51
|
Quas JA, Wallin AR, Papini S, Lench H, Scullin MH. Suggestibility, social support, and memory for a novel experience in young children. J Exp Child Psychol 2005; 91:315-41. [PMID: 15904929 PMCID: PMC2913677 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2004] [Revised: 03/15/2005] [Accepted: 03/15/2005] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
This study examined 5- and 6-year-olds' suggestibility and interviewer demeanor as joint predictors of their memory for a novel experience. Session 1 consisted of children taking part in a novel laboratory event. Session 2 took place after approximately a 1-week delay and consisted of children completing both a memory test concerning what happened during the prior event and the Video Suggestibility Scale for Children (VSSC). During the second session, the interviewer behaved either supportively or nonsupportively. Greater acquiescence on the VSSC was associated with fewer correct responses to misleading questions about the laboratory event in the supportive and nonsupportive conditions and with more errors in response to specific questions in the nonsupportive condition. Results indicate that individual differences in children's suggestibility are related to the accuracy of their memory for separate events, although some of these relations may vary depending on the context in which children are interviewed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jodi A Quas
- Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
52
|
Abstract
Pain is subjective. The pain response is individual and is learned through social learning and experience. Early pain experiences may play a particularly important role in shaping an individual's pain responses. Painful medical procedures such as immunizations, venipunctures and dental care, and minor emergency department procedures such as laceration repair, compose a significant portion of the average child's experience with painful events. Inadequate relief of pain and distress during childhood painful medical procedures may have long-term negative effects on future pain tolerance and pain responses. This article reviews the evidence for long-term negative effects of inadequately treated procedural pain, the determinants of an individual's pain response, tools to assess pain in children, and interventions to reduce procedural pain and distress. Future research directions and a model for conceptualizing and studying pediatric procedural pain are proposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kelly D Young
- David Geffen School of Medicine at University of California-Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
|
54
|
Deffenbacher KA, Bornstein BH, Penrod SD, McGorty EK. A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on eyewitness memory. LAW AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2004; 28:687-706. [PMID: 15732653 DOI: 10.1007/s10979-004-0565-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 190] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
In the past 30 years researchers have examined the impact of heightened stress on the fidelity of eyewitness memory. Meta-analyses were conducted on 27 independent tests of the effects of heightened stress on eyewitness identification of the perpetrator or target person and separately on 36 tests of eyewitness recall of details associated with the crime. There was considerable support for the hypothesis that high levels of stress negatively impact both types of eyewitness memory. Meta-analytic Z-scores, whether unweighted or weighted by sample size, ranged from -5.40 to -6.44 (high stress condition-low stress condition). The overall effect sizes were -.31 for both proportion of correct identifications and accuracy of eyewitness recall. Effect sizes were notably larger for target-present than for target-absent lineups, for eyewitness identification studies than for face recognition studies and for eyewitness studies employing a staged crime than for eyewitness studies employing other means to induce stress.
Collapse
|
55
|
Pipe ME, Sutherland R, Webster N, Jones C, Rooy DL. Do early interviews affect children's long-term event recall? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
56
|
Abstract
Differences in basic memory processes between maltreated and nonmaltreated children were examined in an experiment in which middle-socioeconomic-status (SES; N = 60), low-SES maltreated (N = 48), and low-SES nonmaltreated (N = 51) children (ages 5-7, 8-9, and 10-12 years) studied 12 Deese-Roediger-McDermott lists. Using recall and recognition measures, the results showed that both true and false memories increased with age and, contrary to some speculation, these trends did not differ as a function of maltreatment status. However, there were differences in overall memory performance as a function of SES. These results are discussed in the broader framework of children's memory development and the effects of the chronic stress associated with child maltreatment on basic memory processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark L Howe
- Department of Psychology, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario, Canada.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
57
|
Principe GF. If at First You Don't Remember, Try, Try Again: The Role of Initial Encoding in Children's False Reports. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2004. [DOI: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0503_3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
