1
|
Mindthoff A, Evans JR, Compo NS, Polanco K, Hagsand AV. No evidence that low levels of intoxication at both encoding and retrieval impact scores on the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2021; 238:1633-1644. [PMID: 33646341 DOI: 10.1007/s00213-021-05797-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2020] [Accepted: 02/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
RATIONALE It is not uncommon for police to question alcohol-intoxicated witnesses and suspects; yet, the full extent to which intoxication impacts individuals' suggestibility in the investigative interviewing context remains unclear. OBJECTIVE The present study sought to measure the effect of alcohol-intoxication on interviewee suggestibility by implementing a standardized suggestibility test with participants whose intoxication-state was the same at both encoding and recall. METHODS We randomly assigned participants (N = 165) to an intoxicated (mean breath alcohol level [BrAC] at encoding = 0.06%, and BrAC at retrieval = 0.07%), active placebo (participants believed they consumed alcohol but only consumed an insignificant amount to enhance believability), or control (participants knowingly remained sober) group. An experimenter then implemented the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale (GSS), which produced free recall outcomes (number of correct details and memory confabulations) and suggestibility outcomes (yielding to leading questions and changing answers in response to negative feedback from the experimenter). RESULTS Intoxicated participants recalled fewer correct details than did placebo and control participants but did not make more confabulation errors. No effects of intoxication on suggestibility measures emerged. CONCLUSIONS Moderately intoxicated interviewees may not be more suggestible during investigative interviews than sober interviewees. However, before concrete evidence-based policy recommendations are made to law enforcement, further research is needed examining the effects of alcohol on suggestibility in conditions that are more reflective of the legal context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amelia Mindthoff
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, 11200 S.W. 8th Street, DM 256, Miami, FL, 33199, USA
- Department of Psychology, Iowa State University, 1347 Lagomarcino Hall, 901 Stange Rd., Ames, IA, 50011, USA
| | - Jacqueline R Evans
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, 11200 S.W. 8th Street, DM 256, Miami, FL, 33199, USA.
| | - Nadja Schreiber Compo
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, 11200 S.W. 8th Street, DM 256, Miami, FL, 33199, USA
| | - Karina Polanco
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, 11200 S.W. 8th Street, DM 256, Miami, FL, 33199, USA
| | - Angelica V Hagsand
- Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Haraldsgatan 1, 413 14, Gothenburg, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Luna K, Albuquerque PB. Centrality ratings, forensic relevance, and production frequency: Which one best identifies central and peripheral items? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3400] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Karlos Luna
- Psychology Research Centre; University of Minho; Braga Portugal
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Paz-Alonso PM, Goodman GS, Ibabe I. Adult eyewitness memory and compliance: effects of post-event misinformation on memory for a negative event. BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES & THE LAW 2013; 31:541-558. [PMID: 24022799 DOI: 10.1002/bsl.2081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2012] [Revised: 06/23/2012] [Accepted: 06/30/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
This study investigated effects of misleading post-event information, delay, and centrality definition on eyewitness memory and suggestibility for a negative event (a vividly filmed murder). Either immediately or 2 weeks after viewing the film, 93 adults read a (misleading or control) narrative about the event and then completed a recognition memory test. Misinformation acceptance was operative, but strong evidence for memory malleability was lacking. Compliance predicted misinformation effects, especially on the delayed test. Although accuracy was generally higher for central than peripheral information, centrality criteria influenced the pattern of results. Self-report of greater distress was associated with better recognition accuracy. The results suggest that use of different centrality definitions may partly explain inconsistencies across studies of memory and suggestibility for central and peripheral information. Moreover, social factors appeared, at least in part, to influence misinformation effects for the highly negative event, especially as memory faded. Implications for eyewitness memory and suggestibility are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pedro M Paz-Alonso
- University of California, Davis, CA, U.S.A.; Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language (BCBL), Donostia-San Sebastián, Spain
