a subsidiary diagnostic feature in the logopenic variant of PPA, this result has implications for differentiation of the nonfluent and logopenic variants, and indicates that PPA patients with nonfluent speech in the absence of frank agrammatism or AOS do not necessarily have the logopenic variant.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Naida L Graham
- Department of Speech-Language Pathology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Ont., Canada; Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, Ont, Canada
| | - Carol Leonard
- Department of Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ont, Canada
| | - David F Tang-Wai
- University Health Network Memory Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, Ont., Canada; Department of Medicine (Neurology), University of Toronto, Ont., Canada
| | - Sandra Black
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), University of Toronto, Ont., Canada; L.C. Campbell Cognitive Neurology Research Unit, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Ont., Canada; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ont., Canada
| | - Tiffany W Chow
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), University of Toronto, Ont., Canada; Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ont., Canada; Department of Psychiatry (Geriatric Psychiatry), University of Toronto, Toronto, Ont., Canada
| | - Chris J M Scott
- L.C. Campbell Cognitive Neurology Research Unit, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Ont., Canada
| | - Alicia A McNeely
- L.C. Campbell Cognitive Neurology Research Unit, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Ont., Canada
| | - Mario Masellis
- L.C. Campbell Cognitive Neurology Research Unit, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Ont., Canada
| | - Elizabeth Rochon
- Department of Speech-Language Pathology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Ont., Canada; Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, Ont, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Woollacott IOC, Rohrer JD. The clinical spectrum of sporadic and familial forms of frontotemporal dementia. J Neurochem 2016; 138 Suppl 1:6-31. [PMID: 27144467 DOI: 10.1111/jnc.13654] [Citation(s) in RCA: 89] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2016] [Revised: 04/10/2016] [Accepted: 04/27/2016] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
The term frontotemporal dementia (FTD) describes a clinically, genetically and pathologically diverse group of neurodegenerative disorders. Symptoms of FTD can present in individuals in their 20s through to their 90s, but the mean age at onset is in the sixth decade. The most common presentation is with a change in personality and impaired social conduct (behavioural variant FTD). Less frequently patients present with language problems (primary progressive aphasia). Both of these groups of patients can develop motor features consistent with either motor neuron disease (usually the amyotrophic lateral sclerosis variant) or parkinsonism (most commonly a progressive supranuclear palsy or corticobasal syndrome). In about a third of cases FTD is familial, with mutations in the progranulin, microtubule-associated protein tau and chromosome 9 open reading frame 72 genes being the major causes. Mutations in a number of other genes including TANK-binding kinase 1 are rare causes of familial FTD. This review aims to clarify the often confusing terminology of FTD, and outline the various clinical features and diagnostic criteria of sporadic and familial FTD syndromes. It will also discuss the current major challenges in FTD research and clinical practice, and potential areas for future research. This review clarifies the terminology of frontotemporal dementia (FTD) and summarizes the various clinical features and most recent diagnostic criteria of sporadic and familial FTD syndromes. It also discusses the current major challenges in FTD research and clinical practice, and highlights potential areas for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ione O C Woollacott
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK
| | - Jonathan D Rohrer
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Beales A, Cartwright J, Whitworth A, Panegyres PK. Exploring generalisation processes following lexical retrieval intervention in primary progressive aphasia. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2016; 18:299-314. [PMID: 27063691 DOI: 10.3109/17549507.2016.1151936] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2015] [Revised: 01/26/2016] [Accepted: 02/04/2016] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Within the current literature, positive intervention effects demonstrate the significant potential for people with Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA) to learn/relearn words. Generalisation of intervention effects to other words and/or other contexts, however, remains unclear. METHOD This multiple baseline, case-series design investigated the effects of a self-cueing lexical retrieval intervention across word classes (nouns, verbs and adjectives) on four individuals with PPA, three suggestive of the semantic variant and one of the logopenic variant. The intervention integrated semantic, phonological and orthographic levels of language production and drew on autobiographical memory. Changes in accuracy in retrieving treated and untreated items (pre-intervention, post-intervention and 4-weeks maintenance) were determined using the Cochran's Q test, with follow-up McNemar pairwise comparisons. RESULT All participants showed significant improvements in naming treated items, across all word classes. Different patterns of generalised improvement to untreated words were found for each participant. In discourse, the semantic variant participants demonstrated a significant increase in correct information units, in contrast to the participant with the logopenic variant who remained stable. CONCLUSION This study provides evidence that people with PPA can show improved lexical retrieval following intervention. The findings suggest possible differences in generalisation across word classes and according to underlying deficit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ashleigh Beales
- a School of Psychology and Speech Pathology , Curtin University , Perth , Western Australia , Australia
| | - Jade Cartwright
- b Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology , University of Melbourne , Melbourne , Victoria
| | - Anne Whitworth
- a School of Psychology and Speech Pathology , Curtin University , Perth , Western Australia , Australia
| | - Peter K Panegyres
- c Neurodegenerative Disorders Research , West Perth , Western Australia , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Grey Matter Density Predicts the Improvement of Naming Abilities After tDCS Intervention in Agrammatic Variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia. Brain Topogr 2016; 29:738-51. [DOI: 10.1007/s10548-016-0494-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2015] [Accepted: 05/07/2016] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
55
|
|
56
|
Stålhammar J, Rydén I, Nordlund A, Wallin A. Boston Naming Test automatic credits inflate scores of nonaphasic mild dementia patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2015; 38:381-92. [DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1119254] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
57
|
Vigliecca NS, Báez S. Verbal Neuropsychological Functions in Aphasia: An Integrative Model. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2015; 44:715-732. [PMID: 25168953 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-014-9316-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
A theoretical framework which considers the verbal functions of the brain under a multivariate and comprehensive cognitive model was statistically analyzed. A confirmatory factor analysis was performed to verify whether some recognized aphasia constructs can be hierarchically integrated as latent factors from a homogenously verbal test. The Brief Aphasia Evaluation was used. A sample of 65 patients with left cerebral lesions, and two supplementary samples comprising 35 patients with right cerebral lesions and 30 healthy participants were studied. A model encompassing an all inclusive verbal organizer and two successive organizers was validated. The two last organizers were: three factors of comprehension, expression and a "complementary" verbal factor which included praxia, attention, and memory; followed by the individual (and correlated) factors of auditory comprehension, repetition, naming, speech, reading, writing, and the "complementary" factor. By following this approach all the patients fall inside the classification system; consequently, theoretical improvement is guaranteed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nora Silvana Vigliecca
- Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Instituto de Humanidades (IDH-CONICET), Servicio de Neurología y Neurocirugía del Hospital Córdoba, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina.
- , Luciano de Figueroa 414, Bº Marqués de Sobremonte, CP: 5008, Córdoba, Argentina.
| | - Sandra Báez
- Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva (INECO) Instituto de Neurociencias, Universidad Favaloro, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
58
|
Sitek EJ, Barczak A, Kluj-Kozłowska K, Kozłowski M, Barcikowska M, Sławek J. Is descriptive writing useful in the differential diagnosis of logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia, Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment? Neurol Neurochir Pol 2015; 49:239-44. [PMID: 26188940 DOI: 10.1016/j.pjnns.2015.06.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2015] [Revised: 06/05/2015] [Accepted: 06/08/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Current classification of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) encompasses three variants: non-fluent (nfvPPA), semantic (svPPA) and logopenic (lvPPA). Previously lvPPA was regarded as aphasic form of Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, not all patients with lvPPA phenotype present with AD pathology. Despite abundant literature on differentiation of lvPPA from svPPA and nfvPPA, studies comparing lvPPA with AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) are scarce. This study aimed at analyzing written descriptive output in lvPPA, AD and MCI. Thirty-five patients participated in the study: 9 with lvPPA, 13 with AD and 13 with MCI. Most aspects of writing performance were comparable in three groups. However, letter insertion errors appeared in 44% patients with lvPPA, while they were absent in AD and MCI. Patients with lvPPA used more verbs than patients with AD. Writing profile may complement other neuropsychological assessment results in the differential diagnosis of lvPPA. Letter insertion errors and frequent verb use may raise a query of lvPPA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emilia J Sitek
- Neurology Department, St. Adalbert Hospital, Copernicus Podmiot Leczniczy Sp. z o.o., Gdansk, Poland; Department of Neurological and Psychiatric Nursing, Medical University of Gdansk, Gdansk, Poland.
