1
|
Morris SL, Bridges KG. On the Identification and Use of Social versus Nonsocial Reinforcers: A Review of Research Practices. Perspect Behav Sci 2024; 47:739-761. [PMID: 39584062 PMCID: PMC11582244 DOI: 10.1007/s40614-024-00426-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/27/2024] [Indexed: 11/26/2024] Open
Abstract
Recent research has developed efficacious methods for identifying individualized social reinforcers and utilizing social reinforcers may be beneficial for several reasons. However, the relative likelihood of utilizing social versus nonsocial reinforcers in behavior-analytic research remains unclear. The aim of this study was to evaluate how likely behavior analysts are to employ social versus nonsocial reinforcers in the context of research. We pursued this aim by evaluating the types of reinforcers utilized in research published during the past 9 years in five applied behavior-analytic journals. Results suggest that researchers in applied behavior analysis have been more likely to use individualized nonsocial reinforcers than individualized social reinforcers. Moreover, when social reinforcers were employed, they were much more likely to be generic and not individualized. These data suggest there is room for improvement in the types of programmed reinforcement contingencies we use in research and ways of facilitating such improvement are discussed. Implications and directions for future research evaluating current practices, demonstrating the utility of social reinforcers, and comparing the utility of social versus nonsocial reinforcers are also discussed. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s40614-024-00426-0.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel L. Morris
- Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Goldman KJ, Martinez C, Hack GO, Hernandez R, Laureano B, Argueta T, Sams R, DeLeon IG. Correspondence between preference for and efficacy of behavioral interventions: A systematic review. J Appl Behav Anal 2024. [PMID: 39567258 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.2924] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2023] [Accepted: 11/03/2024] [Indexed: 11/22/2024]
Abstract
Understanding of the correspondence between intervention preference and efficacy is limited. We systematically reviewed 112 articles (457 cases) evaluating efficacy of and preference for behavioral interventions. We analyzed the percentage of cases for whom interventions were preferred and efficacious across broad (e.g., behavior reduction, performance, skill acquisition) and specific (e.g., noncontingent reinforcement, video modeling) intervention types. Authors reported one preferred intervention for most cases. Regarding efficacy, authors reported about half of cases as having one efficacious intervention and the other half having multiple equally efficacious interventions. The same intervention was preferred and efficacious for 74% of cases for whom authors reported one preferred and one efficacious intervention. Several specific interventions were generally preferred and efficacious across cases (e.g., digital stimuli, computer-based instruction, accumulated reinforcement, contingent reinforcement). We discuss clinical recommendations, the importance of assessing preference, and the need for research in developing protocols for assessing intervention preference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kissel J Goldman
- Department of Behavioral Psychology, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | | | - Garret O Hack
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Rachael Hernandez
- Department of Psychology, Western New England University, Springfield, MA, USA
| | - Brianna Laureano
- Department of Child and Family Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | - Tracy Argueta
- Department of Psychology, University of the Pacific, Stockton, CA, USA
| | - Reilly Sams
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Iser G DeLeon
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Pinkston JW, Cook JL, Baruni RR, Rapp JT, Deshmukh S, Miltenberger RG. Application of synchronous music reinforcement to increase walking speed: A novel approach for training intensity. J Exp Anal Behav 2024; 122:362-374. [PMID: 39402862 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.4219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2024] [Accepted: 09/22/2024] [Indexed: 11/19/2024]
Abstract
Walking is a common and preferred form of exercise. Although there are current recommendations for walking volume (e.g., steps per day), recent research has begun to distinguish volume from intensity (e.g., "brisk" walking) as an important dimension of exercise. Increasing intensity may confer health advantages beyond volume measures because it shifts cardiovascular performance to more vigorous training zones. Reinforcement-based approaches have been valuable in increasing volume measures of exercise, and the present study sought to develop a corresponding reinforcement approach to training walking intensity. For this study, we used a continuous reinforcement paradigm where music played only while walking met specified criteria; otherwise, music playback stopped. As a result, music was synchronized with walking performance. Seventeen participants walked on a nonmotorized treadmill at a self-selected pace. Across the session, different conditions arranged for music to play independent of walking speed or contingent on speed increases or decreases. An extinction component assessed performance when music was withdrawn completely. Walking speed was selectively increased and decreased by adjusting the contingencies that were arranged for music, and variability in speed increased during extinction, with both findings indicating that music was a reinforcer. Heart rate was also increased to moderate-vigorous intensities during reinforcement. The findings provide a compelling case that walking intensity can be modified by music reinforcement. We suggest that synchronous schedules may be an important foundation for future exercise technologies that are based on reinforcement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan W Pinkston
