151
|
A holistic picture of Austronesian migrations revealed by phylogeography of Pacific paper mulberry. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2015; 112:13537-42. [PMID: 26438853 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1503205112] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022] Open
Abstract
The peopling of Remote Oceanic islands by Austronesian speakers is a fascinating and yet contentious part of human prehistory. Linguistic, archaeological, and genetic studies have shown the complex nature of the process in which different components that helped to shape Lapita culture in Near Oceania each have their own unique history. Important evidence points to Taiwan as an Austronesian ancestral homeland with a more distant origin in South China, whereas alternative models favor South China to North Vietnam or a Southeast Asian origin. We test these propositions by studying phylogeography of paper mulberry, a common East Asian tree species introduced and clonally propagated since prehistoric times across the Pacific for making barkcloth, a practical and symbolic component of Austronesian cultures. Using the hypervariable chloroplast ndhF-rpl32 sequences of 604 samples collected from East Asia, Southeast Asia, and Oceanic islands (including 19 historical herbarium specimens from Near and Remote Oceania), 48 haplotypes are detected and haplotype cp-17 is predominant in both Near and Remote Oceania. Because cp-17 has an unambiguous Taiwanese origin and cp-17-carrying Oceanic paper mulberries are clonally propagated, our data concur with expectations of Taiwan as the Austronesian homeland, providing circumstantial support for the "out of Taiwan" hypothesis. Our data also provide insights into the dispersal of paper mulberry from South China "into North Taiwan," the "out of South China-Indochina" expansion to New Guinea, and the geographic origins of post-European introductions of paper mulberry into Oceania.
Collapse
|
152
|
Watts J, Sheehan O, Greenhill SJ, Gomes-Ng S, Atkinson QD, Bulbulia J, Gray RD. Pulotu: Database of Austronesian Supernatural Beliefs and Practices. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0136783. [PMID: 26398231 PMCID: PMC4580586 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136783] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2015] [Accepted: 08/07/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Scholars have debated naturalistic theories of religion for thousands of years, but only recently have scientists begun to test predictions empirically. Existing databases contain few variables on religion, and are subject to Galton's Problem because they do not sufficiently account for the non-independence of cultures or systematically differentiate the traditional states of cultures from their contemporary states. Here we present Pulotu: the first quantitative cross-cultural database purpose-built to test evolutionary hypotheses of supernatural beliefs and practices. The Pulotu database documents the remarkable diversity of the Austronesian family of cultures, which originated in Taiwan, spread west to Madagascar and east to Easter Island-a region covering over half the world's longitude. The focus of Austronesian beliefs range from localised ancestral spirits to powerful creator gods. A wide range of practices also exist, such as headhunting, elaborate tattooing, and the construction of impressive monuments. Pulotu is freely available, currently contains 116 cultures, and has 80 variables describing supernatural beliefs and practices, as well as social and physical environments. One major advantage of Pulotu is that it has separate sections on the traditional states of cultures, the post-contact history of cultures, and the contemporary states of cultures. A second major advantage is that cultures are linked to a language-based family tree, enabling the use phylogenetic methods, which can be used to address Galton's Problem by accounting for common ancestry, to infer deep prehistory, and to model patterns of trait evolution over time. We illustrate the power of phylogenetic methods by performing an ancestral state reconstruction on the Pulotu variable "headhunting", finding evidence that headhunting was practiced in proto-Austronesian culture. Quantitative cross-cultural databases explicitly linking cultures to a phylogeny have the potential to revolutionise the field of comparative religious studies in the same way that genetic databases have revolutionised the field of evolutionary biology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph Watts
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
- * E-mail:
| | - Oliver Sheehan
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
- Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
| | - Simon J. Greenhill
- School of Culture, History and Language, ANU College of Asia and the Pacific, Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
- ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
| | | | - Quentin D. Atkinson
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
- Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
| | - Joseph Bulbulia
- School of Art History, Classics and Religious Studies, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
| | - Russell D. Gray
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
- Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
- Research School of the Social Sciences, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
- Allan Wilson Centre for Molecular Ecology and Evolution, Palmerston North, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
153
|
Dissecting the genetic structure and admixture of four geographical Malay populations. Sci Rep 2015; 5:14375. [PMID: 26395220 PMCID: PMC4585825 DOI: 10.1038/srep14375] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2015] [Accepted: 08/24/2015] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
The Malay people are an important ethnic composition in Southeast Asia, but their genetic make-up and population structure remain poorly studied. Here we conducted a genome-wide study of four geographical Malay populations: Peninsular Malaysian Malay (PMM), Singaporean Malay (SGM), Indonesian Malay (IDM) and Sri Lankan Malay (SLM). All the four Malay populations showed substantial admixture with multiple ancestries. We identified four major ancestral components in Malay populations: Austronesian (17%–62%), Proto-Malay (15%–31%), East Asian (4%–16%) and South Asian (3%–34%). Approximately 34% of the genetic makeup of SLM is of South Asian ancestry, resulting in its distinct genetic pattern compared with the other three Malay populations. Besides, substantial differentiation was observed between the Malay populations from the north and the south, and between those from the west and the east. In summary, this study revealed that the genetic identity of the Malays comprises a mixed entity of multiple ancestries represented by Austronesian, Proto-Malay, East Asian and South Asian, with most of the admixture events estimated to have occurred 175 to 1,500 years ago, which in turn suggests that geographical isolation and independent admixture have significantly shaped the genetic architectures and the diversity of the Malay populations.
Collapse
|
154
|
Zhou K, Bowern C. Quantifying uncertainty in the phylogenetics of Australian numeral systems. Proc Biol Sci 2015; 282:20151278. [PMID: 26378214 PMCID: PMC4614748 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1278] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2015] [Accepted: 08/20/2015] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Researchers have long been interested in the evolution of culture and the ways in which change in cultural systems can be reconstructed and tracked. Within the realm of language, these questions are increasingly investigated with Bayesian phylogenetic methods. However, such work in cultural phylogenetics could be improved by more explicit quantification of reconstruction and transition probabilities. We apply such methods to numerals in the languages of Australia. As a large phylogeny with almost universal 'low-limit' systems, Australian languages are ideal for investigating numeral change over time. We reconstruct the most likely extent of the system at the root and use that information to explore the ways numerals evolve. We show that these systems do not increment serially, but most commonly vary their upper limits between 3 and 5. While there is evidence for rapid system elaboration beyond the lower limits, languages lose numerals as well as gain them. We investigate the ways larger numerals build on smaller bases, and show that there is a general tendency to both gain and replace 4 by combining 2 + 2 (rather than inventing a new unanalysable word 'four'). We develop a series of methods for quantifying and visualizing the results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kevin Zhou
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Yale University, 55 Prospect St, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
| | - Claire Bowern
- Department of Linguistics, Yale University, 370 Temple St, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
| |
Collapse
|
155
|
Bantu expansion shows that habitat alters the route and pace of human dispersals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2015; 112:13296-301. [PMID: 26371302 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1503793112] [Citation(s) in RCA: 88] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Unlike most other biological species, humans can use cultural innovations to occupy a range of environments, raising the intriguing question of whether human migrations move relatively independently of habitat or show preferences for familiar ones. The Bantu expansion that swept out of West Central Africa beginning ∼5,000 y ago is one of the most influential cultural events of its kind, eventually spreading over a vast geographical area a new way of life in which farming played an increasingly important role. We use a new dated phylogeny of ∼400 Bantu languages to show that migrating Bantu-speaking populations did not expand from their ancestral homeland in a "random walk" but, rather, followed emerging savannah corridors, with rainforest habitats repeatedly imposing temporal barriers to movement. When populations did move from savannah into rainforest, rates of migration were slowed, delaying the occupation of the rainforest by on average 300 y, compared with similar migratory movements exclusively within savannah or within rainforest by established rainforest populations. Despite unmatched abilities to produce innovations culturally, unfamiliar habitats significantly alter the route and pace of human dispersals.