| | - Anna Barczak
- Neurology Department, Central Clinical Hospital of the Ministry of Interior, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Klaudia Kluj-Kozłowska
- Neurology Department, St. Adalbert Hospital, Copernicus Podmiot Leczniczy Sp. z o.o., Gdansk, Poland; Speech Therapy Department, Faculty of Languages, University of Gdansk, Gdansk, Poland
| | - Marcin Kozłowski
- Speech Therapy Department, Faculty of Languages, University of Gdansk, Gdansk, Poland; Neurological Rehabilitation Department, Specialist Hospital in Koscierzyna, Dzierzazno, Poland
| | - Maria Barcikowska
- Neurodegenerative Disorders Department, Mossakowski Medical Research Center, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Jarosław Sławek
- Neurology Department, St. Adalbert Hospital, Copernicus Podmiot Leczniczy Sp. z o.o., Gdansk, Poland; Department of Neurological and Psychiatric Nursing, Medical University of Gdansk, Gdansk, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Mesulam MM, Thompson CK, Weintraub S, Rogalski EJ. The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. Brain 2015; 138:2423-37. [PMID: 26112340 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awv154] [Citation(s) in RCA: 138] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2015] [Accepted: 04/14/2015] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Wernicke's aphasia is characterized by severe word and sentence comprehension impairments. The location of the underlying lesion site, known as Wernicke's area, remains controversial. Questions related to this controversy were addressed in 72 patients with primary progressive aphasia who collectively displayed a wide spectrum of cortical atrophy sites and language impairment patterns. Clinico-anatomical correlations were explored at the individual and group levels. These analyses showed that neuronal loss in temporoparietal areas, traditionally included within Wernicke's area, leave single word comprehension intact and cause inconsistent impairments of sentence comprehension. The most severe sentence comprehension impairments were associated with a heterogeneous set of cortical atrophy sites variably encompassing temporoparietal components of Wernicke's area, Broca's area, and dorsal premotor cortex. Severe comprehension impairments for single words, on the other hand, were invariably associated with peak atrophy sites in the left temporal pole and adjacent anterior temporal cortex, a pattern of atrophy that left sentence comprehension intact. These results show that the neural substrates of word and sentence comprehension are dissociable and that a circumscribed cortical area equally critical for word and sentence comprehension is unlikely to exist anywhere in the cerebral cortex. Reports of combined word and sentence comprehension impairments in Wernicke's aphasia come almost exclusively from patients with cerebrovascular accidents where brain damage extends into subcortical white matter. The syndrome of Wernicke's aphasia is thus likely to reflect damage not only to the cerebral cortex but also to underlying axonal pathways, leading to strategic cortico-cortical disconnections within the language network. The results of this investigation further reinforce the conclusion that the left anterior temporal lobe, a region ignored by classic aphasiology, needs to be inserted into the language network with a critical role in the multisynaptic hierarchy underlying word comprehension and object naming.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M-Marsel Mesulam
- 1 Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA 2 Department of Neurology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA 3 Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA
| | - Cynthia K Thompson
- 1 Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA 4 Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA
| | - Sandra Weintraub
- 1 Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA 5 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA
| | - Emily J Rogalski
- 1 Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Caine D, Tinelli RJ, Hyare H, De Vita E, Lowe J, Lukic A, Thompson A, Porter MC, Cipolotti L, Rudge P, Collinge J, Mead S. The cognitive profile of prion disease: a prospective clinical and imaging study. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2015; 2:548-58. [PMID: 26000326 PMCID: PMC4435708 DOI: 10.1002/acn3.195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2015] [Accepted: 02/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Objectives Prion diseases are dementing illnesses with poorly defined neuropsychological features. This is probably because the most common form, sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, is often rapidly progressive with pervasive cognitive decline making detailed neuropsychological investigation difficult. This study, which includes patients with inherited, acquired (iatrogenic and variant) and sporadic forms of the disease, is the only large-scale neuropsychological investigation of this patient group ever undertaken and aimed to define a neuropsychological profile of human prion diseases. Methods A tailored short cognitive examination of all of the patients (n = 81), with detailed neuropsychological testing in a subset with mild disease (n = 30) and correlation with demographic, clinical, genetic (PRNP mutation and polymorphic codon 129 genotype), and other variables (MRI brain signal change in cortex, basal ganglia or thalamus; quantitative research imaging, cerebrospinal fluid 14-3-3 protein). Results Comparison with healthy controls showed patients to be impaired on all tasks. Principal components analysis showed a major axis of fronto-parietal dysfunction that accounted for approximately half of the variance observed. This correlated strongly with volume reduction in frontal and parietal gray matter on MRI. Examination of individual patients' performances confirmed early impairment on this axis, suggesting characteristic cognitive features in mild disease: prominent executive impairment, parietal dysfunction, a largely expressive dysphasia, with reduced motor speed. Interpretation Taken together with typical neurological features, these results complete a profile that should improve differential diagnosis in a clinical setting. We propose a tailored neuropsychological battery for early recognition of clinical onset of symptoms with potential for use in clinical trials involving at-risk individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Diana Caine
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; Department of Neuropsychology, NHNN, University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - Renata J Tinelli
- Department of Neuropsychology, NHNN, University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom
| | - Harpreet Hyare
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - Enrico De Vita
- Academic Neuroradiological Unit, Department of Brain Repair and Rehabilitation, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom ; Lysholm Department of Neuroradiology, NHNN, University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom
| | - Jessica Lowe
- Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - Ana Lukic
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - Andrew Thompson
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - Marie-Claire Porter
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - Lisa Cipolotti
- Department of Neuropsychology, NHNN, University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom
| | - Peter Rudge
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - John Collinge
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| | - Simon Mead
- NHS National Prion Clinic, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (NHNN), University College London Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust London, United Kingdom ; MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
61
|
Katsumata Y, Mathews M, Abner EL, Jicha GA, Caban-Holt A, Smith CD, Nelson PT, Kryscio RJ, Schmitt FA, Fardo DW. Assessing the discriminant ability, reliability, and comparability of multiple short forms of the Boston Naming Test in an Alzheimer's disease center cohort. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2015; 39:215-27. [PMID: 25613081 PMCID: PMC4374652 DOI: 10.1159/000370108] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/21/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The Boston Naming Test (BNT) is a commonly used neuropsychological test of confrontation naming that aids in determining the presence and severity of dysnomia. Many short versions of the original 60-item test have been developed and are routinely administered in clinical/research settings. Because of the common need to translate similar measures within and across studies, it is important to evaluate the operating characteristics and agreement of different BNT versions. METHODS We analyzed longitudinal data of research volunteers (n = 681) from the University of Kentucky Alzheimer's Disease Center longitudinal cohort. CONCLUSIONS With the notable exception of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) 15-item BNT, short forms were internally consistent and highly correlated with the full version; these measures varied by diagnosis and generally improved from normal to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to dementia. All short forms retained the ability to discriminate between normal subjects and those with dementia. The ability to discriminate between normal and MCI subjects was less strong for the short forms than the full BNT, but they exhibited similar patterns. These results have important implications for researchers designing longitudinal studies, who must consider that the statistical properties of even closely related test forms may be quite different.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuriko Katsumata
- Department of Biostatistics, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Melissa Mathews
- Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Erin L. Abner
- Department of Epidemiology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Gregory A. Jicha
- Department of Neurology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Allison Caban-Holt
- Department of Behavioral Science, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Charles D. Smith
- Department of Neurology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Peter T. Nelson
- Department of Pathology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Richard J. Kryscio
- Department of Biostatistics, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Department of Statistics, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - Frederick A. Schmitt
- Department of Neurology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Department of Behavioral Science, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| | - David W. Fardo
- Department of Biostatistics, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
,Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Ky., USA
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Object and proper name retrieval in temporal lobe epilepsy: a study of difficulties and latencies. Epilepsy Res 2014; 108:1825-38. [PMID: 25277884 DOI: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2014.09.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2013] [Revised: 08/19/2014] [Accepted: 09/06/2014] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE Retrieving a specific name is sometimes difficult and can be even harder when pathology affects the temporal lobes. Word finding difficulties have been well documented in temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) but analyses have mostly concentrated on the study of accuracy. Our aim here was to go beyond simple accuracy and to provide both a quantitative and a qualitative assessment of naming difficulties and latencies in patients with TLE. METHODS Thirty-two patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (16 with epilepsy affecting the cerebral hemisphere dominant for language (D-TLE) and 16 with epilepsy affecting the cerebral hemisphere non-dominant for language (ND-TLE)) and 34 healthy matched control subjects were included in the study. The experiment involved naming 70 photographs of objects and 70 photographs of celebrities as fast as possible. Accuracy and naming reaction times were recorded. Following each trial, a questionnaire was used to determine the specific nature of each subject's difficulty in retrieving the name (e.g., no difficulty, paraphasia, tip of the tongue, feeling of knowing the name, etc). Reaction times were analysed both across subjects and across trials. KEY FINDINGS D-TLE patients showed consistent and quasi-systematic impairment compared to matched control subjects on both object and famous people naming. This impairment was characterized not only by lower accuracy but also by more qualitative errors and tip of the tongue phenomena. Furthermore, minimum reaction times were slowed down by about 70 ms for objects and 150 ms for famous people naming. In contrast, patients with ND-TLE were less impaired, and their impairment was limited to object naming. SIGNIFICANCE These results suggest that patients with TLE, in particular D-TLE, show a general impairment of lexical access. Furthermore, there was evidence of subtle difficulties (increased reaction times) in patients with TLE.