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
LaPaglia JA, Chan JCK. Testing increases suggestibility for narrative-based misinformation but reduces suggestibility for question-based misinformation. BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES & THE LAW 2013; 31:593-606. [PMID: 24105926 DOI: 10.1002/bsl.2090] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2012] [Revised: 07/30/2013] [Accepted: 08/02/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
A number of recent studies have found that recalling details of an event following its occurrence can increase people's suggestibility to later presented misinformation. However, several other studies have reported the opposite result, whereby earlier retrieval can reduce subsequent eyewitness suggestibility. In the present study, we investigated whether differences in the way misinformation is presented can modulate the effects of testing on suggestibility. Participants watched a video of a robbery and some were questioned about the event immediately afterwards. Later, participants were exposed to misinformation in a narrative (Experiment 1) or in questions (Experiment 2). Consistent with previous studies, we found that testing increased suggestibility when misinformation was presented via a narrative. Remarkably, when misinformation was presented in questions, testing decreased suggestibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica A LaPaglia
- Department of Psychology, Morningside College, 1501 Morningside Ave, Sioux City, IA, 51106, U.S.A
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Delay and déjà vu: Timing and repetition increase the power of false evidence. Psychon Bull Rev 2013; 20:812-8. [DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0398-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
6
|
Acceptance and Confidence of Central and Peripheral Misinformation. SPANISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2013; 12:405-13. [DOI: 10.1017/s1138741600001797] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
We examined the memory for central and peripheral information concerning a crime and the acceptance of false information. We also studied eyewitnesses' confidence in their memory. Participants were shown a video depicting a bank robbery and a questionnaire was used to introduce false central and peripheral information. The next day the participants completed a recognition task in which they rated the confidence of their responses. Performance was better for central information and participants registered more false alarms for peripheral contents. The cognitive system's limited attentional capacity and the greater information capacity of central elements may facilitate processing the more important information. The presentation of misinformation seriously impaired eyewitness memory by prompting a more lenient response criterion. Participants were more confident with central than with peripheral information. Eyewitness memory is easily distorted in peripheral aspects but it is more difficult to make mistakes with central information. However, when false information is introduced, errors in central information can be accompanied by high confidence, thus rendering them credible and legally serious.
Collapse
|
7
|
Bright-Paul A, Jarrold C. Children's eyewitness memory: repeating post-event misinformation reduces the distinctiveness of a witnessed event. Memory 2012; 20:818-35. [PMID: 22963045 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2012.708345] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Children may incorporate misinformation into reports of witnessed events, particularly if the misinformation is repeated. One explanation is that the misinformation trace is strengthened by repetition. Alternatively, repeating misinformation may reduce the discriminability between event and misinformation sources, increasing interference between them. We tested trace strength and distinctiveness accounts by showing 5- and 6-year-olds an event and then presenting either the "same" or "varying" items of post-event misinformation across three iterations. Performance was compared to a baseline in which misinformation was presented once. Repeating the same misinformation increased suggestibility when misinformation was erroneously attributed to both event and misinformation sources, supporting a trace strength interpretation. However, suggestibility measured by attributing misinformation solely to the event, was lower when misinformation was presented repeatedly rather than once. In contrast, identification of the correct source of the event was less likely if the misinformation was repeated, whether the same or different across iterations. Thus a reduction in the distinctiveness of sources disrupted memory for the event source. Moreover, there was strong association between memory for the event and a measure of distinctiveness of sources, which takes into account both the number of confusable source and their apparent temporal spacing from the point of retrieval.