Collapse
|
156
|
Abstract
Our understanding of human evolutionary and population history can be advanced by ecological and evolutionary studies of our parasites. Many parasites flourish only in the presence of very specific human behaviors and in specific habitats, are wholly dependent on us, and have evolved with us for thousands or millions of years. Therefore, by asking when and how we first acquired those parasites, under which environmental and cultural conditions we are the most susceptible, and how the parasites have evolved and adapted to us and we in response to them, we can gain considerable insight into our own evolutionary history. As examples, the tapeworm life cycle is dependent on our consumption of meat, the divergence of body and head lice may have been subsequent to the development of clothing, and malaria hyperendemicity may be associated with agriculture. Thus, the evolutionary and population histories of these parasites are likely intertwined with critical aspects of human biology and culture. Here I review the mechanics of these and multiple other parasite proxies for human evolutionary history and discuss how they currently complement our fossil, archeological, molecular, linguistic, historical, and ethnographic records. I also highlight potential future applications of this promising model for the field of evolutionary anthropology.
Collapse
|
157
|
Affiliation(s)
- Jared Diamond
- Department of Geography, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90095-1524, USA
| |
Collapse
|
158
|
Roberts SG. Commentary: Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice vs. wheat agriculture. Front Psychol 2015. [PMID: 26217274 PMCID: PMC4493317 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00950] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Seán G Roberts
- Language and Cognition Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
159
|
Adaptive Communication: Languages with More Non-Native Speakers Tend to Have Fewer Word Forms. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0128254. [PMID: 26083380 PMCID: PMC4470635 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128254] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2014] [Accepted: 04/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Explaining the diversity of languages across the world is one of the central aims of typological, historical, and evolutionary linguistics. We consider the effect of language contact-the number of non-native speakers a language has-on the way languages change and evolve. By analysing hundreds of languages within and across language families, regions, and text types, we show that languages with greater levels of contact typically employ fewer word forms to encode the same information content (a property we refer to as lexical diversity). Based on three types of statistical analyses, we demonstrate that this variance can in part be explained by the impact of non-native speakers on information encoding strategies. Finally, we argue that languages are information encoding systems shaped by the varying needs of their speakers. Language evolution and change should be modeled as the co-evolution of multiple intertwined adaptive systems: On one hand, the structure of human societies and human learning capabilities, and on the other, the structure of language.
Collapse
|
160
|
Longobardi G, Ghirotto S, Guardiano C, Tassi F, Benazzo A, Ceolin A, Barbujani G. Across language families: Genome diversity mirrors linguistic variation within Europe. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 2015; 157:630-40. [PMID: 26059462 PMCID: PMC5095809 DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.22758] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2015] [Revised: 04/02/2015] [Accepted: 04/14/2015] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Objectives: The notion that patterns of linguistic and biological variation may cast light on each other and on population histories dates back to Darwin's times; yet, turning this intuition into a proper research program has met with serious methodological difficulties, especially affecting language comparisons. This article takes advantage of two new tools of comparative linguistics: a refined list of Indo‐European cognate words, and a novel method of language comparison estimating linguistic diversity from a universal inventory of grammatical polymorphisms, and hence enabling comparison even across different families. We corroborated the method and used it to compare patterns of linguistic and genomic variation in Europe. Materials and Methods: Two sets of linguistic distances, lexical and syntactic, were inferred from these data and compared with measures of geographic and genomic distance through a series of matrix correlation tests. Linguistic and genomic trees were also estimated and compared. A method (Treemix) was used to infer migration episodes after the main population splits. Results: We observed significant correlations between genomic and linguistic diversity, the latter inferred from data on both Indo‐European and non‐Indo‐European languages. Contrary to previous observations, on the European scale, language proved a better predictor of genomic differences than geography. Inferred episodes of genetic admixture following the main population splits found convincing correlates also in the linguistic realm. Discussion: These results pave the ground for previously unfeasible cross‐disciplinary analyses at the worldwide scale, encompassing populations of distant language families. Am J Phys Anthropol 157:630–640, 2015. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giuseppe Longobardi
- Department of Language and Linguistic Science, University of York, York, UK.,Department of Humanities, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
| | - Silvia Ghirotto
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Cristina Guardiano
- Department of Communication and Economics, University of Modena-Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
| | - Francesca Tassi
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Andrea Benazzo
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| | - Andrea Ceolin
- Department of Language and Linguistic Science, University of York, York, UK
| | - Guido Barbujani
- Department of Life Sciences and Biotechnology, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
161
|
Abstract
Among 7100 languages spoken on Earth, the Koreanic language is the 13th largest, with about 77 million speakers in and around the Korean Peninsula. In comparison to other languages of similar size, however, surprisingly little is known about the evolution of the Koreanic language. This is mainly due to two reasons. The first reason is that the genealogical relationship of the Koreanic to other neighboring languages remains uncertain, and thus inference from the linguistic comparative method provides only provisional evidence. The second reason is that, as the ancestral Koreanic speakers lacked their own writing system until around 500 years ago, there are scant historical materials to peer into the past, except for those preserved in Sinitic characters that we have no straightforward way of interpreting. Here I attempt to overcome these disadvantages and shed some light on the linguistic history of the Korean Peninsula, by analyzing the internal variation of the Koreanic language with methods adopted from evolutionary biology. The preliminary results presented here suggest that the evolutionary history of the Koreanic language is characterized by a weak hierarchical structure, and intensive gene/culture flows within the Korean Peninsula seem to have promoted linguistic homogeneity among the Koreanic variants. Despite the gene/culture flows, however, there are still three detectable linguistic barriers in the Korean Peninsula that appear to have been shaped by geographical features such as mountains, elevated areas, and ocean. I discuss these findings in an inclusive manner to lay the groundwork for future studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sean Lee
- Department of Biological Sciences, School of Science, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
162
|
Aguilar E, Ghirlanda S. Modeling the genealogy of a cultural trait. Theor Popul Biol 2015; 101:1-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tpb.2014.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2013] [Revised: 12/23/2014] [Accepted: 12/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
163
|
|
164
|
Endicott P. Introduction: revisiting the "negrito" hypothesis: a transdisciplinary approach to human prehistory in southeast Asia. Hum Biol 2015; 85:7-20. [PMID: 24297218 DOI: 10.3378/027.085.0301] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/02/2013] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
The "negrito" hypothesis predicts that a shared phenotype among various contemporary groups of hunter-gatherers in Southeast Asia--dark skin, short stature, tight curly hair--is due to common descent from a region-wide, pre-Neolithic substrate of humanity. The alternative is that their distinctive phenotype results from convergent evolution. The core issues of the negrito hypothesis are today more relevant than ever to studies of human evolution, including the out-of-Africa migration, admixture with Denisovans, and the effects of environment and ecology on life-history traits. Understanding the current distribution of the negrito phenotype dictates a wide-ranging remit for study, including the articulation of the relationship between foragers and farmers in the present, the development of settled agriculture in the mid-Holocene, and terminal Pleistocene population expansions. The consensus reached by the contributors to this special double issue of Human Biology is that there is not yet conclusive evidence either for or against the negrito hypothesis. Nevertheless, the process of revisiting the problem will benefit the knowledge of the human prehistory of Southeast Asia. Whether the term negrito accurately reflects the all-encompassing nature of the resulting inquiry is in itself questionable, but the publication of this double issue is testament to the enduring ability of this hypothesis to unite disparate academic disciplines in a common purpose.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Phillip Endicott
- Musée de l'Homme, Paris, France and Museum National d'Historie Naturelle, 57 rue Cuvier, Paris, 75005 France
| |
Collapse
|
165
|
Chaubey G, Endicott P. The Andaman Islanders in a regional genetic context: reexamining the evidence for an early peopling of the archipelago from South Asia. Hum Biol 2015; 85:153-72. [PMID: 24297224 DOI: 10.3378/027.085.0307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/02/2013] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
The indigenous inhabitants of the Andaman Islands were considered by many early anthropologists to be pristine examples of a "negrito" substrate of humanity that existed throughout Southeast Asia. Despite over 150 years of research and study, questions over the extent of shared ancestry between Andaman Islanders and other small-bodied, gracile, dark-skinned populations throughout the region are still unresolved. This shared phenotype could be a product of shared history, evolutionary convergence, or a mixture of both. Recent population genetic studies have tended to emphasize long-term physical isolation of the Andaman Islanders and an affinity to ancestral populations of South Asia. We reexamine the genetic evidence from genome-wide autosomal single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) data for a shared history between the tribes of Little Andaman (Onge) and Great Andaman, and between these two groups and the rest of South and Southeast Asia (both negrito and non-negrito groups).
