1
|
Militaru IE, Serapio‐García G, Ebert T, Kong W, Gosling SD, Potter J, Rentfrow PJ, Götz FM. The lay of the land: Associations between environmental features and personality. J Pers 2024; 92:88-110. [PMID: 36776098 PMCID: PMC10952236 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12822] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2022] [Revised: 01/11/2023] [Accepted: 02/06/2023] [Indexed: 02/14/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Personality traits cluster across countries, regions, cities, and neighborhoods. What drives the formation of these clusters? Ecological theory suggests that physical locations shape humans' patterns of behaviors and psychological characteristics. Based on this theory, we examined whether and how differential land-cover relates to individual personality. METHOD We followed a preregistered three-pronged analysis approach to investigate the associations between personality (N = 2,690,878) and land-cover across the United States. We used eleven land-cover categories to classify landscapes and tested their association with personality against broad physical and socioeconomic factors. RESULTS Urban areas were positively associated with openness to experience and negatively associated with conscientiousness. Coastal areas were positively associated with openness to experience and neuroticism but negatively associated with agreeableness and conscientiousness. Cultivated areas were negatively associated with openness. Landscapes at the periphery of human activity, such as shrubs, bare lands, or permanent snows, were not reliably associated with personality traits. CONCLUSIONS Bivariate correlations, multilevel, and random forest models uncovered robust associations between landscapes and personality traits. These findings align with ecological theory suggesting that an individual's environment contributes to their behaviors, thoughts, and feelings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Tobias Ebert
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of MannheimMannheimGermany
| | - Wenyuan Kong
- School of Earth and Space SciencesPeking UniversityBeijingChina
| | - Samuel D. Gosling
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of Texas at AustinAustinTexasUSA
- School of Psychological SciencesUniversity of MelbourneMelbourneAustralia
| | | | | | - Friedrich M. Götz
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverBritish ColumbiaCanada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Ng HKS, Cheung SH. Too hot to help or too cold to care? On the links between ambient temperature, volunteerism, and civic engagement. Br J Psychol 2023; 114:945-968. [PMID: 37309918 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12669] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2022] [Accepted: 05/30/2023] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
We investigated the relationship between ambient temperature and prosocial behaviour in real-life settings. It was guided by two mechanisms of opposite predictions, namely (1) higher temperatures decrease prosociality by harming well-being, and (2) higher temperatures increase prosociality by promoting the embodied cognition of social warmth. In Study 1, U.S. state-level time-series data (2002-2015) supported the first mechanism, with higher temperatures predicting lower volunteer rates through lower well-being. Study 2 furthered the investigation by probing the relationship between neighbourhood temperature and civic engagement of 2268 U.S. citizens. The data partially supported the well-being mechanism and reported findings contradictory to the social embodiment mechanism. Higher temperatures predicted lower interpersonal trust and subsequently lower civic engagement. The unexpected finding hinted at a cognitive effect of heat and a compensatory mechanism in social thermoregulation. We discussed the findings regarding their methodological strengths and weaknesses, with cautions made on ecological fallacies and alternative models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Henry Kin Shing Ng
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
| | - Sing-Hang Cheung
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Allik J, Realo A, McCrae RR. Conceptual and methodological issues in the study of the personality-and-culture relationship. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1077851. [PMID: 37057156 PMCID: PMC10088870 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1077851] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2022] [Accepted: 02/27/2023] [Indexed: 03/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Culture-and-personality studies were central to social science in the early 20th century and have recently been revived (as personality-and-culture studies) by trait and cross-cultural psychologists. In this article we comment on conceptual issues, including the nature of traits and the nature of the personality-and-culture relationship, and we describe methodological challenges in understanding associations between features of culture and aspects of personality. We give an overview of research hypothesizing the shaping of personality traits by culture, reviewing studies of indigenous traits, acculturation and sojourner effects, birth cohorts, social role changes, and ideological interventions. We also consider the possibility that aggregate traits affect culture, through psychological means and gene flow. In all these cases we highlight alternative explanations and the need for designs and analyses that strengthen the interpretation of observations. We offer a set of testable hypotheses based on the premises that personality is adequately described by Five-Factor Theory, and that observed differences in aggregate personality traits across cultures are veridical. It is clear that culture has dramatic effects on the thoughts, feelings, and behaviors from which we infer traits, but it is not yet clear whether, how, and in what degree culture shapes traits themselves.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jüri Allik
- Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
- *Correspondence: Jüri Allik,
| | - Anu Realo
- Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
- Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Szczepańska A, Kaźmierczak R. The Theoretical Model of Decision-Making Behaviour Geospatial Analysis Using Data Obtained from the Games of Chess. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2022; 19:12353. [PMID: 36231648 PMCID: PMC9566314 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph191912353] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2022] [Revised: 09/22/2022] [Accepted: 09/24/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
The game of chess offers a conducive setting to explore basic cognitive processes, including decision-making. The game exercises analytical cause-and-effect thinking skills regardless of the level of play. Moreover, chess portals provide information on the chess games played and serve as a vast database. The numbers of games played thus have the potential to be analyzed comprehensively, including for purposes other than analyzing chess matches only. The primary objective of this study is to develop a methodology for using information obtained from chess games for geospatial social analysis. The assumption is that the methodology will allow for general geographical variation in personality inference in the future, relying on big data from chess databases. Future large-scale studies of the geographical differentiation of personality traits using the developed methodology may be applicable in a number of ways. The results can be used wherever cross-sectional social analyses are needed in the context of personality traits (decision-making) to better understand their geographical background. In turn, the geographical distribution of these traits is accompanied by a range of important social, educational, health, political and economic implications.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agnieszka Szczepańska
- Department of Socio-Economic Geography, Institute of Spatial Management and Geography, Faculty of Geoengineering, University of Warmia and Mazury in Olsztyn, Prawocheńskiego 15, 10-724 Olsztyn, Poland
| | - Rafał Kaźmierczak
- Department of Spatial Analysis and Real Estate Market, Institute of Spatial Management and Geography, Faculty of Geoengineering, University of Warmia and Mazury in Olsztyn, Prawocheńskiego 15, 10-724 Olsztyn, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Borkenau P, Zaltauskas K. Effects of self‐enhancement on agreement on personality profiles. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/per.707] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Effects of self‐enhancement and socially desirable responding (SDR) on rater agreement for personality profiles were studied in 304 students. Dyads of participants described themselves and their peer on the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO‐PI‐R) that measures 30 facets of personality. In addition, participants filled in six scales measuring self‐enhancement or SDR. Data analyses focussed on moderator and suppressor effects of SDR on the similarity between self‐reported and other reported NEO‐PI‐R profiles. Three kinds of profile agreement were distinguished: (a) normative agreement; (b) distinctive agreement and (c) profile normativeness, that is, how strongly a self‐reported personality profile resembled the average profile of all participants. There were no moderator or suppressor effects on distinctive agreement, but SDR predicted profile normativeness quite strongly. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Collapse
|
6
|
Realo A, Allik J, Lönnqvist J, Verkasalo M, Kwiatkowska A, Kööts L, Kütt M, Barkauskiene R, Laurinavicius A, Karpinski K, Kolyshko A, Sebre S, Renge V. Mechanisms of the national character stereotype: How people in six neighbouring countries of Russia describe themselves and the typical Russian. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/per.719] [Citation(s) in RCA: 89] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Altogether, 1448 individuals from six neighbouring countries of Russia in the Baltic Sea region (Finland, Estonia, Latvia, Lithuania, Poland and Belarus) described a ‘typical’ member of their own nation and a ‘typical’ Russian, as well as rated their own personality, using the National Character Survey (NCS). Results suggest that national character stereotypes are widely shared, temporally stable and moderately related to assessed personality traits, if all assessments are made using the same measurement instrument. In all studied countries, agreement between national auto‐stereotypes and assessed personality was positive and in half of the samples statistically significant. Although members of the six nations studied had a relatively similar view of the Russian national character, this view was not related with self‐rated personality traits of Russians but moderately with the Russian auto‐stereotype. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anu Realo