|
58
|
von Baeyer CL, Marche TA, Rocha EM, Salmon K. Children's memory for pain: overview and implications for practice. THE JOURNAL OF PAIN 2004; 5:241-9. [PMID: 15219255 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 155] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Children's memories of painful experiences can have long-term consequences for their reaction to later painful events and their acceptance of later health care interventions. This review surveys research on children's memory for pain, emphasizing implications for clinical practice. Topics reviewed include consequences of children's memories of pain; the development of memory; differences between explicit (declarative, verbal, autobiographic) memory and implicit (nondeclarative, nonverbal) memory; and individual differences, situational, and methodologic factors affecting memories of pain. Methods to prevent the adverse consequences of remembered pain are addressed with reference to current research on editing or reframing memories. PERSPECTIVE This review covers topics of value to clinicians providing care to children undergoing painful procedures. Specific recommendations are offered regarding the importance of acknowledging and assessing children's previous memories of painful experiences, the type of information that benefits children before and after procedures, and the most appropriate questioning strategies. It might be possible to prevent or reduce the adverse effects of memories of pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carl L von Baeyer
- St Thomas More College, University of Saskatchewan, 9 Campus Drive, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan S7N 5A5, Canada.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
59
|
Bruck M, Melnyk L. Individual differences in children's suggestibility: a review and synthesis. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1070] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
|
60
|
Gilstrap LL, Papierno PB. Is the cart pushing the horse? the effects of child characteristics on children's and adults' interview behaviours. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1072] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
61
|
Ornstein PA, Elischberger HB. Studies of suggestibility: some observations and suggestions. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
62
|
Melnyk L, Bruck M. Timing moderates the effects of repeated suggestive interviewing on children's eyewitness memory. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
63
|
Erdmann K, Volbert R, Böhm C. Children report suggested events even when interviewed in a non-suggestive manner: what are its implications for credibility assessment? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
64
|
Brainerd CJ, Reyna VF, Wright R, Mojardin AH. Recollection rejection: false-memory editing in children and adults. Psychol Rev 2003; 110:762-84. [PMID: 14599242 DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.110.4.762] [Citation(s) in RCA: 135] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Mechanisms for editing false events out of memory reports have fundamental implications for theories of false memory and for best practice in applied domains in which false reports must be minimized (e.g., forensic psychological interviews, sworn testimony). A mechanism posited in fuzzy-trace theory, recollection rejection, is considered. A process analysis of false-memory editing is presented, which assumes that false-but-gist-consistent events (e.g., the word SOFA, when the word COUCH was experienced) sometimes cue the retrieval of verbatim traces of the corresponding true events (COUCH), generating mismatches that counteract the high familiarity of false-but-gist-consistent events. Empirical support comes from 2 qualitative phenomena: recollective suppression of semantic false memory and inverted-U relations between retrieval time and semantic false memory. Further support comes from 2 quantitative methodologies: conjoint recognition and receiver operating characteristics. The analysis also predicts a novel false-memory phenomenon (erroneous recollection rejection), in which true events are inappropriately edited out of memory reports.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C J Brainerd
- University of Arizona, College of Education, Department of Special Education, Rehabilitation, and School Psychology, Tucson, AZ 85721-0069, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
65
|
Ceci SJ. Cast in Six Ponds and You'll Reel in Something: Looking Back on 25 Years of Research. AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2003; 58:855-864. [PMID: 14609372 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.58.11.855] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The author describes his program of research over the past 25 years. This research falls into 6 areas that are interdependent and that inform each other. The overall program is guided by 3 bioecological principles that posit the need for proximal processes and motivation to actualize biological potential. The author presents examples of experiments that fall into each of the 6 areas and show that human potential is highly contextualized and that, consequently, the same person who fails at a task in one domain is often able to succeed at it in a different domain.