Collapse
|
63
|
Mesulam MM, Rogalski EJ, Wieneke C, Hurley RS, Geula C, Bigio EH, Thompson CK, Weintraub S. Primary progressive aphasia and the evolving neurology of the language network. Nat Rev Neurol 2014; 10:554-69. [PMID: 25179257 PMCID: PMC4201050 DOI: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 221] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is caused by selective neurodegeneration of the language-dominant cerebral hemisphere; a language deficit initially arises as the only consequential impairment and remains predominant throughout most of the course of the disease. Agrammatic, logopenic and semantic subtypes, each reflecting a characteristic pattern of language impairment and corresponding anatomical distribution of cortical atrophy, represent the most frequent presentations of PPA. Such associations between clinical features and the sites of atrophy have provided new insights into the neurology of fluency, grammar, word retrieval, and word comprehension, and have necessitated modification of concepts related to the functions of the anterior temporal lobe and Wernicke's area. The underlying neuropathology of PPA is, most commonly, frontotemporal lobar degeneration in the agrammatic and semantic forms, and Alzheimer disease (AD) pathology in the logopenic form; the AD pathology often displays atypical and asymmetrical anatomical features consistent with the aphasic phenotype. The PPA syndrome reflects complex interactions between disease-specific neuropathological features and patient-specific vulnerability. A better understanding of these interactions might help us to elucidate the biology of the language network and the principles of selective vulnerability in neurodegenerative diseases. We review these aspects of PPA, focusing on advances in our understanding of the clinical features and neuropathology of PPA and what they have taught us about the neural substrates of the language network.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M-Marsel Mesulam
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, 320 East Superior Street, Searle Building, 11-450, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - Emily J Rogalski
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, 320 East Superior Street, Searle Building, 11-450, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - Christina Wieneke
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, 320 East Superior Street, Searle Building, 11-450, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - Robert S Hurley
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, 320 East Superior Street, Searle Building, 11-450, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - Changiz Geula
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, 320 East Superior Street, Searle Building, 11-450, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - Eileen H Bigio
- Department of Neuropathology, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, 710 North Fairbanks Court, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - Cynthia K Thompson
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, 633 Clark Street, Evanston, IL 60208, USA
| | - Sandra Weintraub
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, 320 East Superior Street, Searle Building, 11-450, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Abstract
The Boston Naming Test (BNT) (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983) is the most commonly used test of confrontation naming in neuropsychology (Rabin, Barr, & Burton, 2005). However, there are significant criticisms of the BNT which suggest that it might not be the assessment measure of choice. These criticisms are that the BNT has poor psychometric properties, is not adequately standardized, and has inadequate norms. It is further suggested that when considered in the context of contemporary conceptualizations of the neuropsychology of naming, the BNT does not adequately capture the processes known to be required for successful naming, and does not sample widely enough from the content domain of "naming". These criticisms suggest that the BNT is flawed as a measure of naming, and are discussed in detail in this review. Other stand-alone visual confrontation naming tasks are reviewed to evaluate whether any might be viable substitutes for the BNT in clinical neuropsychology. The Naming Test from the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (Stern & White, 2009) was identified as a possible alternative to the BNT, however, neither of these tests was designed with reference to models of the neuropsychology of naming, and development of a new test of naming is indicated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Harry
- a School of Psychological Science, Faculty of Science , Technology and Engineering, La Trobe University , Bundoora , Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
65
|
Kuemmel (This author contributed equally to this work.) A, Haberstroh (This author contributed equally to this work.) J, Pantel J. CODEM Instrument. GEROPSYCH-THE JOURNAL OF GERONTOPSYCHOLOGY AND GERIATRIC PSYCHIATRY 2014. [DOI: 10.1024/1662-9647/a000100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Communication and communication behaviors in situational contexts are essential conditions for well-being and quality of life in people with dementia. Measuring methods, however, are limited. The CODEM instrument, a standardized observational communication behavior assessment tool, was developed and evaluated on the basis of the current state of research in dementia care and social-communicative behavior. Initially, interrater reliability was examined by means of videoratings (N = 10 people with dementia). Thereupon, six caregivers in six German nursing homes observed 69 residents suffering from dementia and used CODEM to rate their communication behavior. The interrater reliability of CODEM was excellent (mean κ = .79; intraclass correlation = .91). Statistical analysis indicated that CODEM had excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .95). CODEM also showed excellent convergent validity (Pearson’s R = .88) as well as discriminant validity (Pearson’s R = .63). Confirmatory factor analysis verified the two-factor solution of verbal/content aspects and nonverbal/relationship aspects. With regard to the severity of the disease, the content and relational aspects of communication exhibited different trends. CODEM proved to be a reliable, valid, and sensitive assessment tool for examining communication behavior in the field of dementia. CODEM also provides researchers a feasible examination tool for measuring effects of psychosocial intervention studies that strive to improve communication behavior and well-being in dementia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Johannes Pantel
- Institute of General Practice, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
Gil-Navarro S, Lladó A, Rami L, Castellví M, Bosch B, Bargalló N, Lomeña F, Reñé R, Montagut N, Antonell A, Molinuevo JL, Sánchez-Valle R. Neuroimaging and biochemical markers in the three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2013; 35:106-17. [PMID: 23392204 DOI: 10.1159/000346289] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/01/2012] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND/AIM To investigate in variants of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) the association between current clinical and neuroimaging criteria and biochemical/genetic markers at the individual level. METHODS Thirty-two PPA patients were classified as non-fluent/agrammatic (nfvPPA), semantic (svPPA), or logopenic variant (lvPPA) or as unclassifiable (uPPA). In all patients, we evaluated the neuroimaging criteria (magnetic resonance imaging and/or single photon emission computed tomography/positron emission tomography) of each variant and studied serum progranulin levels, APOE genotype and Alzheimer's disease (AD)-cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers. Cases with a first-degree family history of early-onset dementia were genetically tested. RESULTS Ten of 15 (66%) nfvPPA, 5/5 (100%) svPPA and 7/7 (100%) lvPPA patients showed at least one positive neuroimaging-supported diagnostic criterion. All lvPPA and 3/5 (60%) uPPA patients presented AD-CSF biomarkers, which were absent in nfvPPA and svPPA cases. Four (27%) nfvPPA patients had dementia-causing mutations: 2 carried a GRN mutation and 2 the C9ORF72 hexanucleotide expansion. CONCLUSIONS There was an excellent association between clinical criteria and neuroimaging-supported biomarkers in svPPA and lvPPA, as well as with AD-CSF biochemical markers in the lvPPA. Neuroimaging, biochemical and genetic findings in nfvPPA were heterogeneous. Incorporating biochemical/genetic markers into the PPA clinical diagnosis would allow clinicians to improve their predictions of PPA neuropathology, especially in nfvPPA and uPPA cases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Gil-Navarro
- Alzheimer's Disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
67
|
Yochim BP, Rashid K, Raymond NC, Beaudreau SA. How Frequently are Words used on Naming Tests used in Spoken Conversation? Clin Neuropsychol 2013; 27:973-87. [DOI: 10.1080/13854046.2013.797501] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
68
|
Afasia progresiva primaria: del síndrome a la enfermedad. Neurologia 2013; 28:366-74. [DOI: 10.1016/j.nrl.2012.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2012] [Accepted: 04/06/2012] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
|
69
|
|
70
|
Mahoney CJ, Malone IB, Ridgway GR, Buckley AH, Downey LE, Golden HL, Ryan NS, Ourselin S, Schott JM, Rossor MN, Fox NC, Warren JD. White matter tract signatures of the progressive aphasias. Neurobiol Aging 2013; 34:1687-99. [PMID: 23312804 PMCID: PMC3601331 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.12.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 81] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2012] [Revised: 10/30/2012] [Accepted: 12/03/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The primary progressive aphasias (PPA) are a heterogeneous group of language-led neurodegenerative diseases resulting from large-scale brain network degeneration. White matter (WM) pathways bind networks together, and might therefore hold information about PPA pathogenesis. Here we used diffusion tensor imaging and tract-based spatial statistics to compare WM tract changes between PPA syndromes and with respect to Alzheimer's disease and healthy controls in 33 patients with PPA (13 nonfluent/agrammatic PPA); 10 logopenic variant PPA; and 10 semantic variant PPA. Nonfluent/agrammatic PPA was associated with predominantly left-sided and anterior tract alterations including uncinate fasciculus (UF) and subcortical projections; semantic variant PPA with bilateral alterations in inferior longitudinal fasciculus and UF; and logopenic variant PPA with bilateral but predominantly left-sided alterations in inferior longitudinal fasciculus, UF, superior longitudinal fasciculus, and subcortical projections. Tract alterations were more extensive than gray matter alterations, and the extent of alteration across tracts and PPA syndromes varied between diffusivity metrics. These WM signatures of PPA syndromes illustrate the selective vulnerability of brain language networks in these diseases and might have some pathologic specificity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Colin J Mahoney