Collapse
|
166
|
Pugach I, Stoneking M. Genome-wide insights into the genetic history of human populations. INVESTIGATIVE GENETICS 2015; 6:6. [PMID: 25834724 PMCID: PMC4381409 DOI: 10.1186/s13323-015-0024-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2014] [Accepted: 03/05/2015] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Although mtDNA and the non-recombining Y chromosome (NRY) studies continue to provide valuable insights into the genetic history of human populations, recent technical, methodological and computational advances and the increasing availability of large-scale, genome-wide data from contemporary human populations around the world promise to reveal new aspects, resolve finer points, and provide a more detailed look at our past demographic history. Genome-wide data are particularly useful for inferring migrations, admixture, and fine structure, as well as for estimating population divergence and admixture times and fluctuations in effective population sizes. In this review, we highlight some of the stories that have emerged from the analyses of genome-wide SNP genotyping data concerning the human history of Southern Africa, India, Oceania, Island South East Asia, Europe and the Americas and comment on possible future study directions. We also discuss advantages and drawbacks of using SNP-arrays, with a particular focus on the ascertainment bias, and ways to circumvent it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irina Pugach
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, D04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Mark Stoneking
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, D04103 Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
167
|
van den Berg P, Molleman L, Weissing FJ. Focus on the success of others leads to selfish behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2015; 112:2912-7. [PMID: 25730855 PMCID: PMC4352783 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1417203112] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
It has often been argued that the spectacular cognitive capacities of humans are the result of selection for the ability to gather, process, and use information about other people. Recent studies show that humans strongly and consistently differ in what type of social information they are interested in. Although some individuals mainly attend to what the majority is doing (frequency-based learning), others focus on the success that their peers achieve with their behavior (success-based learning). Here, we show that such differences in social learning have important consequences for the outcome of social interactions. We report on a decision-making experiment in which individuals were first classified as frequency- and success-based learners and subsequently grouped according to their learning strategy. When confronted with a social dilemma situation, groups of frequency-based learners cooperated considerably more than groups of success-based learners. A detailed analysis of the decision-making process reveals that these differences in cooperation are a direct result of the differences in information use. Our results show that individual differences in social learning strategies are crucial for understanding social behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pieter van den Berg
- Theoretical Biology Group, University of Groningen, 9747 AG Groningen, The Netherlands; and
| | - Lucas Molleman
- Theoretical Biology Group, University of Groningen, 9747 AG Groningen, The Netherlands; and The Centre for Decision Research and Experimental Economics, University of Nottingham, Nottingham NG7 2RD, United Kingdom
| | - Franz J Weissing
- Theoretical Biology Group, University of Groningen, 9747 AG Groningen, The Netherlands; and
| |
Collapse
|
168
|
Affiliation(s)
- Keith Hunley
- Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131
| |
Collapse
|
169
|
Tamariz M, Ellison TM, Barr DJ, Fay N. Cultural selection drives the evolution of human communication systems. Proc Biol Sci 2015; 281:20140488. [PMID: 24966310 PMCID: PMC4083785 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0488] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Human communication systems evolve culturally, but the evolutionary mechanisms that drive this evolution are not well understood. Against a baseline that communication variants spread in a population following neutral evolutionary dynamics (also known as drift models), we tested the role of two cultural selection models: coordination- and content-biased. We constructed a parametrized mixed probabilistic model of the spread of communicative variants in four 8-person laboratory micro-societies engaged in a simple communication game. We found that selectionist models, working in combination, explain the majority of the empirical data. The best-fitting parameter setting includes an egocentric bias and a content bias, suggesting that participants retained their own previously used communicative variants unless they encountered a superior (content-biased) variant, in which case it was adopted. This novel pattern of results suggests that (i) a theory of the cultural evolution of human communication systems must integrate selectionist models and (ii) human communication systems are functionally adaptive complex systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Monica Tamariz
- School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9LL, UK
| | - T Mark Ellison
- School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia 6009, Australia
| | - Dale J Barr
- Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QB, UK
| | - Nicolas Fay
- School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia 6009, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
170
|
Abstract
The effect of population size on patterns and rates of language evolution is controversial. Do languages with larger speaker populations change faster due to a greater capacity for innovation, or do smaller populations change faster due to more efficient diffusion of innovations? Do smaller populations suffer greater loss of language elements through founder effects or drift, or do languages with more speakers lose features due to a process of simplification? Revealing the influence of population size on the tempo and mode of language evolution not only will clarify underlying mechanisms of language change but also has practical implications for the way that language data are used to reconstruct the history of human cultures. Here, we provide, to our knowledge, the first empirical, statistically robust test of the influence of population size on rates of language evolution, controlling for the evolutionary history of the populations and formally comparing the fit of different models of language evolution. We compare rates of gain and loss of cognate words for basic vocabulary in Polynesian languages, an ideal test case with a well-defined history. We demonstrate that larger populations have higher rates of gain of new words whereas smaller populations have higher rates of word loss. These results show that demographic factors can influence rates of language evolution and that rates of gain and loss are affected differently. These findings are strikingly consistent with general predictions of evolutionary models.
Collapse
|
171
|
Abstract
Linguists have long identified sound changes that occur in parallel. Now novel research shows how Bayesian modeling can capture complex concerted changes, revealing how evolution of sounds proceeds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claire Bowern
- Department of Linguistics, 370 Temple St, Rm 313, New Haven, CT 06511, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
172
|
Currie TE, Mace R. Evolution of cultural traits occurs at similar relative rates in different world regions. Proc Biol Sci 2014; 281:20141622. [PMID: 25297866 PMCID: PMC4213619 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1622] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2014] [Accepted: 09/09/2014] [Indexed: 02/04/2023] Open
Abstract
A fundamental issue in understanding human diversity is whether or not there are regular patterns and processes involved in cultural change. Theoretical and mathematical models of cultural evolution have been developed and are increasingly being used and assessed in empirical analyses. Here, we test the hypothesis that the rates of change of features of human socio-cultural organization are governed by general rules. One prediction of this hypothesis is that different cultural traits will tend to evolve at similar relative rates in different world regions, despite the unique historical backgrounds of groups inhabiting these regions. We used phylogenetic comparative methods and systematic cross-cultural data to assess how different socio-cultural traits changed in (i) island southeast Asia and the Pacific, and (ii) sub-Saharan Africa. The relative rates of change in these two regions are significantly correlated. Furthermore, cultural traits that are more directly related to external environmental conditions evolve more slowly than traits related to social structures. This is consistent with the idea that a form of purifying selection is acting with greater strength on these more environmentally linked traits. These results suggest that despite contingent historical events and the role of humans as active agents in the historical process, culture does indeed evolve in ways that can be predicted from general principles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas E Currie
- Department of Biosciences, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Penryn Campus, Cornwall TR10 9EZ, UK Department of Anthropology, University College London, 14 Taviton St., London WC1H 0BH, UK
| | - Ruth Mace
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, 14 Taviton St., London WC1H 0BH, UK
| |
Collapse
|
173
|
Eisenstein J, O'Connor B, Smith NA, Xing EP. Diffusion of lexical change in social media. PLoS One 2014; 9:e113114. [PMID: 25409166 PMCID: PMC4237389 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 94] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2014] [Accepted: 10/20/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Computer-mediated communication is driving fundamental changes in the nature of written language. We investigate these changes by statistical analysis of a dataset comprising 107 million Twitter messages (authored by 2.7 million unique user accounts). Using a latent vector autoregressive model to aggregate across thousands of words, we identify high-level patterns in diffusion of linguistic change over the United States. Our model is robust to unpredictable changes in Twitter's sampling rate, and provides a probabilistic characterization of the relationship of macro-scale linguistic influence to a set of demographic and geographic predictors. The results of this analysis offer support for prior arguments that focus on geographical proximity and population size. However, demographic similarity - especially with regard to race - plays an even more central role, as cities with similar racial demographics are far more likely to share linguistic influence. Rather than moving towards a single unified "netspeak" dialect, language evolution in computer-mediated communication reproduces existing fault lines in spoken American English.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacob Eisenstein
- School of Interactive Computing, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia, United States of America
| | - Brendan O'Connor
- School of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Noah A. Smith
- School of Computer Science, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Eric P. Xing
- School of Computer Science, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
174
|
Ho LST, Ané C. Intrinsic inference difficulties for trait evolution with Ornstein-Uhlenbeck models. Methods Ecol Evol 2014. [DOI: 10.1111/2041-210x.12285] [Citation(s) in RCA: 124] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Lam Si Tung Ho
- Department of Statistics; University of Wisconsin; 1300 University Ave. Madison WI 53706 USA
| | - Cécile Ané
- Department of Statistics; University of Wisconsin; 1300 University Ave. Madison WI 53706 USA
- Department of Botany; University of Wisconsin; Madison WI USA
| |
Collapse
|
175
|
Abstract
Although ecological forces are known to shape the expression of sociality across a broad range of biological taxa, their role in shaping human behavior is currently disputed. Both comparative and experimental evidence indicate that beliefs in moralizing high gods promote cooperation among humans, a behavioral attribute known to correlate with environmental harshness in nonhuman animals. Here we combine fine-grained bioclimatic data with the latest statistical tools from ecology and the social sciences to evaluate the potential effects of environmental forces, language history, and culture on the global distribution of belief in moralizing high gods (n = 583 societies). After simultaneously accounting for potential nonindependence among societies because of shared ancestry and cultural diffusion, we find that these beliefs are more prevalent among societies that inhabit poorer environments and are more prone to ecological duress. In addition, we find that these beliefs are more likely in politically complex societies that recognize rights to movable property. Overall, our multimodel inference approach predicts the global distribution of beliefs in moralizing high gods with an accuracy of 91%, and estimates the relative importance of different potential mechanisms by which this spatial pattern may have arisen. The emerging picture is neither one of pure cultural transmission nor of simple ecological determinism, but rather a complex mixture of social, cultural, and environmental influences. Our methods and findings provide a blueprint for how the increasing wealth of ecological, linguistic, and historical data can be leveraged to understand the forces that have shaped the behavior of our own species.