Collapse
|
8
|
O'Neill S, Zajac R. The role of repeated interviewing in children's responses to cross-examination-style questioning. Br J Psychol 2012; 104:14-38. [PMID: 23320440 DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02096.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
The negative effect of cross-examination-style questioning on children's accuracy is likely to be due to the complex and credibility-challenging questions that characterize the interview. Given that cross-examination occurs after at least one prior interview, however, it is equally possible that repeated interviewing per se impairs children's accuracy, and that the questions asked have little bearing on children's responses. To examine this issue, 5- and 6-year-old children (n= 82) and 9- and 10-year-old children (n= 103) took part in a surprise event and were then interviewed using an analogue of direct examination. Either 1 week or 6 months later, half of the children were re-interviewed with an analogue of cross-examination designed to challenge their direct examination responses. Remaining children were re-interviewed with the same questions that were asked during direct examination. Children's accuracy decreased following their second interview, irrespective of age or delay; however, delay particularly impacted younger children's second interview performance. Children's accuracy was most impaired following a cross-examination-style interview. Overall, cross-examination-style questioning appears to be particularly detrimental to obtaining accurate event reports from children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah O'Neill
- Psychology Department, Queens College of the City University of New York, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Thoresen C, Lønnum K, Melinder A, Magnussen S. Forensic interviews with children in CSA cases: A large-sample study of Norwegian police interviews. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2009. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1534] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
10
|
Compo NS, Parker JF. Gaining insight into long-term effects of inviting speculation: Does recantation help? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2009. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1599] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
11
|
Hünefeldt T, Rossi-Arnaud C, Furia A. Effects of information type on children's interrogative suggestibility: is Theory-of-Mind involved? Cogn Process 2009; 10:199-207. [PMID: 19568780 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-009-0269-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2009] [Revised: 06/15/2009] [Accepted: 06/15/2009] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
This research was aimed at learning more about the different psychological mechanisms underlying children's suggestibility to leading questions, on the one hand, and children's suggestibility to negative feedback, on the other, by distinguishing between interview questions concerning different types of information. Results showed that, unlike the developmental pattern of children's suggestibility to leading questions, the developmental pattern of children's suggestibility to negative feedback differed depending on whether the interview questions concerned external facts (physical states and events) or internal facts (mental states and events). This difference was not manifested in response to questions concerning central versus peripheral facts. Results are interpreted in terms of the hypothesis that children's suggestibility to negative feedback is differently affected by "Theory-of-Mind" abilities than children's suggestibility to leading questions. Further research is needed in order to test this hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Hünefeldt
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome La Sapienza, Via dei Marsi 78, 00185, Rome, Italy.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Melinder A, Gilstrap LL. The relationships between child and forensic interviewer behaviours and individual differences in interviews about a medical examination. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2009. [DOI: 10.1080/17405620701210445] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
13
|
Paz-Alonso PM, Goodman GS. Trauma and memory: effects of post-event misinformation, retrieval order, and retention interval. Memory 2008; 16:58-75. [PMID: 17852727 DOI: 10.1080/09658210701363146] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
The present study concerned effects of misinformation, retrieval order, and retention interval on eyewitness memory for a traumatic event (a vivid murder). Relations between misinformation acceptance and compliance were also examined. The classic three-stage misinformation paradigm (Loftus, 1979) was employed, with a multi-component recognition test added. Either immediately or 2 weeks after viewing a distressing film, 232 adults read a narrative (misleading or control) about the murder and then took a recognition test that tapped memory for central and peripheral details. Test-item order either matched the chronology of the film or was randomly determined. Significant misinformation effects were obtained. Moreover, control participants were more accurate in response to questions about central than peripheral information; however, this was not so for misinformed participants. Sequential but not random retrieval order resulted in a higher proportion of correct responses for central as opposed to peripheral misinformation questions. Compliance was significantly related to misinformation effects. Delay increased participants' suggestibility, impaired memory accuracy, and produced higher confidence ratings for misinformed participants compared to controls. Findings indicate that even for a highly negative event, adults' memory is not immune to inaccuracies and suggestive influences.