Collapse
|
66
|
Finnilä K, Mahlberg N, Santtila P, Sandnabba K, Niemi P. Validity of a test of children's suggestibility for predicting responses to two interview situations differing in their degree of suggestiveness. J Exp Child Psychol 2003; 85:32-49. [PMID: 12742761 DOI: 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00025-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
In the present study the relative contributions of internal and external sources of variation in children's suggestibility in interrogative situations were examined. One hundred and eleven children (48 4- to 5-year-olds and 63 7- to 8-year-olds) were administered a suggestibility test (BTSS) and the most suggestible (N=36) and the least suggestible (N=36) children were randomly assigned to either an interview condition containing several suggestive techniques or to one containing only suggestive questions. The effects of internal sources of variation in suggestibility were compared with the effects of the interview styles on the children's answers. The former did influence the children, but the external sources of variation in suggestibility had a stronger impact. Influences of cognitive, developmental factors could be found, but not when abuse-related questions were asked and high pressured interview methods were used. These findings indicate that individual assessment of suggestibility can be of some assistance when interviewing children, but diminishing suggestive influences in interrogations must be given priority.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katarina Finnilä
- Department of Psychology, Abo Akademi University, The Police College of Finland, Laivurinkatu 37 D 22, Helsinki 00150, Finland.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
67
|
Powell MB, Jones CH, Campbell C. A comparison of preschoolers' recall of experienced versus non-experienced events across multiple interviews. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2003. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.932] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
68
|
Eisen ML, Qin J, Goodman GS, Davis SL. Memory and suggestibility in maltreated children: age, stress arousal, dissociation, and psychopathology. J Exp Child Psychol 2002; 83:167-212. [PMID: 12457859 DOI: 10.1016/s0022-0965(02)00126-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The present study was designed to assess children's memory and suggestibility in the context of ongoing child maltreatment investigations. One hundred eighty-nine 3-17-year-olds involved in evaluations of alleged maltreatment were interviewed with specific and misleading questions about an anogenital examination and clinical assessment. For the anogenital examination, children's stress arousal was indexed both behaviorally and physiologically. For all children, individual-difference data were gathered on intellectual and short-term memory abilities, general psychopathology, and dissociative tendencies. Interviewers' ratings were available for a subset of children concerning the amount of detail provided in abuse disclosures. Results indicated that general psychopathology, short-term memory, and intellectual ability predicted facets of children's memory performance. Older compared to younger children evinced fewer memory errors and greater suggestibility resistance. Age was also significantly related to the amount of detail in children's abuse disclosures. Neither dissociation nor stress arousal significantly predicted children's memory. Implications for understanding maltreated children's eyewitness memory are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mitchell L Eisen
- Department of Psychology, California State University, King Hall, 5151 State University Drive, Los Angeles, CA 90032, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
69
|
Abstract
There are significant differences between a clinical evaluation and a forensic evaluation [289-291]. These differences must be kept solidly in mind in performing the evaluation. The forensic evaluator needs to assess the validity of complaints, including the possibility of malingering and the child's ability to describe symptoms accurately, the connection between the symptoms and a given incident, and the potential long-term sequelae of a trauma. The goal of the interview is not to treat, but to obtain information. Assessing the validity of complaints is perhaps the greatest challenge. This requires obtaining and reconciling data from numerous sources, including interviews with the child and parents, and information from other sources, as well as rating scales and validity testing. One must be very cautious in asking leading questions and using standardized PTSD protocols, lest they teach the parents and child about the symptoms of PTSD and thereby distort the information they provide as a result. The forensic interviewer should consider what will be needed when called to testify in court. What data will convince the jury? How might the opposing attorney challenge the assessment? What scientific studies support the findings and conclusions concerning the diagnosis, functional impairment, and validity. The precise DSM-IV-TR diagnosis is not always key in a forensic evaluation. What is essential is establishing the connection between the trauma and ensuing emotional problems. All of the symptoms the individual has as a result of the trauma become important, whether or not they contribute to fulfillment of DSM-IV-TR criteria. This contrasts with a clinical evaluation in which one needs to demonstrate the existence of a DSM-IV-TR diagnosis for reimbursement purposes. Finally, the forensic evaluator should be familiar with current practice guidelines for examination of children with PTSD. Any deviation may need to be explained in court [264,292].
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roy Lubit
- Department of Psychiatry, Saint Vincent Catholic Medical Centers, 144 West 12th Street, New York, NY 10011, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
70
|
|
71
|
Gobbo C, Mega C, Pipe ME. Does the nature of the experience influence suggestibility? A study of children's event memory. J Exp Child Psychol 2002; 81:502-30. [PMID: 11890734 DOI: 10.1006/jecp.2002.2662] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Two experiments examined the effects of event modality on children's memory and suggestibility. In Experiment 1, 3- and 5-year-old children directly participated in, observed, or listened to a narrative about an event. In an interview immediately after the event, free recall was followed by misleading or leading questions and, in turn, test recall questions. One week later children were reinterviewed. In Experiment 2, 4-year-old children either participated in or listened to a story about an event, either a single time or to a criterion level of learning. Misleading questions were presented either immediately or 1 week after learning, followed by test recall questions. Five-year-old children were more accurate than 3-year-olds and those participating were more accurate than those either observing or listening to a narrative. However, method of assessment, level of event learning, delay to testing, and variables relating to the misled items also influenced the magnitude of misinformation effects.