Collapse
|
176
|
Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behav Brain Sci 2014; 39:e30. [PMID: 25347943 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x1400106x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 220] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
Human cooperation is highly unusual. We live in large groups composed mostly of non-relatives. Evolutionists have proposed a number of explanations for this pattern, including cultural group selection and extensions of more general processes such as reciprocity, kin selection, and multi-level selection acting on genes. Evolutionary processes are consilient; they affect several different empirical domains, such as patterns of behavior and the proximal drivers of that behavior. In this target article, we sketch the evidence from five domains that bear on the explanatory adequacy of cultural group selection and competing hypotheses to explain human cooperation. Does cultural transmission constitute an inheritance system that can evolve in a Darwinian fashion? Are the norms that underpin institutions among the cultural traits so transmitted? Do we observe sufficient variation at the level of groups of considerable size for group selection to be a plausible process? Do human groups compete, and do success and failure in competition depend upon cultural variation? Do we observe adaptations for cooperation in humans that most plausibly arose by cultural group selection? If the answer to one of these questions is "no," then we must look to other hypotheses. We present evidence, including quantitative evidence, that the answer to all of the questions is "yes" and argue that we must take the cultural group selection hypothesis seriously. If culturally transmitted systems of rules (institutions) that limit individual deviance organize cooperation in human societies, then it is not clear that any extant alternative to cultural group selection can be a complete explanation.
Collapse
|
177
|
Abstract
The ordering of subject, verb, and object is one of the fundamental components of the syntax of natural languages. The distribution of basic word orders across the world's languages is highly nonuniform, with the majority of languages being either subject-object-verb (SOV) or subject-verb-object (SVO). Explaining this fact using psychological accounts of language acquisition or processing requires understanding how the present distribution has resulted from ancestral distributions and the rates of change between orders. We show that Bayesian phylogenetics can provide quantitative answers to three important questions: how word orders are likely to change over time, which word orders were dominant historically, and whether strong inferences about the origins of syntax can be drawn from modern languages. We find that SOV to SVO change is more common than the reverse and VSO to SVO change is more common than VSO to SOV, and that if the seven language families we consider share a common ancestor then that common ancestor likely had SOV word order, but also that there are limits on how confidently we can make inferences about ancestral word order based on modern-day observations. These results shed new light on old questions from historical linguistics and provide clear targets for psychological explanations of word-order distributions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luke Maurits
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94704
| | | |
Collapse
|
178
|
Pakendorf B. Coevolution of languages and genes. Curr Opin Genet Dev 2014; 29:39-44. [PMID: 25170984 DOI: 10.1016/j.gde.2014.07.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2014] [Revised: 07/08/2014] [Accepted: 07/25/2014] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
The evolution of languages shares certain characteristics with that of genes, such as the predominantly vertical line of transmission and the retention of traces of past events such as contact. Thus, studies of language phylogenies and their correlations with genetic phylogenies can enrich our understanding of human prehistory, while insights gained from genetic studies of past population contact can help shed light on the processes underlying language contact and change. As demonstrated by recent research, these evolutionary processes are more complex than simple models of gene-language coevolution predict, with linguistic boundaries only occasionally functioning as barriers to gene flow. More frequently, admixture takes place irrespective of linguistic differences, but with a detectable impact of contact-induced changes in the languages concerned.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brigitte Pakendorf
- Laboratoire Dynamique du Langage, UMR5596, CNRS & Université Lyon Lumière 2, Lyon, France.
| |
Collapse
|
179
|
Dunn M. Evolutionary Linguistics by April McMahon and Robert McMahon. AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST 2014. [DOI: 10.1111/aman.12136_17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
180
|
Duggan AT, Stoneking M. Recent developments in the genetic history of East Asia and Oceania. Curr Opin Genet Dev 2014; 29:9-14. [PMID: 25170982 DOI: 10.1016/j.gde.2014.06.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2014] [Accepted: 06/30/2014] [Indexed: 01/11/2023]
Abstract
Recent developments in our understanding of the genetic history of Asia and Oceania have been driven by technological advances. Specifically, our understanding of the past has been augmented by: genome sequences from ancient hominins and ancient modern humans; more comprehensive studies of existing populations (e.g., complete mtDNA genome sequences and genome-wide data) and the development of new statistics and analytical methods to interpret the abundance of new data. We review some of the new discoveries since we entered the age of archaic and modern genomics and how they have changed our understanding of the settlement and subsequent population dynamics in Asia and the Pacific.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ana T Duggan
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, D04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Mark Stoneking
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, D04103 Leipzig, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
181
|
Lipson M, Loh PR, Patterson N, Moorjani P, Ko YC, Stoneking M, Berger B, Reich D. Reconstructing Austronesian population history in Island Southeast Asia. Nat Commun 2014; 5:4689. [PMID: 25137359 PMCID: PMC4143916 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms5689] [Citation(s) in RCA: 130] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2014] [Accepted: 07/14/2014] [Indexed: 12/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Austronesian languages are spread across half the globe, from Easter Island to Madagascar. Evidence from linguistics and archaeology indicates that the ‘Austronesian expansion,’ which began 4,000–5,000 years ago, likely had roots in Taiwan, but the ancestry of present-day Austronesian-speaking populations remains controversial. Here, we analyse genome-wide data from 56 populations using new methods for tracing ancestral gene flow, focusing primarily on Island Southeast Asia. We show that all sampled Austronesian groups harbour ancestry that is more closely related to aboriginal Taiwanese than to any present-day mainland population. Surprisingly, western Island Southeast Asian populations have also inherited ancestry from a source nested within the variation of present-day populations speaking Austro-Asiatic languages, which have historically been nearly exclusive to the mainland. Thus, either there was once a substantial Austro-Asiatic presence in Island Southeast Asia, or Austronesian speakers migrated to and through the mainland, admixing there before continuing to western Indonesia. Populations speaking Austronesian languages are numerous and widespread, but their history remains controversial. Here, the authors analyse genetic data from Southeast Asia and show that all populations harbour ancestry most closely related to aboriginal Taiwanese, while some also contain a component closest to Austro-Asiatic speakers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Lipson
- Department of Mathematics and Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139, USA
| | - Po-Ru Loh
- 1] Department of Mathematics and Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139, USA [2]
| | - Nick Patterson
- Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02142, USA
| | - Priya Moorjani
- 1] Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02142, USA [2] Department of Genetics, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115, USA [3]
| | - Ying-Chin Ko
- Graduate Institute of Clinical Medical Science, China Medical University, Taichung 40402, Taiwan
| | - Mark Stoneking
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | - Bonnie Berger