- Dementia Research Centre, UCL Institute of Neurology, London, UK.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
71
|
Mesulam MM, Wieneke C, Hurley R, Rademaker A, Thompson CK, Weintraub S, Rogalski EJ. Words and objects at the tip of the left temporal lobe in primary progressive aphasia. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013; 136:601-18. [PMID: 23361063 DOI: 10.1093/brain/aws336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 154] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
Abstract
Eleven of 69 prospectively enrolled primary progressive aphasics were selected for this study because of peak atrophy sites located predominantly or exclusively within the anterior left temporal lobe. Cortical volumes in these areas were reduced to less than half of control values, whereas average volume elsewhere in the left hemisphere deviated from control values by only 8%. Failure to name objects emerged as the most consistent and severe deficit. Naming errors were attributed to pure retrieval failure if the object could not be named even when the denoting word was understood, the object recognized and the two accurately matched. Surprisingly many of the naming errors reflected pure retrieval failures, without discernible semantic or associative component. The remaining set of errors had associative components. These errors reflected the inability to define the word denoting the object more often than the inability to define the nature of the pictured object. In a separate task where the same object had to be linked to verbal or non-verbal associations, performance was abnormal only in the verbal format. Excessive taxonomic interference was observed for picture-word, but not picture-picture, matching tasks. This excessive interference reflected a blurring of intra- rather than inter-category distinctions as if the acuity of word-object associations had been diminished so that correspondences were easier to recognize at generic than specific levels. These dissociations between verbal and non-verbal markers of object knowledge indicate that the reduced neural mass at peak atrophy sites of the left temporal tip, accounting for half or more of the presumed premorbid volume, was unlikely to have contained domain-independent semantic representations of the type that would be expected in a strictly amodal hub. A more likely arrangement entails two highly interactive routes--a strongly left lateralized temporosylvian language network for verbal concepts, and a presumably more bilateral or right-sided inferotemporal/fusiform object recognition network, which remained relatively spared because peak atrophy sites were concentrated on the left. The current results also suggest that the left anterior temporal neocortex should be inserted into the language network where it is likely to play a major role in selecting verbal labels for objects and mediating the progression of word comprehension from generic to specific levels of precision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M-Marsel Mesulam
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Centre, Northwestern University, 320 East Superior Street Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL 60611, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
72
|
Pakhomov SVS, Marino SE, Birnbaum AK. Quantification of Speech Disfluency as a Marker of Medication-Induced Cognitive Impairment: An Application of Computerized Speech Analysis in Neuropharmacology. COMPUT SPEECH LANG 2013; 27:116-134. [PMID: 37539014 PMCID: PMC10399282 DOI: 10.1016/j.csl.2012.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
We present the results of a study investigating the use of speech and language characteristics extracted from spontaneous spoken discourse to assess changes in cognitive function. Specifically, we investigated the use of automatic speech recognition technology to characterize spontaneous speech disfluency induced by topiramate, an anti-epileptic medication with language-related side-effects. We audio recorded spontaneous speech samples from 20 participants during several picture description tasks and analyzed the recordings automatically and manually to extract a range of spoken fluency measurements including speech discontinuities (e.g., filled pauses, false starts, and repetitions), silent pause duration, speaking rate and vowel lengthening. Our results indicate that some of these paralinguistic speech characteristics are a) sensitive to the effects of topiramate, b) are associated with topiramate concentrations in the blood, and c) complement standard neuropsychological tests typically used to investigate cognitive effects of medications. This work demonstrates the use of computational linguistic tools to assess cognitive effects in a more sensitive, objective and reproducible manner than is currently available with standard tests.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Serguei V S Pakhomov
- Center for Clinical and Cognitive Neuropharmacology, 7-125F Weaver Densford Hall, 308 Harvard St. SE, Minneapolis, MN, 55406 USA
| | - Susan E Marino
- Center for Clinical and Cognitive Neuropharmacology, 7-125F Weaver Densford Hall, 308 Harvard St. SE, Minneapolis, MN, 55406 USA
| | - Angela K Birnbaum
- Center for Clinical and Cognitive Neuropharmacology, 7-125F Weaver Densford Hall, 308 Harvard St. SE, Minneapolis, MN, 55406 USA
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Marino S, Pakhomov S, Han S, Anderson K, Ding M, Eberly L, Loring D, Hawkins-Taylor C, Rarick J, Leppik I, Cibula J, Birnbaum A. The effect of topiramate plasma concentration on linguistic behavior, verbal recall and working memory. Epilepsy Behav 2012; 24:365-72. [PMID: 22658432 PMCID: PMC3804073 DOI: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.04.120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2012] [Revised: 04/22/2012] [Accepted: 04/23/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
This is the first study of the effect of topiramate on linguistic behavior and verbal recall using a computational linguistics system for automated language and speech analysis to detect and quantify drug-induced changes in speech recorded during discourse-level tasks. Healthy volunteers were administered a single, 100-mg oral dose of topiramate in two double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover studies. Subjects' topiramate plasma levels ranged from 0.23 to 2.81 μg/mL. We found a significant association between topiramate levels and impairment on measures of verbal fluency elicited during a picture description task, correct number of words recalled on a paragraph recall test, and reaction time recorded during a working memory task. Using the tools of clinical pharmacology and computational linguistics, we elucidated the relationship between the determinants of a drug's disposition as reflected in plasma concentrations and their impact on cognitive functioning as reflected in spoken language discourse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S.E. Marino
- Center for Clinical and Cognitive Neuropharmacology, University of Minnesota,Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, University of Minnesota
| | - S.V.S. Pakhomov
- Center for Clinical and Cognitive Neuropharmacology, University of Minnesota,Pharmaceutical Care and Health Systems, University of Minnesota
| | - S. Han
- The J. Crayton Pruitt Family Dept of Biomedical Engineering, University of Florida, Gainesville FL
| | - K.L. Anderson
- The J. Crayton Pruitt Family Dept of Biomedical Engineering, University of Florida, Gainesville FL
| | - M. Ding
- The J. Crayton Pruitt Family Dept of Biomedical Engineering, University of Florida, Gainesville FL
| | - L.E. Eberly
- Division of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis MN
| | - D.W. Loring
- Dept of Neurology, Emory University, Atlanta GA
| | | | - J.O. Rarick
- Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, University of Minnesota
| | - I.E. Leppik
- Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, University of Minnesota
| | - J.E. Cibula
- Dept of Neurology, University of Florida, Gainesville FL
| | - A.K. Birnbaum
- Center for Clinical and Cognitive Neuropharmacology, University of Minnesota,Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, University of Minnesota
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
Naming outcomes of anterior temporal lobectomy in epilepsy patients: a systematic review of the literature. Epilepsy Behav 2012; 24:194-8. [PMID: 22569529 DOI: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.04.115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2012] [Revised: 04/02/2012] [Accepted: 04/03/2012] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL) is the standard surgical treatment for medically intractable temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). While seizure outcome is favorable, cognitive outcomes are a concern, particularly in respect of memory and naming. A systematic review of the literature on the naming outcomes of ATL is presented in this article. Searches were conducted on PubMed and PsycInfo, yielding a total of 93 articles, 21 of which met inclusion criteria. Declines in visual naming are common following ATL in the dominant hemisphere, and particularly, for naming living stimuli or famous faces. The Boston Naming Test (BNT) declines by a mean of 5.8 points, exceeding the Reliable Change Index (RCI). There are no reports of deficits in auditory naming following ATL, despite the fact that auditory naming has shown to be a more sensitive measure of dysnomia than the BNT in TLE patients. The absence of structural hippocampal pathology and late-onset epilepsy are the strongest predictors of naming decline. Recommendations are made for further study.