Collapse
|
72
|
Lindberg MA. The role of suggestions and personality characteristics in producing illness reports and desires for suing the responsible party. THE JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2002; 136:125-40. [PMID: 12081088 DOI: 10.1080/00223980209604144] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022] Open
Abstract
For this project, 92 students entered an abandoned theater room in an old basement of the university where sand was scattered throughout. The purpose of the study was to experimentally demonstrate that psychological suggestions could produce illness reports and to explore who is most likely to say that they would sue for personal damages. The students filled out the Trait-State Anger Scale and two subscales, Anger Temperament and Anger Reaction (C. D. Spielberger, G. A. Jacobs, S. Russell, & R. S. Crance, 1983) as well as the Costello-Comrey Anxiety Scale (G. C. Costello & A. L. Comrey, 1967), the Hardiness Inventory (S. C. Kobasa, 1982), the Pennebaker Inventory of Limbic Languidness (J. W. Pennebaker, 1982), and, embedded in the Hardiness Inventory, measures of current illness as a result of exposure to the basement room. Half the participants were met by a confederate student who claimed to be cleaning up the remains of a production of "Lawrence of Arabia," and the other half were met by a confederate construction worker who claimed that "The stuff will tear up your skin and your lungs." Those in the experimental groups who perceived danger and scored low in the hardiness dimension of challenge were more likely to report symptoms of illness. Willingness to file a law suit was predicted by a model including perceived danger and the personality characteristic of anger reactivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc A Lindberg
- Department of Psychology, Marshall University, Huntington, WV 25755-2672, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Burgwyn-Bailes E, Baker-Ward L, Gordon BN, Ornstein PA. Children's memory for emergency medical treatment after one year: the impact of individual difference variables on recall and suggestibility. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2001. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.833] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
|
74
|
Roberts KP, Powell MB. Describing individual incidents of sexual abuse: a review of research on the effects of multiple sources of information on children's reports. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2001; 25:1643-1659. [PMID: 11814161 DOI: 10.1016/s0145-2134(01)00290-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE For successful prosecution of child sexual abuse, children are often required to provide reports about individual, alleged incidents. Although verbally or mentally rehearsing memory of an incident can strengthen memories, children's report of individual incidents can also be contaminated when they experience other events related to the individual incidents (e.g., informal interviews, dreams of the incident) and/or when they have similar, repeated experiences of an incident, as in cases of multiple abuse. METHOD Research is reviewed on the positive and negative effects of these related experiences on the length, accuracy, and structure of children's reports of a particular incident. RESULTS Children's memories of a particular incident can be strengthened when exposed to information that does not contradict what they have experienced, thus promoting accurate recall and resistance to false, suggestive influences. When the encountered information differs from children's experiences of the target incident, however, children can become confused between their experiences-they may remember the content but not the source of their experiences. CONCLUSIONS We discuss the implications of this research for interviewing children in sexual abuse investigations and provide a set of research-based recommendations for investigative interviewers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K P Roberts
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
75
|
Abstract
PURPOSE The objectives of the case study reported in this article were twofold. The first objective was to follow the path by which a naive suggestion made in the course of a mother-child conversation was transformed into an allegation of severe sexual abuse. The second objective was to analyze the child's interview scientifically and explore the limitations of scientific tools for detecting implausible allegations. METHODS Independent case facts were collected and analyzed to determine whether the event described by the child was likely to have happened. The credibility of the child's account was assessed using Criterion-Based Content Analysis and the information provided in both the "implausible" and "corrected" statements was compared to quantify the fabricated details in the implausible statement. RESULTS The event described by the child was "very unlikely to have happened" but the credibility assessment failed to detect its implausibility. Comparison of the two statements revealed that the child did fabricate central details but incorporated them into a description of an event she really experienced, and most of the information provided was truthful. CONCLUSIONS The pressure to conform to suggestions can be irresistible, inducing some children to make false allegations of severe sexual abuse. Scientific tools designed for credibility assessment are limited and may fail to detect implausible statements especially when they incorporate information about genuinely experienced events.