- 1] Department of Mathematics and Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139, USA [2] Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02142, USA
| | - David Reich
- 1] Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02142, USA [2] Department of Genetics, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115, USA [3] Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115, USA
| |
Collapse
|
182
|
Li YH, Chu HP, Jiang YN, Lin CY, Li SH, Li KT, Weng GJ, Cheng CC, Lu DJ, Ju YT. Empirical Selection of Informative Microsatellite Markers within Co-ancestry Pig Populations Is Required for Improving the Individual Assignment Efficiency. ASIAN-AUSTRALASIAN JOURNAL OF ANIMAL SCIENCES 2014; 27:616-27. [PMID: 25049996 PMCID: PMC4093195 DOI: 10.5713/ajas.2013.13365] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2013] [Revised: 09/12/2013] [Accepted: 12/12/2013] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
The Lanyu is a miniature pig breed indigenous to Lanyu Island, Taiwan. It is distantly related to Asian and European pig breeds. It has been inbred to generate two breeds and crossed with Landrace and Duroc to produce two hybrids for laboratory use. Selecting sets of informative genetic markers to track the genetic qualities of laboratory animals and stud stock is an important function of genetic databases. For more than two decades, Lanyu derived breeds of common ancestry and crossbreeds have been used to examine the effectiveness of genetic marker selection and optimal approaches for individual assignment. In this paper, these pigs and the following breeds: Berkshire, Duroc, Landrace and Yorkshire, Meishan and Taoyuan, TLRI Black Pig No. 1, and Kaohsiung Animal Propagation Station Black pig are studied to build a genetic reference database. Nineteen microsatellite markers (loci) provide information on genetic variation and differentiation among studied breeds. High differentiation index (FST) and Cavalli-Sforza chord distances give genetic differentiation among breeds, including Lanyu's inbred populations. Inbreeding values (FIS) show that Lanyu and its derived inbred breeds have significant loss of heterozygosity. Individual assignment testing of 352 animals was done with different numbers of microsatellite markers in this study. The testing assigned 99% of the animals successfully into their correct reference populations based on 9 to 14 markers ranking D-scores, allelic number, expected heterozygosity (HE) or FST, respectively. All miss-assigned individuals came from close lineage Lanyu breeds. To improve individual assignment among close lineage breeds, microsatellite markers selected from Lanyu populations with high polymorphic, heterozygosity, FST and D-scores were used. Only 6 to 8 markers ranking HE, FST or allelic number were required to obtain 99% assignment accuracy. This result suggests empirical examination of assignment-error rates is required if discernible levels of co-ancestry exist. In the reference group, optimum assignment accuracy was achievable achieved through a combination of different markers by ranking the heterozygosity, FST and allelic number of close lineage populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Y H Li
- Taitung Animal Propagation Station, Livestock Research Institute, Taitung, Taiwan
| | - H P Chu
- Taitung Animal Propagation Station, Livestock Research Institute, Taitung, Taiwan
| | - Y N Jiang
- Taitung Animal Propagation Station, Livestock Research Institute, Taitung, Taiwan
| | - C Y Lin
- Taitung Animal Propagation Station, Livestock Research Institute, Taitung, Taiwan
| | - S H Li
- Department of Life Science, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - K T Li
- Institute of History and Philology, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - G J Weng
- Institute of Wildlife Conservation, National Pingtung University of Science and Technology, Pintung, Taiwan
| | - C C Cheng
- Graduate Institute of Hakka Cultural Industry, National Pingtung University of Science and Technology, Pintung, Taiwan
| | - D J Lu
- School of Forestry and Resource Conservation, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Y T Ju
- Taitung Animal Propagation Station, Livestock Research Institute, Taitung, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
183
|
Rana NP, Weerakkody V, Dwivedi YK, Piercy NC. Profiling Existing Research on Social Innovation in the Public Sector. INFORMATION SYSTEMS MANAGEMENT 2014. [DOI: 10.1080/10580530.2014.923271] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
184
|
Chang JS, Lin SM, Chao JCJ, Chen YC, Wang CM, Chou NH, Pan WH, Bai CH. Serum ferritin contributes to racial or geographic disparities in metabolic syndrome in Taiwan. Public Health Nutr 2014; 17:1498-506. [PMID: 23866264 PMCID: PMC10282300 DOI: 10.1017/s1368980013001596] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/19/2012] [Revised: 04/17/2013] [Accepted: 05/08/2013] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Asians and Pacific Islanders have higher circulating serum ferritin (SF) compared with Caucasians but the clinical significance of this is unclear. There is a higher prevalence of metabolic syndrome (MetS) in Taiwanese Indigenous than Han Chinese. Genetically, Indigenous are related to Austronesians and account for 2 % of Taiwan's population. We tested the hypothesis that accumulation of Fe in the body contributes to the ethnic/racial disparities in MetS in Taiwan. DESIGN A population-based, cross-sectional study. SETTING National Nutrition and Health Survey in Taiwan and Penghu Island. SUBJECTS A total of 2638 healthy adults aged ≥19 years. Three ethnic groups were included. RESULTS Han Chinese and Indigenous people had comparable levels of SF. Austronesia origin was independently associated with MetS (OR = 2·61, 95 % CI 2·02, 3·36). After multiple adjustments, the odds for MetS (OR = 2·49, 95 % CI 1·15, 5·28) was significantly higher among Indigenous people in the highest SF tertile compared with those in the lowest tertile. Hakka and Penghu Islanders yielded the lowest risks (OR = 1·08, 95 % CI 0·44, 2·65 and OR = 1·21, 95 % CI 0·52, 2·78, respectively). Indigenous people in the highest SF tertile had increased risk for abnormal levels of fasting glucose (OR = 2·34, 95 % CI 1·27, 4·29), TAG (OR = 1·94, 95 % CI 1·11, 3·39) and HDL-cholesterol (OR = 2·10, 95 % CI 1·18, 3·73) than those in the lowest SF tertile. CONCLUSIONS Our results raise the possibility that ethnic/racial differences in body Fe store susceptibility may contribute to racial and geographic disparities in MetS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jung-Su Chang
- School of Nutrition and Health Sciences, Taipei Medical University, Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | - Shiue-Ming Lin
- Department of Public Health, College of Medicine, Taipei Medical University, 250 Wu-Hsing Street, Taipei City, Taiwan 110, Republic of China
| | - Jane C-J Chao
- School of Nutrition and Health Sciences, Taipei Medical University, Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | - Yi-Chun Chen
- School of Nutrition and Health Sciences, Taipei Medical University, Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | - Chi-Mei Wang
- School of Nutrition and Health Sciences, Taipei Medical University, Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | - Ni-Hsin Chou
- School of Nutrition and Health Sciences, Taipei Medical University, Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | - Wen-Harn Pan
- Institute of Biomedical Science, Academia Sinica, Nankang, Taipei, Taiwan, Republic of China
- Institute of Population Health Sciences, National Health Research Institutes, Zhunan, Miaoli County, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | - Chyi-Huey Bai
- Department of Public Health, College of Medicine, Taipei Medical University, 250 Wu-Hsing Street, Taipei City, Taiwan 110, Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
185
|
|
186
|
Gamermann D, Montagud A, Conejero JA, Urchueguía JF, de Córdoba PF. New approach for phylogenetic tree recovery based on genome-scale metabolic networks. J Comput Biol 2014; 21:508-19. [PMID: 24611553 PMCID: PMC4082356 DOI: 10.1089/cmb.2013.0150] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
A wide range of applications and research has been done with genome-scale metabolic models. In this work, we describe an innovative methodology for comparing metabolic networks constructed from genome-scale metabolic models and how to apply this comparison in order to infer evolutionary distances between different organisms. Our methodology allows a quantification of the metabolic differences between different species from a broad range of families and even kingdoms. This quantification is then applied in order to reconstruct phylogenetic trees for sets of various organisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Gamermann
- Cátedra Energesis de Tecnología Interdisciplinar, Universidad Católica de Valencia San Vicente Mártir, Valencia, Spain