Collapse
|
75
|
Rohrer JD, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Alzheimer's pathology in primary progressive aphasia. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33:744-52. [PMID: 20580129 PMCID: PMC3314936 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.05.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2009] [Revised: 03/13/2010] [Accepted: 05/17/2010] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a neurodegenerative disorder with language impairment as the primary feature. Different subtypes have been described and the 3 best characterized are progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA), semantic dementia (SD) and logopenic/phonological aphasia (LPA). Of these subtypes, LPA is most commonly associated with Alzheimer's disease (AD) pathology. However, the features of PPA associated with AD have not been fully defined. Here we retrospectively identified 14 patients with PPA and either pathologically confirmed AD or cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers consistent with AD. Analysis of neurological and neuropsychological features revealed that all patients had a syndrome of LPA with relatively nonfluent spontaneous speech, phonemic errors, and reduced digit span; most patients also had impaired verbal episodic memory. Analysis of the pattern of cortical thinning in these patients revealed left posterior superior temporal, inferior parietal, medial temporal, and posterior cingulate involvement and in patients with more severe disease, increasing involvement of left anterior temporal and frontal cortices and right hemisphere areas in the temporo-parietal junction, posterior cingulate, and medial temporal lobe. We propose that LPA may be a "unihemispheric" presentation of AD, and discuss this concept in relation to accumulating evidence concerning language dysfunction in AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Jason D. Warren
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Leyton CE, Villemagne VL, Savage S, Pike KE, Ballard KJ, Piguet O, Burrell JR, Rowe CC, Hodges JR. Subtypes of progressive aphasia: application of the international consensus criteria and validation using β-amyloid imaging. Brain 2011; 134:3030-43. [DOI: 10.1093/brain/awr216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 243] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
|
77
|
Lomlomdjian C, Solis P, Medel N, Kochen S. A study of word finding difficulties in Spanish speakers with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2011; 97:37-44. [PMID: 21784616 DOI: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2011.06.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2011] [Revised: 05/15/2011] [Accepted: 06/26/2011] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
It is well established that naming deficits can be found in temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). The aim of this study was to determine in Spanish speakers with pharmacoresistant TLE the characteristics of subjective naming difficulties and to examine performance in a definition task and a picture task in left TLE and right TLE. We observed that almost one-third of patients report frequent and severe word finding problems during spontaneous speech. In naming tests, our patients exhibited delayed times for finding words. Even if the target word was identified and semantically activated, there was difficulty with lexical access, which improved when a phonetic cue was given. Left TLE patients derived a lower benefit from phonetic cues in accessing words, even when the word is known and recognized semantically. These findings were not related to any demographic or clinical characteristics analyzed. The fact that the only weakly lateralized variable has been a lexical access facilitation measurement could support a lexical access hypothesis for naming deficits in TLE.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Lomlomdjian
- Epilepsy Center, Ramos Mejía Hospital, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
78
|
|
79
|
Diesfeldt HFA. [The phonological variant of primary progressive aphasia, a single case study]. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr 2011; 42:79-90. [PMID: 21574504 DOI: 10.1007/s12439-011-0013-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a neurodegenerative syndrome characterized by an insidious onset and gradual progression of deficits that can involve any aspect of language, including word finding, object naming, fluency, syntax, phonology and word comprehension. The initial symptoms occur in the absence of major deficits in other cognitive domains, including episodic memory, visuospatial abilities and visuoconstruction. According to recent diagnostic guidelines, PPA is typically divided into three variants: nonfluent variant PPA (also termed progressive nonfluent aphasia), semantic variant PPA (also termed semantic dementia) and logopenic/phonological variant PPA (also termed logopenic progressive aphasia). The paper describes a 79-yr old man, who presented with normal motor speech and production rate, impaired single word retrieval and phonemic errors in spontaneous speech and confrontational naming. Confrontation naming was strongly affected by lexical frequency. He was impaired on repetition of sentences and phrases. Reading was intact for regularly spelled words but not for irregular words (surface dyslexia). Comprehension was spared at the single word level, but impaired for complex sentences. He performed within the normal range on the Dutch equivalent of the Pyramids and Palm Trees (PPT) Pictures Test, indicating that semantic processing was preserved. There was, however, a slight deficiency on the PPT Words Test, which appeals to semantic knowledge of verbal associations. His core deficit was interpreted as an inability to retrieve stored lexical-phonological information for spoken word production in spontaneous speech, confrontation naming, repetition and reading aloud.