Collapse
|
76
|
Lindberg MA, Jones S, Collard LM, Thomas SW. Similarities and differences in eyewitness testimonies of children who directly versus vicariously experience stress. J Genet Psychol 2001; 162:314-33. [PMID: 11678366 DOI: 10.1080/00221320109597486] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
This study tested questions of ecological validity by comparing the eyewitness testimonies of children directly experiencing a painful inoculation experience those of children in a yoked-control group who vicariously experienced the inoculation onwith videotape. The study involved 86 5-year-olds, divided between 2 groups: the experiential and yoked control. The experiential group was followed through a health department with a video camera as they received diphtheria, pertussis, tetanus (DPT), and oral polio inoculations. They were tested immediately, 20 min later, and 1 month later. Each child in the yoked-control group merely watched the videotape of his or her counterpart in the experiential group, made similar ratings of pain, and was given the same tests and suggestions. Stress and personal experience affected items congruent with the stressor to produce flashbulb-like memories, with slower rates of forgetting for some items, such as nurse identifications, and greater suggestibility for other items, such as estimates of needle size. These and the apparently conflicting results in the literature were said to make sense when personally experienced stress was viewed from S.-A. Christianson's (1992) interactive perspective rather than as a single ubiquitous variable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M A Lindberg
- Department of Psychology, Marshall University, Huntington, WV 25755, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
77
|
Poole DA, Lindsay DS. Children's eyewitness reports after exposure to misinformation from parents. J Exp Psychol Appl 2001; 7:27-50. [PMID: 11577617 DOI: 10.1037/1076-898x.7.1.27] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
This study examined how misleading suggestions from parents influenced children's eyewitness reports. Children (3 to 8 years old) participated in science demonstrations, listened to their parents read a story that described experienced and nonexperienced events, and subsequently discussed the science experience in two follow-up interviews. Many children described fictitious events in response to open-ended prompts, and there were no age differences in suggestibility during this phase of the interview. Accuracy declined markedly in response to direct questions, especially for the younger children. Although the older children retracted many of their false reports after receiving source-monitoring instructions, the younger children did not. Path analyses indicated that acquiescence, free recall, and source monitoring all contribute to mediating patterns of suggestibility across age. Results indicate that judgments about the accuracy of children's testimony must consider the possibility of exposure to misinformation prior to formal interviews.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D A Poole
- Department of Psychology, 231 Sloan Hall, Central Michigan University, Mt. Pleasant, Michigan 48859, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
78
|
Thierry KL, Spence MJ, Memon A. Before Misinformation is Encountered: Source Monitoring Decreases Child Witness Suggestibility. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2001. [DOI: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0201_1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
|
79
|
Peterson C, Whalen N. Five years later: children's memory for medical emergencies. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2001. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.832] [Citation(s) in RCA: 81] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
80
|
Eyewitness Testimony for Physical Abuse as a Function of Personal Experience, Development, and Focus of Study. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2000. [DOI: 10.1016/s0193-3973(00)00054-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
81
|
|
82
|
Principe GF, Ornstein PA, Baker-Ward L, Gordon BN. The effects of intervening experiences on children's memory for a physical examination. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2000. [DOI: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(200001)14:1<59::aid-acp637>3.0.co;2-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
83
|
Malinoski PT, Lynn SJ. The plasticity of early memory reports: social pressure, hypnotizability, compliance, and interrogative suggestibility. Int J Clin Exp Hypn 1999; 47:320-45. [PMID: 10553313 DOI: 10.1080/00207149908410040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Early autobiographical memory reports by adults were very sensitive to social influence in a leading interview. The mean age of initial earliest memory report was 3.7 years. When participants were instructed to close their eyes, visualize, and focus on their 2nd birthday, 59% reported a birthday memory. After repeated probes for earlier memories, 78% of subjects reported memories at or prior to 24 months of age, and 33% reported memories within the first 12 months of age. The mean age of the final earliest memory reported was 1.6 years. Participants rated their memory reports as accurate and did not recant them when given an opportunity. The age of earliest memory reports in the suggestive interview correlated negatively with measures of compliance, hypnotizability, and interrogative suggestibility.