- Instituto Universitario de Matemática Pura y Aplicada, Universidad Politécnica de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | - Arnaud Montagud
- Instituto Universitario de Matemática Pura y Aplicada, Universidad Politécnica de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | - J. Alberto Conejero
- Instituto Universitario de Matemática Pura y Aplicada, Universidad Politécnica de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | - Javier F. Urchueguía
- Instituto Universitario de Matemática Pura y Aplicada, Universidad Politécnica de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | - Pedro Fernández de Córdoba
- Instituto Universitario de Matemática Pura y Aplicada, Universidad Politécnica de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
187
|
Lee S, Hasegawa T. Oceanic barriers promote language diversification in the Japanese Islands. J Evol Biol 2014; 27:1905-12. [PMID: 24953224 DOI: 10.1111/jeb.12442] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2014] [Accepted: 05/30/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Good barriers make good languages. Scholars have long speculated that geographical barriers impede linguistic contact between speech communities and promote language diversification in a manner similar to the process of allopatric speciation. This hypothesis, however, has seldom been tested systematically and quantitatively. Here, we adopt methods from evolutionary biology and attempt to quantify the influence of oceanic barriers on the degree of lexical diversity in the Japanese Islands. Measuring the degree of beta diversity from basic vocabularies, we find that geographical proximity and, more importantly, isolation by surrounding ocean, independently explains a significant proportion of lexical variation across Japonic languages. Further analyses indicate that our results are neither a by-product of using a distance matrix derived from a Bayesian language phylogeny nor an epiphenomenon of accelerated evolutionary rates in languages spoken by small communities. Moreover, we find that the effect of oceanic barriers is reproducible with the Ainu languages, indicating that our analytic approach as well as the results can be generalized beyond Japonic language family. The findings we report here are the first quantitative evidence that physical barriers formed by ocean can influence language diversification and points to an intriguing common mechanism between linguistic and biological evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Lee
- Department of Biological Sciences, Graduate School of Science, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
188
|
Zeng Z, Rowold DJ, Garcia-Bertrand R, Calderon S, Regueiro M, Li L, Zhong M, Herrera RJ. Taiwanese aborigines: genetic heterogeneity and paternal contribution to Oceania. Gene 2014; 542:240-7. [PMID: 24613753 DOI: 10.1016/j.gene.2014.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2014] [Accepted: 03/04/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
In the present study, for the first time, 293 Taiwanese aboriginal males from all nine major tribes (Ami, Atayal, Bunun, Rukai, Paiwan, Saisat, Puyuma, Tsou, Yami) were genotyped with 17 YSTR loci in a attend to reveal migrational patterns connected with the Austronesian expansion. We investigate the paternal genetic relationships of these Taiwanese aborigines to 42 Asia-Pacific reference populations, geographically selected to reflect various locations within the Austronesian domain. The Tsou and Puyuma tribes exhibit the lowest (0.1851) and the highest (0.5453) average total genetic diversity, respectively. Further, the fraction of unique haplotypes is also relatively high in the Puyuma (86.7%) and low in Tsou (33.3%) suggesting different demographic histories. Multidimensional scaling (MDS) and analysis of molecular variance (AMOVA) revealed several notable findings: 1) the Taiwan indigenous populations are highly diverse. In fact, the level of inter-population heterogeneity displayed by the Taiwanese aboriginal populations is close to that exhibited among all 51 Asia-Pacific populations examined; 2) the asymmetrical contribution of the Taiwanese aborigines to the Oceanic groups. Ami, Bunun and Saisiyat tribes exhibit the strongest paternal links to the Solomon and Polynesian island communities, whereas most of the remaining Taiwanese aboriginal groups are more genetically distant to these Oceanic inhabitants; 3) the present YSTR analyses does not reveal a strong paternal affinity of the nine Taiwanese tribes to their continental Asian neighbors. Overall, our current findings suggest that, perhaps, only a few of the tribes were involved in the migration out of Taiwan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhaoshu Zeng
- Department of Forensic Medicine, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Zhengzhou University, China
| | - Diane J Rowold
- Foundation for Applied Molecular Science (FfAME), Gainesville, FL 32601, USA
| | | | - Silvia Calderon
- Department of Dentistry, New York University, New York, NY, USA
| | | | - Li Li
- Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology, Zhengzhou Central Hospital, Zhengzhou University, China
| | - Mingxia Zhong
- Department of Forensic Medicine, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Zhengzhou University, China
| | | |
Collapse
|
189
|
Gosling AL, Matisoo-Smith E, Merriman TR. Hyperuricaemia in the Pacific: why the elevated serum urate levels? Rheumatol Int 2014; 34:743-57. [PMID: 24378761 DOI: 10.1007/s00296-013-2922-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2013] [Accepted: 12/13/2013] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
Pacific Island populations, particularly those of Polynesian descent, have a high prevalence of hyperuricaemia and gout. This is due to an inherently higher urate level among these populations with a demonstrated genetic predisposition. While an excess of urate can cause pathology, urate is also important for human health. It has been implicated as an antioxidant, has a neuroprotective role and is involved in innate immune responses. This paper provides a brief review of urate levels worldwide, with a particular focus on island Southeast Asia and the Pacific. We then present possible evolutionary explanations for the elevated serum urate levels among Pacific populations in the context of the physiological importance of urate and of the settlement history of the region. Finally, we propose that ancestry may play a significant role in hyperuricaemia in these populations and that exposure to malaria prior to population expansion into the wider Pacific may have driven genetic selection for variants contributing to high serum urate.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna L Gosling
- Department of Anatomy, Allan Wilson Centre for Molecular Ecology and Evolution, University of Otago, PO Box 913, Dunedin, New Zealand,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
190
|
Duggan A, Evans B, Friedlaender F, Friedlaender J, Koki G, Merriwether D, Kayser M, Stoneking M. Maternal history of Oceania from complete mtDNA genomes: contrasting ancient diversity with recent homogenization due to the Austronesian expansion. Am J Hum Genet 2014; 94:721-33. [PMID: 24726474 DOI: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.03.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2014] [Accepted: 03/18/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Archaeology, linguistics, and existing genetic studies indicate that Oceania was settled by two major waves of migration. The first migration took place approximately 40 thousand years ago and these migrants, Papuans, colonized much of Near Oceania. Approximately 3.5 thousand years ago, a second expansion of Austronesian-speakers arrived in Near Oceania and the descendants of these people spread to the far corners of the Pacific, colonizing Remote Oceania. To assess the female contribution of these two human expansions to modern populations and to investigate the potential impact of other migrations, we obtained 1,331 whole mitochondrial genome sequences from 34 populations spanning both Near and Remote Oceania. Our results quantify the magnitude of the Austronesian expansion and demonstrate the homogenizing effect of this expansion on almost all studied populations. With regards to Papuan influence, autochthonous haplogroups support the hypothesis of a long history in Near Oceania, with some lineages suggesting a time depth of 60 thousand years, and offer insight into historical interpopulation dynamics. Santa Cruz, a population located in Remote Oceania, is an anomaly with extreme frequencies of autochthonous haplogroups of Near Oceanian origin; simulations to investigate whether this might reflect a pre-Austronesian versus Austronesian settlement of the island failed to provide unequivocal support for either scenario.