Collapse
|
14
|
Krähenbühl S, Blades M. The effect of question repetition within interviews on young children’s eyewitness recall. J Exp Child Psychol 2006; 94:57-67. [PMID: 16438980 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.12.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2005] [Revised: 11/24/2005] [Accepted: 12/07/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
This study investigated the influence of question repetition and question type (answerable, unanswerable, or opinion seeking) on children's recall. A total of 136 children (5-, 7-, and 9-year-olds) watched a live 15-min presentation. One week later, the children were asked 20 questions that were repeated an additional two times within the interview. Accuracy of children's responses to unanswerable questions declined with repetition. Children were more likely to change a response to an unanswerable question than to an answerable question. Overall, children maintained the same answers to only three-quarters of the repeated questions. The most common pattern of change was for children to change their answer the second time a question was asked and then to maintain that answer when questioned again. The high percentage of changed answers within a single interview has important implications for forensic interviewing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Krähenbühl
- Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S10 2TN, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
BACKGROUND Research into the effect of interviewing techniques has been predominantly within the paradigm of eyewitness testimony. This review focuses on the issues of questioning and examines whether children's responses are affected by questioning techniques, and whether these effects are generic to all interviewing contexts. METHODS Systematic literature searches were used to identify areas of concern and current findings in research on interviewing young children (aged 4-12). RESULTS The style and wording of questioning can affect children's responses and accuracy positively and negatively. These effects were especially apparent in interviews with the youngest children. CONCLUSIONS The implications of these findings are relevant in all contexts where an adult questions a child. It has been demonstrated that interviewing techniques can affect responses from children and that it is therefore imperative that interviewers are aware of, understand and control their influence in order to elicit complete, accurate and reliable information from the child.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Krähenbühl
- Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Connolly DA, Price HL. Children’s suggestibility for an instance of a repeated event versus a unique event: The effect of degree of association between variable details. J Exp Child Psychol 2006; 93:207-23. [PMID: 16111696 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2004] [Revised: 05/29/2005] [Accepted: 06/06/2005] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Are children who experience an event repeatedly more suggestible about an instance of the event than children who experience it once? Researchers have answered this question both in the affirmative and in the negative. In this study, we hypothesized that the degree of association between details that changed across instantiations of the event would help to explain the discrepancy. Preschoolers (4- and 5-year-olds) and first graders (6- and 7-year-olds) participated in either a single play session or four repeated play sessions, each of which contained 16 critical details. Across play sessions in the repeat-event condition, half of the critical details were associated and half were not associated. During a biasing interview 2 weeks later, children were misinformed about half of the critical details. The next day, children answered free and cued recall questions about the target play session. Among older children, repeat-event participants were more suggestible than single-event participants, especially for high-association details. Among younger children, repeat-event participants were more suggestible than single-event participants for low-association details. Consistent with some current theories of children's memory, older children were more suggestible than younger children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Deborah A Connolly
- Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada V5A 1S6.
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Abstract
Past research using a deferred imitation task has shown that 6-month-olds remember a 3-part action sequence for only 1 day. The concept of a time window suggests that there is a limited period within which additional information can be integrated with a prior memory. Its width tracks the forgetting function of the memory. This study asked if retrieving the memory of the modeled actions at the end of the time window protracts its retention, if the type of retrieval (active or passive) differentially influences retention, and if the retrieval delay influences its specificity. In Experiment 1, 6-month-olds either imitated the modeled actions (active retrieval group) or merely watched them modeled again (passive retrieval group) 1 day after the original demonstration. Both groups showed deferred imitation after 10 days. In Experiment 2, 6-month-olds who repeatedly retrieved the memory at or near the end of the time window deferred imitation for 2.5 months. In Experiment 3,6-month-olds spontaneously generalized imitation late in the time window after 1 prior retrieval, whether it was active or passive. These studies reveal that the retention benefit of multiple retrievals late in the time window is huge. Because most retrievals are undoubtedly latent, the contribution of repeated events to the growth of the knowledge base early in infancy has been greatly underestimated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel Barr
- Department of Psychology Georgetown University
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Shapiro LR, Blackford C, Chen CF. Eyewitness memory for a simulated misdemeanor crime: the role of age and temperament in suggestibility. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
19
|
Price HL, Connolly DA. Event frequency and children's suggestibility: a study of cued recall responses. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
20
|