Collapse
|
84
|
|
85
|
Raviv A, Raviv A, Shimoni H, Fox NA, Leavitt LA. Children's Self-Report of Exposure to Violence and Its Relation to Emotional Distress. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 1999. [DOI: 10.1016/s0193-3973(99)00020-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
86
|
Bruck M. A Summary of an Affidavit Prepared for Commonwealth of Massachusetts v. Cheryl Amirault LeFave. APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE 1999. [DOI: 10.1207/s1532480xads0302_5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
|
87
|
Quas JA, Goodman GS, Bidrose S, Pipe ME, Craw S, Ablin DS. Emotion and memory: Children's long-term remembering, forgetting, and suggestibility. J Exp Child Psychol 1999; 72:235-70. [PMID: 10074380 DOI: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2491] [Citation(s) in RCA: 186] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Children's memories for an experienced and a never-experienced medical procedure were examined. Three- to 13-year-olds were questioned about a voiding cystourethrogram fluoroscopy (VCUG) they endured between 2 and 6 years of age. Children 4 years or older at VCUG were more accurate than children younger than 4 at VCUG. Longer delays were associated with providing fewer units of correct information but not with more inaccuracies. Parental avoidant attachment style was related to increased errors in children's VCUG memory. Children were more likely to assent to the false medical procedure when it was alluded to briefly than when described in detail, and false assents were related to fewer "do-not-know" responses about the VCUG. Results have implications for childhood amnesia, stress and memory, individual differences, and eyewitness testimony.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J A Quas
- University of California, Berkeley, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
88
|
Abstract
In this review, we describe a shift that has taken place in the area of developmental suggestibility. Formerly, studies in this area indicated that there were pronounced age-related differences in suggestibility, with preschool children being particularly susceptible to misleading suggestions. The studies on which this conclusion was based were criticized on several grounds (e.g. unrealistic scenarios, truncated age range). Newer studies that have addressed these criticisms, however, have largely confirmed the earlier conclusions. These studies indicate that preschool children are disproportionately vulnerable to a variety of suggestive influences. There do not appear to any strict boundary conditions to this conclusion, and preschool children will sometimes succumb to suggestions about bodily touching, emotional events, and participatory events. The evidence for this assertion is presented in this review.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Bruck
- Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec.
| | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Assessing the accuracy of young children's reports: Lessons from the investigation of child sexual abuse. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1998. [DOI: 10.1016/s0962-1849(98)80019-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
90
|
Abstract
Fuzzy-trace theory's concepts of identity judgment, nonidentity judgment, and similarity judgment provide a unified account of the false-memory phenomena that have been most commonly studied in children: false-recognition effects and misinformation effects. False-recognition effects (elevated false-alarm rates for unpresented distractors that preserve the meanings of presented targets) are due to increased rates of similarity or false identity judgment about distractors or to decreased rates of nonidentity judgment. Misinformation effects (erroneous acceptance of misleading postevent information and erroneous rejection of actual events) are also due to variability in rates of similarity, identity, and nonidentity judgment. Two experimental paradigms are presented, one for false recognition (conjoint recognition) and one for misinformation (conjoint misinformation), that allow investigators to tease apart the contributions of these processes to children's false-memory reports. Each paradigm is implemented in a mathematical model that provides numerical estimates of the processes.
Collapse
|
91
|
Memory suggestibility and metacognition in child eyewitness testimony: The roles of source monitoring and self-efficacy. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY OF EDUCATION 1998. [DOI: 10.1007/bf03172812] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
92
|
Abstract
OBJECTIVES In this series of studies, the authors sought to determine some of the cognitive and social boundary conditions that can undermine the accuracy of young children's reporting. Care was taken to include events and interviewing variables that more accurately reflect the experiences of children in real-world investigations of alleged sexual abuse. Videotaped interviews with preschool children were presented to experts to determine how adept they are at distinguishing between true and false accounts. METHOD All the studies were designed to investigate the susceptibility to suggestion in young preschool children. The difference between studies was the form of that suggestion and the nature of the event to which the children were exposed. All studies measured recall accuracy, false assent rate, and the change in these outcomes over time and/or successive interviews. RESULTS Very young preschool children (aged 3 and 4 years) were significantly more vulnerable to suggestions than were older preschool children (aged 5 and 6 years). The number of interviews and the length of the interval over which they were presented resulted in the greatest level of suggestibility. CONCLUSIONS While some types of events (negative, genital, salient) were more difficult to implant in children's statements, some children appeared to internalize the false suggestions and resisted debriefing. These children's false statements were quite convincing to professionals, who were unable to distinguish between true and false accounts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S J Ceci
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
93
|
Bruck M, Ceci SJ, Melnyk L. External and internal sources of variation in the creation of false reports in children. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 1997. [DOI: 10.1016/s1041-6080(97)90011-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
94
|
|
95
|
|
96
|
Portwood SG, Reppucci N. Adults' impact on the suggestibility of preschoolers' recollections. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 1996. [DOI: 10.1016/s0193-3973(96)90024-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|