Collapse
|
80
|
Haberstroh J, Neumeyer K, Krause K, Franzmann J, Pantel J. TANDEM: Communication training for informal caregivers of people with dementia. Aging Ment Health 2011; 15:405-13. [PMID: 21491226 DOI: 10.1080/13607863.2010.536135] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Dementia increasingly diminishes the ability to communicate. We aimed to develop and evaluate a psychosocial intervention program that focuses on communication in dementia care. This was intended to enhance the quality of life (QoL) of people with dementia and to reduce the burden on their informal caregivers. METHOD A training program for informal caregivers of people with dementia was developed. The training combines the expertise of geriatric psychiatry, geriatric care, and educational psychology. Caregivers acquire and deepen competencies required to improve communication in dementia care. The training was evaluated with a pre-post-control group design and time-series analyses. Twenty-four informal caregivers participated in the study. RESULTS The results of the study provide evidence that TANDEM training increases caregivers' use of strategies that are relevant for communication in dementia care and the care receivers' QoL. CONCLUSION The results of research in this program show the relevance of including caregivers in interventions and the importance of communication for the QoL of people with dementia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Haberstroh
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine, and Psychotherapy, Goethe University, D-60528 Frankfurt a. M., Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
81
|
Comment on "Semantic dementia combined with motor neuron disease". J Clin Neurosci 2011; 17:1223. [PMID: 20627585 DOI: 10.1016/j.jocn.2009.12.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/19/2009] [Accepted: 12/14/2009] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
82
|
Atrofie focali. Neurologia 2011. [DOI: 10.1016/s1634-7072(11)70571-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
83
|
Rohrer JD, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Syndromes of nonfluent primary progressive aphasia: a clinical and neurolinguistic analysis. Neurology 2010; 75:603-10. [PMID: 20713949 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0b013e3181ed9c6b] [Citation(s) in RCA: 77] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Despite recent work, the nosology of nonfluent primary progressive aphasia (PPA) remains unresolved. METHODS We describe a clinical and neurolinguistic cross-sectional analysis of a cohort of 24 patients with nonfluent PPA. Patients were initially classified based on analysis of spontaneous speech into 4 groups: apraxia of speech (AOS)/agrammatism (10 patients); AOS/no agrammatism (4 patients); no AOS/agrammatism (3 patients); no AOS/no agrammatism (7 patients). These groups were further characterized using a detailed neurolinguistic and neuropsychological battery. Parkinsonism was present in 3/10 patients in the AOS/agrammatism group. All patients in the no AOS/agrammatism group had mutations in the progranulin (GRN) gene, while 5/7 cases in the no AOS/no agrammatism group had CSF findings compatible with Alzheimer disease. RESULTS The groups without AOS showed more severe neurolinguistic impairments for a given disease stage, and sentence comprehension, speech repetition, and reading were impaired in all groups. Prolonged word-finding pauses and impaired single word comprehension were salient features in the no AOS/agrammatism group. Additional impairments of executive function and praxis were present in both groups with agrammatism, and impaired episodic memory was a feature of the no AOS/no agrammatism group. CONCLUSION PPA with AOS is aligned with the syndrome previously designated progressive nonfluent aphasia; agrammatism may emerge as the syndrome evolves, or alternatively, the pure AOS group may be pathophysiologically distinct. PPA without AOS resembles the syndrome designated logopenic/phonologic aphasia; however, there is evidence for a distinct subsyndrome of GRN-associated aphasia. The findings provide a rationale for further longitudinal studies with pathologic correlation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan D Rohrer
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
84
|
Pakhomov SVS, Smith GE, Chacon D, Feliciano Y, Graff-Radford N, Caselli R, Knopman DS. Computerized analysis of speech and language to identify psycholinguistic correlates of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cogn Behav Neurol 2010; 23:165-77. [PMID: 20829666 PMCID: PMC3365864 DOI: 10.1097/wnn.0b013e3181c5dde3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To evaluate the use of a semiautomated computerized system for measuring speech and language characteristics in patients with frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD). BACKGROUND FTLD is a heterogeneous disorder comprising at least 3 variants. Computerized assessment of spontaneous verbal descriptions by patients with FTLD offers a detailed and reproducible view of the underlying cognitive deficits. METHODS Audiorecorded speech samples of 38 patients from 3 participating medical centers were elicited using the Cookie Theft stimulus. Each patient underwent a battery of neuropsychologic tests. The audio was analyzed by the computerized system to measure 15 speech and language variables. Analysis of variance was used to identify characteristics with significant differences in means between FTLD variants. Factor analysis was used to examine the implicit relations between subsets of the variables. RESULTS Semiautomated measurements of pause-to-word ratio and pronoun-to-noun ratio were able to discriminate between some of the FTLD variants. Principal component analysis of all 14 variables suggested 4 subjectively defined components (length, hesitancy, empty content, grammaticality) corresponding to the phenomenology of FTLD variants. CONCLUSION Semiautomated language and speech analysis is a promising novel approach to neuropsychologic assessment that offers a valuable contribution to the toolbox of researchers in dementia and other neurodegenerative disorders.
Collapse
|
85
|
Budd MA, Kortte K, Cloutman L, Newhart M, Gottesman RF, Davis C, Heidler-Gary J, Seay MW, Hillis AE. The nature of naming errors in primary progressive aphasia versus acute post-stroke aphasia. Neuropsychology 2010; 24:581-9. [PMID: 20804246 PMCID: PMC3085899 DOI: 10.1037/a0020287] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To compare the distribution of error types across subgroups of primary progressive aphasia and poststroke aphasia in different vascular locations. METHOD We analyzed naming errors in 49 individuals with acute left hemisphere ischemic stroke and 55 individuals with three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Location of atrophy or ischemic stroke was characterized using MRI. RESULTS We found that distribution of error types was very similar across all subgroups, irrespective of the site or etiology of the lesion. The only significant difference across groups was the percentage of circumlocutions (F(7, 96) = 3.02, p = .005). Circumlocution errors were highest among logopenic variant PPA (24%) and semantic variant PPA (24%). Semantic coordinate errors were common in all groups, probably because they can arise from disruption of different cognitive processes underlying naming and, therefore, from different locations of brain damage. CONCLUSIONS Semantic errors are common among all types of primary progressive aphasia and poststroke aphasia, and the type of error depends in part on the location of damage.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maggi A Budd
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 600 North Wolfe Street, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
86
|
Progranulin-associated primary progressive aphasia: a distinct phenotype? Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:288-97. [PMID: 19766663 PMCID: PMC2808475 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2009] [Revised: 08/24/2009] [Accepted: 09/14/2009] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
The neuropsychological features of the primary progressive aphasia (PPA) syndromes continue to be defined. Here we describe a detailed neuropsychological case study of a patient with a mutation in the progranulin (GRN) gene who presented with progressive word-finding difficulty. Key neuropsychological features in this case included gravely impoverished propositional speech with anomia and prolonged word-finding pauses, impaired speech repetition most marked for sentences, and severely impaired verbal (with preserved spatial) short-term memory. There was a dissociated profile of performance on semantic processing tasks: visual semantic processing was intact, while within the verbal domain, verb comprehension was impaired and processing of nouns was intact on tasks requiring direct semantic processing but impaired on tasks requiring associative or inferential processing. Brain MRI showed asymmetric left cerebral atrophy particularly affecting the temporo-parietal junction, supero-lateral temporal and inferior frontal lobes. This case most closely resembles the PPA syndrome known as the logopenic/phonological aphasia variant (LPA) however there were also deficits of grammar and speech repetition suggesting an overlap with the progressive non-fluent aphasia (agrammatic) variant (PNFA). Certain prominent features of this case (in particular, the profile of semantic impairment) have not been emphasised in previous descriptions of LPA or PNFA, suggesting that GRN may cause an overlapping PPA syndrome but with a distinctive cognitive profile. This neuropsychological evidence suggests that GRN-PPA may result from damage involving the temporo-parietal junction and its functional connections in both the dorsal and ventral language networks, with implications for our understanding of language network pathophysiology.