Collapse
|
191
|
Sand A, Holt MK, Johansen J, Brodal GS, Mailund T, Pedersen CNS. tqDist: a library for computing the quartet and triplet distances between binary or general trees. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2014; 30:2079-80. [PMID: 24651968 DOI: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu157] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED tqDist is a software package for computing the triplet and quartet distances between general rooted or unrooted trees, respectively. The program is based on algorithms with running time [Formula: see text] for the triplet distance calculation and [Formula: see text] for the quartet distance calculation, where n is the number of leaves in the trees and d is the degree of the tree with minimum degree. These are currently the fastest algorithms both in theory and in practice. AVAILABILITY AND IMPLEMENTATION tqDist can be installed on Windows, Linux and Mac OS X. Doing this will install a set of command-line tools together with a Python module and an R package for scripting in Python or R. The software package is freely available under the GNU LGPL licence at http://birc.au.dk/software/tqDist.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andreas Sand
- Bioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, DenmarkBioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Morten K Holt
- Bioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Jens Johansen
- Bioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Gerth Stølting Brodal
- Bioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, DenmarkBioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Thomas Mailund
- Bioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Christian N S Pedersen
- Bioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, DenmarkBioinformatics Research Centre, Department of Computer Science and MADALGO, Center for Massive Data Algorithms, a Center of the Danish National Research Foundation, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
192
|
Early Austronesians: into and out of Taiwan. Am J Hum Genet 2014; 94:426-36. [PMID: 24607387 DOI: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 92] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2013] [Accepted: 02/10/2014] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
A Taiwan origin for the expansion of the Austronesian languages and their speakers is well supported by linguistic and archaeological evidence. However, human genetic evidence is more controversial. Until now, there had been no ancient skeletal evidence of a potential Austronesian-speaking ancestor prior to the Taiwan Neolithic ~6,000 years ago, and genetic studies have largely ignored the role of genetic diversity within Taiwan as well as the origins of Formosans. We address these issues via analysis of a complete mitochondrial DNA genome sequence of an ~8,000-year-old skeleton from Liang Island (located between China and Taiwan) and 550 mtDNA genome sequences from 8 aboriginal (highland) Formosan and 4 other Taiwanese groups. We show that the Liangdao Man mtDNA sequence is closest to Formosans, provides a link to southern China, and has the most ancestral haplogroup E sequence found among extant Austronesian speakers. Bayesian phylogenetic analysis allows us to reconstruct a history of early Austronesians arriving in Taiwan in the north ~6,000 years ago, spreading rapidly to the south, and leaving Taiwan ~4,000 years ago to spread throughout Island Southeast Asia, Madagascar, and Oceania.
Collapse
|
193
|
Saslis-Lagoudakis CH, Hawkins JA, Greenhill SJ, Pendry CA, Watson MF, Tuladhar-Douglas W, Baral SR, Savolainen V. The evolution of traditional knowledge: environment shapes medicinal plant use in Nepal. Proc Biol Sci 2014; 281:20132768. [PMID: 24523269 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Traditional knowledge is influenced by ancestry, inter-cultural diffusion and interaction with the natural environment. It is problematic to assess the contributions of these influences independently because closely related ethnic groups may also be geographically close, exposed to similar environments and able to exchange knowledge readily. Medicinal plant use is one of the most important components of traditional knowledge, since plants provide healthcare for up to 80% of the world's population. Here, we assess the significance of ancestry, geographical proximity of cultures and the environment in determining medicinal plant use for 12 ethnic groups in Nepal. Incorporating phylogenetic information to account for plant evolutionary relatedness, we calculate pairwise distances that describe differences in the ethnic groups' medicinal floras and floristic environments. We also determine linguistic relatedness and geographical separation for all pairs of ethnic groups. We show that medicinal uses are most similar when cultures are found in similar floristic environments. The correlation between medicinal flora and floristic environment was positive and strongly significant, in contrast to the effects of shared ancestry and geographical proximity. These findings demonstrate the importance of adaptation to local environments, even at small spatial scale, in shaping traditional knowledge during human cultural evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Haris Saslis-Lagoudakis
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Reading, , Reading RG6 6BX, UK, Imperial College London, , Silwood Park, Ascot, Berkshire SL5 7PY, UK, Centre for Macroevolution and Macroecology, Research School of Biology, Australian National University, , Canberra, Australian Capital Territory 0200, Australia, School of Culture, History and Language, ANU College of Asia and the Pacific, Australian National University, , Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia, Royal Botanic Garden Edinburgh, , Edinburgh EH3 5LR, UK, School of Divinity, History and Philosophy, King's College, University of Aberdeen, , Aberdeen AB24 3UB, UK, Department of Plant Resources, National Herbarium and Plant Laboratories, , PO Box 3708, Godawari, Lalitpur, Nepal, Royal Botanic Gardens, Kew TW9 3DS, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
194
|
Delfin F, Min-Shan Ko A, Li M, Gunnarsdóttir ED, Tabbada KA, Salvador JM, Calacal GC, Sagum MS, Datar FA, Padilla SG, De Ungria MCA, Stoneking M. Complete mtDNA genomes of Filipino ethnolinguistic groups: a melting pot of recent and ancient lineages in the Asia-Pacific region. Eur J Hum Genet 2014; 22:228-37. [PMID: 23756438 PMCID: PMC3895641 DOI: 10.1038/ejhg.2013.122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2013] [Revised: 05/03/2013] [Accepted: 05/08/2013] [Indexed: 12/23/2022] Open
Abstract
The Philippines is a strategic point in the Asia-Pacific region for the study of human diversity, history and origins, as it is a cross-road for human migrations and consequently exhibits enormous ethnolinguistic diversity. Following on a previous in-depth study of Y-chromosome variation, here we provide new insights into the maternal genetic history of Filipino ethnolinguistic groups by surveying complete mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) genomes from a total of 14 groups (11 groups in this study and 3 groups previously published) including previously published mtDNA hypervariable segment (HVS) data from Filipino regional center groups. Comparison of HVS data indicate genetic differences between ethnolinguistic and regional center groups. The complete mtDNA genomes of 14 ethnolinguistic groups reveal genetic aspects consistent with the Y-chromosome, namely: diversity and heterogeneity of groups, no support for a simple dichotomy between Negrito and non-Negrito groups, and different genetic affinities with Asia-Pacific groups that are both ancient and recent. Although some mtDNA haplogroups can be associated with the Austronesian expansion, there are others that associate with South Asia, Near Oceania and Australia that are consistent with a southern migration route for ethnolinguistic group ancestors into the Asia-Pacific, with a timeline that overlaps with the initial colonization of the Asia-Pacific region, the initial colonization of the Philippines and a possible separate post-colonization migration into the Philippine archipelago.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Frederick Delfin
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany
- DNA Analysis Laboratory, Natural Sciences Research Institute, University of the Philippines, Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
| | - Albert Min-Shan Ko
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Mingkun Li
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Ellen D Gunnarsdóttir
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany
- deCODE Genetics, Sturlugata 8, 101 Reykjavic, Iceland
| | - Kristina A Tabbada
- DNA Analysis Laboratory, Natural Sciences Research Institute, University of the Philippines, Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
- The Babraham Institute, Babraham Research Campus, Cambridge, UK
| | - Jazelyn M Salvador
- DNA Analysis Laboratory, Natural Sciences Research Institute, University of the Philippines, Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
| | - Gayvelline C Calacal
- DNA Analysis Laboratory, Natural Sciences Research Institute, University of the Philippines, Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
| | - Minerva S Sagum
- DNA Analysis Laboratory, Natural Sciences Research Institute, University of the Philippines, Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
| | - Francisco A Datar
- Department of Anthropology, College of Social Sciences and Philosophy, Faculty Center, University of the Philippines, Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
| | - Sabino G Padilla
- Department of Behavioral Sciences, College of Arts and Sciences, University of the Philippines, Manila, Ermita, Manila, Philippines
- AnthroWatch.org, Quezon City, Philippines
| | - Maria Corazon A De Ungria
- DNA Analysis Laboratory, Natural Sciences Research Institute, University of the Philippines, Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
| | - Mark Stoneking
- Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
195
|