- Department of Applied Behavior Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
| | - Jennifer L Cook
- Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - Rasha R Baruni
- Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - John T Rapp
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
| | - Shreeya Deshmukh
- Department of Child and Family Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Diaz de Villegas SC, Dozier CL, Kanaman KC, Leslie SC, Kamlowsky ME. Comparison of synchronous reinforcement and accumulated reinforcement for increasing on-task behavior in preschoolers. J Appl Behav Anal 2024; 57:725-741. [PMID: 38847459 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.1080] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2023] [Accepted: 04/13/2024] [Indexed: 07/21/2024]
Abstract
In synchronous-reinforcement schedules, the duration of behavior directly controls the duration of reinforcement on a moment-to-moment basis. We replicated and extended Diaz de Villegas et al. (2020) by comparing the effects of synchronous reinforcement with two accumulated-reinforcement schedules for increasing on-task behavior for seven preschoolers. One accumulated schedule was the same as the one used in Diaz de Villegas et al. and did not include tokens, whereas the other accumulated schedule included the delivery of tokens within session. Furthermore, we assessed preference for the three reinforcement schedules. The results showed that synchronous reinforcement was effective for increasing on-task behavior for all seven participants. Furthermore, it was most effective for increasing on-task behavior for three out of seven participants and preferred by all participants. For some participants, other schedules were also similarly effective. The results are discussed with respect to implications for application.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Claudia L Dozier
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas, USA
| | - Ky C Kanaman
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas, USA
| | - Stacha C Leslie
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas, USA
| | - Marissa E Kamlowsky
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Cook JL, Baruni RR, Pinkston JW, Rapp JT, Miltenberger RG, Deshmukh S, Walker E, Tai S. Comparing stimulus preference and response force in a conjugate preparation: A replication with auditory stimulation. J Exp Anal Behav 2024; 122:11-24. [PMID: 38724460 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.915] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2023] [Accepted: 04/14/2024] [Indexed: 07/03/2024]
Abstract
This study examined a conjugate approach for evaluating auditory stimulus preference for 81 participants using force as a continuous response dimension. First, the researchers used a verbal preference assessment to evaluate each participant's preference for listening to five genres of music. This process identified high-preference and low-preference music for each participant. Thereafter, the researchers exposed each participant to the five music genres in a randomized order while using a hand dynamometer to measure their response force to increase the auditory clarity of the music. The results indicate (a) 63% of the participants' high-preference music genres corresponded to the genre for which they exerted the highest mean force and (b) most participants' low-preference music genres corresponded to the genre for which they exerted the lowest mean force. These findings are consistent with those from Davis et al. (2021) and further support using conjugate preparations for measuring the relative value of some stimulus events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer L Cook
- Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - Rasha R Baruni
- Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada
| | - Jonathan W Pinkston
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
| | - John T Rapp
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
| | | | - Shreeya Deshmukh
- Department of Child and Family Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | - Emma Walker
- Department of Child and Family Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | - Sharayah Tai
- Department of Child and Family Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Sheridan DJ, Rapp JT, Edgemon AK, Pinkston JW. Assessing stimulus preference using response force in a conjugate preparation: A replication and extension. J Exp Anal Behav 2024; 122:25-41. [PMID: 38837371 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.926] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2023] [Accepted: 05/17/2024] [Indexed: 06/07/2024]
Abstract
The current study examined 98 participants' preferences for five pictorial stimuli. The researchers used a verbal multiple-stimulus-without-replacement (VMSWO) preference assessment with each participant to identify high-preference and low-preference pictorial stimuli. Next, participants viewed each pictorial stimulus in a randomized order on a computer while using a hand dynamometer that measured the amount of force they exerted to increase or maintain the visual clarity of each image. The results indicate that over 75% of participants' force response ranks corresponded with participants' VMSWO high-preference stimuli, VMSWO low-preference stimuli, or both. The results of the current study provide further evidence for the use of conjugate schedules in the assessment of stimulus preference with potential for use as a reinforcer assessment. Implications along with directions for future research and limitations of the findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel J Sheridan
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
| | - John T Rapp
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
| | - Anna Kate Edgemon
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
| | - Jonathan W Pinkston
- Department of Psychology, Western New England University, Springfield, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Bergmann S, Long BP, St Peter CC, Brand D, Strum MD, Han JB, Wallace MD. A detailed examination of reporting procedural fidelity in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. J Appl Behav Anal 2023; 56:708-719. [PMID: 37572025 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.1015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2023] [Accepted: 07/09/2023] [Indexed: 08/14/2023]