Collapse
|
87
|
Rohrer JD, Paviour D, Bronstein AM, O'Sullivan SS, Lees A, Warren JD. Progressive supranuclear palsy syndrome presenting as progressive nonfluent aphasia: a neuropsychological and neuroimaging analysis. Mov Disord 2010; 25:179-188. [PMID: 20077483 PMCID: PMC4608044 DOI: 10.1002/mds.22946] [Citation(s) in RCA: 78] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
There is currently considerable interest in the clinical spectrum of progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA) and progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) and the intersection of these two entities. Here, we undertook a detailed prospective clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging analysis of 14 consecutive patients presenting with PNFA to identify cases meeting clinical criteria for PSP. These patients had further detailed assessment of extrapyramidal and oculomotor functions. All patients had high-resolution MR brain volumetry and a cortical thickness analysis was undertaken on the brain images. Four patients presenting with PNFA subsequently developed features of a PSP syndrome, including a typical oculomotor palsy. The neuropsychological profile in these cases was similar to other patients with PNFA, however, with more marked reduction in propositional speech, fewer speech errors, less marked impairment of literacy skills but more severe associated deficits of episodic memory and praxis. These PSP-PNFA cases had less prominent midbrain atrophy but more marked prefrontal atrophy than a comparison group of five patients with pathologically confirmed PSP without PNFA and more prominent midbrain atrophy but less marked perisylvian atrophy than other PNFA cases. In summary, although the PSP-PNFA syndrome overlaps with PNFA without PSP, certain neuropsychological and neuroanatomical differences may help predict the development of a PSP syndrome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan D Rohrer
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Dominic Paviour
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 3BG, UK
- Reta Lila Weston Institute, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 1PJ, UK
| | - Adolfo M Bronstein
- Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Charing Cross Hospital, Imperial College, London, UK
| | - Sean S O'Sullivan
- Reta Lila Weston Institute, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 1PJ, UK
| | - Andrew Lees
- Reta Lila Weston Institute, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 1PJ, UK
| | - Jason D Warren
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 3BG, UK
| |
Collapse
|
88
|
Rohrer JD, Ridgway GR, Crutch SJ, Hailstone J, Goll JC, Clarkson MJ, Mead S, Beck J, Mummery C, Ourselin S, Warrington EK, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Progressive logopenic/phonological aphasia: erosion of the language network. Neuroimage 2010; 49:984-93. [PMID: 19679189 PMCID: PMC2943046 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 169] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2009] [Revised: 07/09/2009] [Accepted: 08/04/2009] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The primary progressive aphasias (PPA) are paradigmatic disorders of language network breakdown associated with focal degeneration of the left cerebral hemisphere. Here we addressed brain correlates of PPA in a detailed neuroanatomical analysis of the third canonical syndrome of PPA, logopenic/phonological aphasia (LPA), in relation to the more widely studied clinico-anatomical syndromes of semantic dementia (SD) and progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA). 32 PPA patients (9 SD, 14 PNFA, 9 LPA) and 18 cognitively normal controls had volumetric brain MRI with regional volumetry, cortical thickness, grey and white matter voxel-based morphometry analyses. Five of nine patients with LPA had cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers consistent with Alzheimer (AD) pathology (AD-PPA) and 2/9 patients had progranulin (GRN) mutations (GRN-PPA). The LPA group had tissue loss in a widespread left hemisphere network. Compared with PNFA and SD, the LPA group had more extensive involvement of grey matter in posterior temporal and parietal cortices and long association white matter tracts. Overlapping but distinct networks were involved in the AD-PPA and GRN-PPA subgroups, with more anterior temporal lobe involvement in GRN-PPA. The importance of these findings is threefold: firstly, the clinico-anatomical entity of LPA has a profile of brain damage that is complementary to the network-based disorders of SD and PNFA; secondly, the core phonological processing deficit in LPA is likely to arise from temporo-parietal junction damage but disease spread occurs through the dorsal language network (and in GRN-PPA, also the ventral language network); and finally, GRN mutations provide a specific molecular substrate for language network dysfunction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan D. Rohrer
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Gerard R. Ridgway
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
- Centre for Medical Image Computing, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, UK
| | - Sebastian J. Crutch
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Julia Hailstone
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Johanna C. Goll
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Matthew J. Clarkson
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
- Centre for Medical Image Computing, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, UK
| | - Simon Mead
- MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Jonathan Beck
- MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London, WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Cath Mummery
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Sebastien Ourselin
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
- Centre for Medical Image Computing, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, UK
| | | | - Martin N. Rossor
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Jason D. Warren
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| |
Collapse
|
89
|
Goll JC, Crutch SJ, Loo JHY, Rohrer JD, Frost C, Bamiou DE, Warren JD. Non-verbal sound processing in the primary progressive aphasias. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2009; 133:272-85. [PMID: 19797352 PMCID: PMC2801322 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awp235] [Citation(s) in RCA: 97] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Little is known about the processing of non-verbal sounds in the primary progressive aphasias. Here, we investigated the processing of complex non-verbal sounds in detail, in a consecutive series of 20 patients with primary progressive aphasia [12 with progressive non-fluent aphasia; eight with semantic dementia]. We designed a novel experimental neuropsychological battery to probe complex sound processing at early perceptual, apperceptive and semantic levels, using within-modality response procedures that minimized other cognitive demands and matching tests in the visual modality. Patients with primary progressive aphasia had deficits of non-verbal sound analysis compared with healthy age-matched individuals. Deficits of auditory early perceptual analysis were more common in progressive non-fluent aphasia, deficits of apperceptive processing occurred in both progressive non-fluent aphasia and semantic dementia, and deficits of semantic processing also occurred in both syndromes, but were relatively modality specific in progressive non-fluent aphasia and part of a more severe generic semantic deficit in semantic dementia. Patients with progressive non-fluent aphasia were more likely to show severe auditory than visual deficits as compared to patients with semantic dementia. These findings argue for the existence of core disorders of complex non-verbal sound perception and recognition in primary progressive aphasia and specific disorders at perceptual and semantic levels of cortical auditory processing in progressive non-fluent aphasia and semantic dementia, respectively.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Johanna C Goll
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
90
|
Rohrer JD, Warren JD, Modat M, Ridgway GR, Douiri A, Rossor MN, Ourselin S, Fox NC. Patterns of cortical thinning in the language variants of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 2009; 72:1562-9. [PMID: 19414722 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0b013e3181a4124e] [Citation(s) in RCA: 185] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) is a clinically, genetically, and pathologically heterogeneous neurodegenerative disorder. Two subtypes commonly present with a language disorder: semantic dementia (SemD) and progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA). METHODS Patients meeting consensus criteria for PNFA and SemD who had volumetric MRI of sufficient quality to allow cortical thickness analysis were recruited from a tertiary referral clinic: 44 (11 pathologically confirmed) patients with SemD and 32 (4 pathologically confirmed) patients with PNFA and 29 age-matched and gender-matched healthy controls were recruited. Cortical thickness analysis was performed using the Freesurfer software tools. RESULTS Patients with SemD had significant cortical thinning in the left temporal lobe, particularly temporal pole, entorhinal cortex, and parahippocampal, fusiform, and inferior temporal gyri. A similar but less extensive pattern of loss was seen in the right temporal lobe and (with increasing severity) also in left orbitofrontal, inferior frontal, insular, and cingulate cortices. Patients with PNFA had involvement particularly of the left superior temporal lobe, inferior frontal lobe, and insula, and (with increasing severity) other areas in the left frontal, lateral temporal, and anterior parietal lobes. Similar patterns were seen in the pathologically confirmed cases. Patterns of cortical thinning differed between groups: SemD had significantly more cortical thinning in the temporal lobes bilaterally while PNFA had significantly more thinning in the frontal and parietal lobes. CONCLUSIONS The language variants of frontotemporal lobar degeneration have distinctive and significantly different patterns of cortical thinning. Increasing disease severity is associated with spread of cortical thinning and the pattern of spread is consistent with progression of clinical deficits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J D Rohrer
- Dementia Research Centre, Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
91
|
Mesulam M, Rogalski E, Wieneke C, Cobia D, Rademaker A, Thompson C, Weintraub S. Neurology of anomia in the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia. Brain 2009; 132:2553-65. [PMID: 19506067 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awp138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
The semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is characterized by the combination of word comprehension deficits, fluent aphasia and a particularly severe anomia. In this study, two novel tasks were used to explore the factors contributing to the anomia. The single most common factor was a blurring of distinctions among members of a semantic category, leading to errors of overgeneralization in word-object matching tasks as well as in word definitions and object descriptions. This factor was more pronounced for natural kinds than artifacts. In patients with the more severe anomias, conceptual maps were more extensively disrupted so that inter-category distinctions were as impaired as intra-category distinctions. Many objects that could not be named aloud could be matched to the correct word in patients with mild but not severe anomia, reflecting a gradual intensification of the semantic factor as the naming disorder becomes more severe. Accurate object descriptions were more frequent than accurate word definitions and all patients experienced prominent word comprehension deficits that interfered with everyday activities but no consequential impairment of object usage or face recognition. Magnetic resonance imaging revealed three characteristics: greater atrophy of the left hemisphere; atrophy of anterior components of the perisylvian language network in the superior and middle temporal gyri; and atrophy of anterior components of the face and object recognition network in the inferior and medial temporal lobes. The left sided asymmetry and perisylvian extension of the atrophy explains the more profound impairment of word than object usage and provides the anatomical basis for distinguishing the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia from the partially overlapping group of patients that fulfil the widely accepted diagnostic criteria for semantic dementia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marsel Mesulam
- Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer's Disease Center, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
92
|
Abnormal laughter-like vocalisations replacing speech in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Sci 2009; 284:120-3. [PMID: 19435636 PMCID: PMC2729814 DOI: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.04.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2009] [Revised: 04/08/2009] [Accepted: 04/14/2009] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
We describe ten patients with a clinical diagnosis of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) (pathologically confirmed in three cases) who developed abnormal laughter-like vocalisations in the context of progressive speech output impairment leading to mutism. Failure of speech output was accompanied by increasing frequency of the abnormal vocalisations until ultimately they constituted the patient's only extended utterance. The laughter-like vocalisations did not show contextual sensitivity but occurred as an automatic vocal output that replaced speech. Acoustic analysis of the vocalisations in two patients revealed abnormal motor features including variable note duration and inter-note interval, loss of temporal symmetry of laugh notes and loss of the normal decrescendo. Abnormal laughter-like vocalisations may be a hallmark of a subgroup in the PPA spectrum with impaired control and production of nonverbal vocal behaviour due to disruption of fronto-temporal networks mediating vocalisation.