Linz B, Vololonantenainab CRR, Seck A, Carod JF, Dia D, Garin B, Ramanampamonjy RM, Thiberge JM, Raymond J, Breurec S. Population genetic structure and isolation by distance of Helicobacter pylori in Senegal and Madagascar. PLoS One 2014; 9:e87355. [PMID: 24498084 PMCID: PMC3907543 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087355] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2013] [Accepted: 12/19/2013] [Indexed: 01/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Helicobacter pylori has probably infected the human stomach since our origins and subsequently diversified in parallel with their human hosts. The genetic population history of H. pylori can therefore be used as a marker for human migration. We analysed seven housekeeping gene sequences of H. pylori strains isolated from 78 Senegalese and 24 Malagasy patients and compared them with the sequences of strains from other geographical locations. H. pylori from Senegal and Madagascar can be placed in the previously described HpAfrica1 genetic population, subpopulations hspWAfrica and hspSAfrica, respectively. These 2 subpopulations correspond to the distribution of Niger-Congo speakers in West and most of subequatorial Africa (due to Bantu migrations), respectively. H. pylori appears as a single population in Senegal, indicating a long common history between ethnicities as well as frequent local admixtures. The lack of differentiation between these isolates and an increasing genetic differentiation with geographical distance between sampling locations in Africa was evidence for genetic isolation by distance. The Austronesian expansion that started from Taiwan 5000 years ago dispersed one of the 10 subgroups of the Austronesian language family via insular Southeast Asia into the Pacific and Madagascar, and hspMaori is a marker for the entire Austronesian expansion. Strain competition and replacement of hspMaori by hpAfrica1 strains from Bantu migrants are the probable reasons for the presence of hspSAfrica strains in Malagasy of Southeast Asian descent. hpAfrica1 strains appear to be generalist strains that have the necessary genetic diversity to efficiently colonise a wide host spectrum.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bodo Linz
- Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | | | - Abdoulaye Seck
- Laboratoire de Biologie Médicale, Institut Pasteur, Dakar, Senegal
| | | | - Daouda Dia
- Centre Hospitalier Le Dantec, Département de Gastro-entérologie, Dakar, Senegal
| | - Benoit Garin
- Laboratoire de Biologie Médicale, Institut Pasteur, Dakar, Senegal
- Laboratoire de Bactériologie expérimentale, Institut Pasteur, Antananarivo, Madagascar
| | - Rado Manitrala Ramanampamonjy
- Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Joseph Ravoahangy Befelatanana, Département de Gastro-entérologie, Antananarivo, Madagascar
| | - Jean-Michel Thiberge
- Plate-forme Génotypage des Pathogènes et Santé Publique, Institut Pasteur, Paris, France
| | - Josette Raymond
- Unité Postulante Pathogenèse de Helicobacter, Institut Pasteur, Paris, France
| | - Sebastien Breurec
- Laboratoire de Biologie Médicale, Institut Pasteur, Dakar, Senegal
- Institut Pasteur, Laboratoire de Biologie médicale, Bangui, République Centrafricaine
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
196
|
The mutation spectrum of the phenylalanine hydroxylase (PAH) gene and associated haplotypes reveal ethnic heterogeneity in the Taiwanese population. J Hum Genet 2014; 59:145-52. [PMID: 24401910 DOI: 10.1038/jhg.2013.136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2013] [Revised: 11/29/2013] [Accepted: 12/05/2013] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Phenylalanine hydroxylase (PAH) deficiency is responsible for most cases of phenylketonuria (PKU). In this study of the PAH mutation spectrum in the Taiwanese population, 139 alleles were identified including 34 different mutations. The V190G, Q267R and F392I mutations are first reported in this study. The most common mutations, R241C, R408Q and Ex6-96A>G, account for 23.2%, 12.0% and 9.2%, of the mutant alleles, respectively. Haplotype analysis shows that R241C and Ex6-96A>G are exclusively associated with haplotype 4.3 to suggest founder effects. On the other hand, R408Q is found on two distinct haplotypes suggesting recurrent mutations. The spectrum of PAH mutations in Taiwan shows various links to those of other Asian regions, yet remarkable differences exist. Notably, R408Q, E286K and -4173_-407del, accounting for 21% of all mutant alleles in Taiwan, are very rare or are undetected among PKU cohorts of other Asian regions to suggest local founder effects. Moreover, the low homozygosity value of 0.092 hints at a high degree of ethnic heterogeneity within the Taiwanese population. Our study of PAH mutation spectrum and the associated haplotypes is useful for subsequent study on the origin and migration pattern via Taiwan, an island at the historical crossroad of migration of ancient populations.
Collapse
|
197
|
Affiliation(s)
- Tom Güldemann
- Humboldt University Berlin / Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig
| |
Collapse
|
198
|
Heyer E, Georges M, Pachner M, Endicott P. Genetic diversity of four Filipino negrito populations from Luzon: comparison of male and female effective population sizes and differential integration of immigrants into Aeta and Agta communities. Hum Biol 2013; 85:189-208. [PMID: 24297226 DOI: 10.3378/027.085.0310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/02/2013] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
Genetic data corresponding to four negrito populations (two Aeta and two Agta; n = 120) from the Luzon region of the Philippines have been analyzed. These data comprise mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) hypervariable segment 1 haplotypes and haplogroups, Y-chromosome haplogroups and short tandem repeats (STRs), autosomal STRs, and X-chromosome STRs. The genetic diversity and structure of the populations were investigated at a local, regional, and interregional level. We found a high level of autosomal differentiation, combined with no significant reduction in diversity, consistent with long-term settlement of the Luzon region by the ancestors of the Agta and Aeta followed by reduced gene flow between these two ethnolinguistic groups. Collectively, the Aeta have a much higher ratio of female:male effective population size than do the Agta, a finding that supports phylogenetic analysis of their mtDNA and Y-chromosome haplogroups, which suggests different genetic sex-biased contributions from putative Austronesian source populations. We propose that factors of social organization that led to the reduction in Agta female effective population size may also be linked to the limited incorporation of female lineages associated with the settlement of the Philippines by Austronesian speakers; conversely, the reduction in Aeta male effective population size, relative to females, could be indicative of a limited incorporation of male lineages associated with this demographic process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Heyer
- UMR7206, EcoAnthropologie et Ethnobiologie, MNHN, CNRS, Université Paris Diderot, Paris, France
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
199
|
Abstract
Researchers have long been fascinated by the strong continuities evident in the oral traditions associated with different cultures. According to the 'historic-geographic' school, it is possible to classify similar tales into "international types" and trace them back to their original archetypes. However, critics argue that folktale traditions are fundamentally fluid, and that most international types are artificial constructs. Here, these issues are addressed using phylogenetic methods that were originally developed to reconstruct evolutionary relationships among biological species, and which have been recently applied to a range of cultural phenomena. The study focuses on one of the most debated international types in the literature: ATU 333, 'Little Red Riding Hood'. A number of variants of ATU 333 have been recorded in European oral traditions, and it has been suggested that the group may include tales from other regions, including Africa and East Asia. However, in many of these cases, it is difficult to differentiate ATU 333 from another widespread international folktale, ATU 123, 'The Wolf and the Kids'. To shed more light on these relationships, data on 58 folktales were analysed using cladistic, Bayesian and phylogenetic network-based methods. The results demonstrate that, contrary to the claims made by critics of the historic-geographic approach, it is possible to identify ATU 333 and ATU 123 as distinct international types. They further suggest that most of the African tales can be classified as variants of ATU 123, while the East Asian tales probably evolved by blending together elements of both ATU 333 and ATU 123. These findings demonstrate that phylogenetic methods provide a powerful set of tools for testing hypotheses about cross-cultural relationships among folktales, and point towards exciting new directions for research into the transmission and evolution of oral narratives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jamshid J. Tehrani
- Department of Anthropology and Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, Durham University, Science Site, South Road, Durham, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
200
|
Brown S, Savage PE, Ko AMS, Stoneking M, Ko YC, Loo JH, Trejaut JA. Correlations in the population structure of music, genes and language. Proc Biol Sci 2013; 281:20132072. [PMID: 24225453 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2072] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023] Open
Abstract
We present, to our knowledge, the first quantitative evidence that music and genes may have coevolved by demonstrating significant correlations between traditional group-level folk songs and mitochondrial DNA variation among nine indigenous populations of Taiwan. These correlations were of comparable magnitude to those between language and genes for the same populations, although music and language were not significantly correlated with one another. An examination of population structure for genetics showed stronger parallels to music than to language. Overall, the results suggest that music might have a sufficient time-depth to retrace ancient population movements and, additionally, that it might be capturing different aspects of population history than language. Music may therefore have the potential to serve as a novel marker of human migrations to complement genes, language and other markers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven Brown
- Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster University, , 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada , L8S 4K1, Department of Musicology, Tokyo University of the Arts, , Tokyo, Japan, Department of Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, , Leipzig, Germany, Graduate Institute of Clinical Medical Science, China Medical University, , Taichung, Taiwan, Republic of China, Mackay Memorial Hospital, , New Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|