Abstract
Few reviews on procedural fidelity-the degree to which procedures are implemented as designed-provide details to gauge the quality of fidelity reporting in behavior-analytic research. This review focused on experiments in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (2006-2021) with "integrity" or "fidelity" in the abstract or body. When fidelity data were collected, the coders characterized measurement details (e.g., description of calculation, report of single or multiple values, frequency of fidelity checks, checklist use). The researchers found increasing trends in describing the calculation(s), reporting multiple values, and stating the frequency of measurement. Few studies described using a checklist. Most studies reported fidelity as a percentage, with high obtained values (M = 97%). When not collecting fidelity data was stated as a limitation, authors were unlikely to provide a rationale for the omission. We discuss recommendations for reporting procedural fidelity to increase the quality of and transparency in behavior-analytic research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samantha Bergmann
- Department of Behavior Analysis, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA
| | - Brian P Long
- Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, USA
| | - Claire C St Peter
- Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, USA
| | - Denys Brand
- Department of Psychology, California State University, Sacramento, CA, USA
| | - Marcus D Strum
- Department of Behavior Analysis, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA
| | - Justin B Han
- Department of Child and Family Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | - Michele D Wallace
- Department of Special Education & Counseling, California State University, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
McHugh CL, Dozier CL, Diaz de Villegas SC, Kanaman NA. Using synchronous reinforcement to increase mask wearing in adults with intellectual and developmental disabilities. J Appl Behav Anal 2022; 55:1157-1171. [PMID: 35920113 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.950] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2022] [Accepted: 07/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
In 2020 the Centers for Disease Control provided the public with recommendations to slow the spread of COVID-19 by wearing a mask in the community. In the current study, experimenters coached group home staff via telehealth to implement synchronous schedules of reinforcement to increase mask wearing for 5 adults with intellectual and developmental disabilities. Results showed the intervention effectively increased mask wearing for all participants for up to 30 min. Additionally, some participants for whom we assessed generalization of mask wearing demonstrated generalization to various community environments. Furthermore, procedural integrity data suggested staff could be coached via telehealth to implement the intervention, and staff surveys suggested the procedures and coaching were socially valid.
Collapse
|
9
|
Davis WT, Rapp JT, Brogan KM, Pinkston JW, Chinnappan B. Comparing stimulus preference and response force in a conjugate preparation. J Exp Anal Behav 2021; 116:96-113. [PMID: 34261190 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.705] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2021] [Revised: 05/13/2021] [Accepted: 06/05/2021] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Researchers used a conjugate preparation to evaluate how response force changed based on participants' preferences for visual stimuli. First, researchers used a verbal preference assessment to evaluate each participant's preference for viewing for five object categories of visual stimuli; this process identified high preference (HP) stimuli and low preference stimuli for most participants. Thereafter, researchers exposed each participant to the five stimulus categories in a randomized order while using a force dynamometer to measure their response force to increase visual clarity of each stimulus. Results indicate the majority of participants' HP stimuli corresponded to the stimulus category for which they exerted the highest mean force. These preliminary findings suggest conjugate preparations involving response force may provide another option for measuring the relative value of some stimulus events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - John T Rapp
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Auburn University
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Lillie MA, Harman MJ, Hurd M, Smalley MR. Increasing passive compliance to wearing a facemask in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Appl Behav Anal 2021; 54:582-599. [PMID: 33740281 PMCID: PMC8251131 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.829] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2020] [Revised: 12/31/2020] [Accepted: 12/31/2020] [Indexed: 01/14/2023]
Abstract
The current study taught 6 children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) to increase passive compliance of wearing a facemask across sequentially increasing durations of time. A changing-criterion design embedded within a nonconcurrent multiple baseline design was used to evaluate the effectiveness of a resetting differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) without escape extinction procedure on passive compliance. Terminal probe sessions determined DRO fading intervals. Results showed that 2 participants acquired mastery level passive compliance (30 min) without fading during the initial baseline sessions. The remaining 4 participants acquired mastery level passive compliance following fading intervals within the DRO intervention. Participants' passive compliance generalized across 2 novel settings. This study replicates previous studies and extends empirical support for the use of DRO without escape extinction interventions for increasing passive compliance with medical devices in children with ASD.
Collapse
|