Collapse
|
93
|
Fonseca ATD, Guedj E, Alario FX, Laguitton V, Mundler O, Chauvel P, Liegeois-Chauvel C. Brain regions underlying word finding difficulties in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 2009; 132:2772-84. [DOI: 10.1093/brain/awp083] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
94
|
Electrophysiological study of the basal temporal language area: A convergence zone between language perception and production networks. Clin Neurophysiol 2009; 120:539-50. [DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.12.042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2008] [Revised: 12/02/2008] [Accepted: 12/27/2008] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
95
|
Rohrer JD, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Neologistic jargon aphasia and agraphia in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Sci 2009; 277:155-9. [PMID: 19033077 PMCID: PMC2633035 DOI: 10.1016/j.jns.2008.10.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2008] [Revised: 10/07/2008] [Accepted: 10/08/2008] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The terms 'jargon aphasia' and 'jargon agraphia' describe the production of incomprehensible language containing frequent phonological, semantic or neologistic errors in speech and writing, respectively. Here we describe two patients with primary progressive aphasia (PPA) who produced neologistic jargon either in speech or writing. We suggest that involvement of the posterior superior temporal-inferior parietal region may lead to a disconnection between stored lexical representations and language output pathways leading to aberrant activation of phonemes in neologistic jargon. Parietal lobe involvement is relatively unusual in PPA, perhaps accounting for the comparative rarity of jargon early in the course of these diseases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Jason D. Warren
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
96
|
Mapping the progression of progranulin-associated frontotemporal lobar degeneration. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2008; 4:455-60. [PMID: 18648346 DOI: 10.1038/ncpneuro0869] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2007] [Accepted: 06/02/2008] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND A 55-year-old woman was followed over a 13-year period as part of a longitudinal study of people at risk for familial dementia. She was a member of a family with an autosomal dominant familial dementia that fulfilled consensus criteria for frontotemporal lobar degeneration. The patient was initially asymptomatic but developed progressive behavioral and cognitive decline characterized by apathy, impaired emotion recognition, mixed aphasia and parietal lobe dysfunction. INVESTIGATIONS Clinical assessments, neuropsychometry, volumetric brain MRI, and genetic mutation screening. DIAGNOSIS Progranulin-associated frontotemporal lobar degeneration. MANAGEMENT Explanation of the patient's condition and genetic counseling for her family.
Collapse
|
97
|
Beck J, Rohrer JD, Campbell T, Isaacs A, Morrison KE, Goodall EF, Warrington EK, Stevens J, Revesz T, Holton J, Al-Sarraj S, King A, Scahill R, Warren JD, Fox NC, Rossor MN, Collinge J, Mead S. A distinct clinical, neuropsychological and radiological phenotype is associated with progranulin gene mutations in a large UK series. Brain 2008; 131:706-20. [PMID: 18234697 PMCID: PMC2577762 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awm320] [Citation(s) in RCA: 165] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Mutations in the progranulin gene (GRN) are a major cause of frontotemporal lobar degeneration with ubiquitin-positive, tau-negative inclusions (FTLD-U) but the distinguishing clinical and anatomical features of this subgroup remain unclear. In a large UK cohort we found five different frameshift and premature termination mutations likely to be causative of FTLD in 25 affected family members. A previously described 4-bp insertion mutation in GRN exon 2 comprised the majority of cases in our cohort (20/25), with four novel mutations being identified in the other five affected members. Additional novel missense changes were discovered, of uncertain pathogenicity, but deletion of the entire gene was not detected. The patient collection was investigated by a single tertiary referral centre and is enriched for familial early onset FTLD with a high proportion of patients undergoing neuropsychological testing, MRI and eventual neuropathological diagnosis. Age at onset was variable, but four mutation carriers presented in their 40s and when analysed as a group, the mean age at onset of disease in GRN mutation carriers was later than tau gene (MAPT) mutation carriers and duration of disease was shorter when compared with both MAPT and FTLD-U without mutation. The most common clinical presentation seen in GRN mutation carriers was behavioural variant FTLD with apathy as the dominant feature. However, many patients had language output impairment that was either a progressive non-fluent aphasia or decreased speech output consistent with a dynamic aphasia. Neurological and neuropsychological examination also suggests that parietal lobe dysfunction is a characteristic feature of GRN mutation and differentiates this group from other patients with FTLD. MR imaging showed evidence of strikingly asymmetrical atrophy with the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes all affected. Both right- and left-sided predominant atrophy was seen even within the same family. As a group, the GRN carriers showed more asymmetry than in other FTLD groups. All pathologically investigated cases showed extensive type 3 TDP-43-positive pathology, including frequent neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions, dystrophic neurites in both grey and white matter and also neuronal intranuclear inclusions. Finally, we confirmed a modifying effect of APOE-E4 genotype on clinical phenotype with a later onset in the GRN carriers suggesting that this gene has distinct phenotypic effects in different neurodegenerative diseases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan Beck
- MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Jonathan D. Rohrer
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Tracy Campbell
- MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Adrian Isaacs
- MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Karen E. Morrison
- Division of Neuroscience, the Medical School,University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK
- Department of Neuroscience, Queen Elizabeth Hospital, University Hospitals Birmingham NHS Foundation Trust, Birmingham, UK
| | - Emily F. Goodall
- Division of Neuroscience, the Medical School,University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK
| | - Elizabeth K. Warrington
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - John Stevens
- Department of Clinical Neuroradiology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Tamas Revesz
- Department of Neuropathology, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Janice Holton
- Department of Neuropathology, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Safa Al-Sarraj
- Department of Clinical Neuropathology, King's College Hospital, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Andrew King
- Department of Clinical Neuropathology, King's College Hospital, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Rachael Scahill
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Jason D. Warren
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Nick C. Fox
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Martin N. Rossor
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - John Collinge
- MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Simon Mead
- MRC Prion Unit, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| |
Collapse
|
98
|
Abstract
The term semantic dementia was devised by Snowden et al. in 1989 and nowadays,
the semantic dementia syndrome is recognized as one of the clinical forms of
frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) and is characterized by a language
semantic disturbance associated to non-verbal semantic memory impairment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mirna Lie Hosogi Senaha
- Speech Pathologist, Member of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology Unit of the Department of Neurology, University of São Paulo School of Medicine, São Paulo, Brazil
| | - Paulo Caramelli
- Associate Professor, Department of Internal Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Federal University of Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil
| | - Claudia Sellitto Porto
- Neuropsychologist, Member of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology Unit of the Department of Neurology, University of São Paulo School of Medicine, São Paulo, Brazil
| | - Ricardo Nitrini
- Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology Unit of the Department of Neurology, and Cognitive Disorders Reference Center (CEREDIC). Hospital das Clínicas of the University of São Paulo School of Medicine, São Paulo, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|