Schreiber N, Parker JF. Inviting witnesses to speculate: effects of age and interaction on children's recall. J Exp Child Psychol 2004; 89:31-52. [PMID: 15336917 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2003] [Revised: 03/23/2004] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Inviting speculation has been found to increase children's false recall. In this study, kindergartners and third graders saw a clown perform actions alone or in interaction with a child. Two weeks later, the speculation group recalled all actions and was asked to speculate on half the actions. The control group recalled all actions without speculating. Four weeks after the show, all children recalled all actions again. The speculation group gave more false answers to the speculated items than the control group. Surprisingly, older children tended to report as many if not more false responses than younger children, regardless of speculation. In the speculation group, there were fewer false answers for interactions than actions, but false answers did not differ across observation types in the control group. Finally, speculation did not affect free and cued recall differentially.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nadja Schreiber
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Roebers CM, Gelhaar T, Schneider W. “It’s magic!” The effects of presentation modality on children’s event memory, suggestibility, and confidence judgments. J Exp Child Psychol 2004; 87:320-35. [PMID: 15050457 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2003] [Revised: 01/26/2004] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The current study investigated the influence of presentation modality (live, video, and slide show) on children's memory, suggestibility, recognition, and metamemorial monitoring processes. A total of 270 children in three age groups (5- and 6-year-olds, 7- and 8-year-olds, and 9- and 10-year-olds) watched a magic show and were questioned about it 1 week later. The live show yielded more correct answers to nonleading questions, higher resistance to misleading questions, and better recognition memory than did the video condition, which in turn resulted in better performance than did the slide show. Although presentation modality raised the general level of memory performance, the effects were equally strong in all age groups and did not affect memory phenomena such as the size of the misinformation effect and confidence judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia M Roebers
- Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg, 97070 Würzburg, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Ein Falschinformationseffekt - klassischerweise im Zusammenhang mit Zeugenaussagen untersucht - tritt auf, wenn nach der Enkodierung von Originalinformation falsche Detailinformationen suggeriert und in die Gedächtnisaussage übernommen werden. Es wird ein Modell vorgestellt, welches die Informationsverarbeitungsprozesse beschreibt, die dem Falschinformationseffekt zugrunde liegen. Dem Modell inhärent sind dabei sowohl gedächtnis- wie sozialpsychologische Annahmen, und es kann eine große Anzahl von Befunden aus beiden Bereichen integriert werden. Besonders bedeutsam innerhalb des Modells sind die Variablen Unsicherheit, metakognitive Schlussfolgerungsprozesse und sozialer Einfluss. Das Modell beschreibt das Zusammenwirken dieser Variablen und erlaubt Vorhersagen darüber, ob und aufgrund welcher Prozesse es zu falschen Erinnerungen kommt.
Collapse
|
23
|
Miles KL, Powell MB, Stokes MA. A comparison of the effectiveness of two suggestibility paradigms in predicting preschoolers' tendency to report a non-experienced event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
24
|
Melnyk L, Bruck M. Timing moderates the effects of repeated suggestive interviewing on children's eyewitness memory. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
25
|
Roebers CM, McConkey KM. Mental reinstatement of the misinformation context and the misinformation effect in children and adults. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2003. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.886] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
26
|
Abstract
The present work investigated the role of children's and adults' metacognitive monitoring and control processes for unbiased event recall tasks and for suggestibility. Three studies were conducted in which children and adults indicated their degree of confidence that their answers were correct after (Study 1) and before (Study 2) answering either unbiased or misleading questions or (Study 3) forced-choice recognition questions. There was a strong tendency for overestimation of confidence regardless of age and question format. However, children did not lack the principal metacognitive competencies when these questions were asked in a neutral interview. Under misleading questioning, in contrast, children's monitoring skills were seriously impaired. Within each age group, better metacognitive differentiation was positively associated with recall accuracy in the suggestive interview.
Collapse
|
27
|
Gobbo C, Mega C, Pipe ME. Does the nature of the experience influence suggestibility? A study of children's event memory. J Exp Child Psychol 2002; 81:502-30. [PMID: 11890734 DOI: 10.1006/jecp.2002.2662] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Two experiments examined the effects of event modality on children's memory and suggestibility. In Experiment 1, 3- and 5-year-old children directly participated in, observed, or listened to a narrative about an event. In an interview immediately after the event, free recall was followed by misleading or leading questions and, in turn, test recall questions. One week later children were reinterviewed. In Experiment 2, 4-year-old children either participated in or listened to a story about an event, either a single time or to a criterion level of learning. Misleading questions were presented either immediately or 1 week after learning, followed by test recall questions. Five-year-old children were more accurate than 3-year-olds and those participating were more accurate than those either observing or listening to a narrative. However, method of assessment, level of event learning, delay to testing, and variables relating to the misled items also influenced the magnitude of misinformation effects.
Collapse
|
28
|
|
29
|
Gobbo C. Commentaries and Author's Reply on: “Naturally Occurring and Suggestion- Dependent Memory Distortions:The Convergence of Disparate Research Traditions” by Giuliana Mazzoni. EUROPEAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2002. [DOI: 10.1027//1016-9040.7.1.33] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Camilla Gobbo
- Department of Developmental and Socialization Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| |
Collapse
|