- University of Tartu and The Estonian Center of Behavioral and Health Sciences, Estonia
| | - Jüri Allik
- University of Tartu and The Estonian Center of Behavioral and Health Sciences, Estonia
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Abstract
Culture and the human mind are deeply interdependent, because they co‐evolved. Personality traits were a preexisting feature of the primate mind and must have left an imprint on forms of culture. Trait taxonomies can structure ethnographies, by specifying institutions that reflect the operation of traits. Facets of ethos can be assessed by expert ratings or objective indicators. Ratings of ethos in Japan and the US were reliable and yielded plausible descriptions of culture. However, measures of ethos based on the analysis of stories were not meaningfully correlated with aggregate personality traits or national character stereotypes. Profiles of ethos may provide another axis that can be used with aggregate personality trait levels to predict behaviour and understand the operation of culture. Published in 2009 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert R. McCrae
- Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Abstract
In the broadest sense, personality refers to stable inter‐individual variability in behavioural organisation within a particular population. Researching personality in human as well as nonhuman species provides unique possibilities for comparisons across species with different phylogenies, ecologies and social systems. It also allows insights into mechanisms and processes of the evolution of population differences within and between species. The enormous diversity across species entails particular challenges to methodology. This paper explores theoretical approaches and analytical methods of deriving dimensions of inter‐individual variability on different population levels from a personality trait perspective. The existing diversity suggests that some populations, especially some species, may exhibit different or even unique trait domains. Therefore, a methodology is needed that identifies ecologically valid and comprehensive representations of the personality variation within each population. I taxonomise and compare current approaches in their suitability for this task. I propose a new bottom–up approach—the behavioural repertoire approach—that is tailored to the specific methodological requirements of comparative personality research. Initial empirical results in nonhuman primates emphasise the viability of this approach and highlight interesting implications for human personality research. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Collapse
|
9
|
|
10
|
Grigoryev D, Fiske ST, Batkhina A. Mapping Ethnic Stereotypes and Their Antecedents in Russia: The Stereotype Content Model. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1643. [PMID: 31379677 PMCID: PMC6646730 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01643] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2019] [Accepted: 06/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The stereotype content model (SCM), originating in the United States and generalized across nearly 50 countries, has yet to address ethnic relations in one of the world's most influential nations. Russia and the United States are somewhat alike (large, powerful, immigrant-receiving), but differ in other ways relevant to intergroup images (culture, religions, ideology, and history). Russian ethnic stereotypes are understudied, but significant for theoretical breadth and practical politics. This research tested the SCM on ethnic stereotypes in a Russian sample (N = 1115). Study 1 (N = 438) produced an SCM map of the sixty most numerous domestic ethnic groups (both ethnic minorities and immigrants). Four clusters occupied the SCM warmth-by-competence space. Study 2 (N = 677) compared approaches to ethnic stereotypes in terms of status and competition, cultural distance, perceived region, and four intergroup threats. Using the same Study 1 groups, the Russian SCM map showed correlated warmth and competence, with few ambivalent stereotypes. As the SCM predicts, status predicted competence, and competition negatively predicted warmth. Beyond the SCM, status and property threat both were robust antecedents for both competence and warmth for all groups. Besides competition, cultural distance also negatively predicted warmth for all groups. The role of the other antecedents, as expected, varied from group to group. To examine relative impact, a network analysis demonstrated that status, competition, and property threat centrally influence many other variables in the networks. The SCM, along with antecedents from other models, describes Russian ethnic-group images. This research contributes: (1) a comparison of established approaches to ethnic stereotypes (from acculturation and intergroup relations) showing the stability of the main SCM predictions; (2) network structures of the multivariate dependencies of the considered variables; (3) systematically cataloged images of ethnic groups in Russia for further comparisons, illuminating the Russian historical, societal, and interethnic context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dmitry Grigoryev
- National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Snefjella B, Schmidtke D, Kuperman V. National character stereotypes mirror language use: A study of Canadian and American tweets. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0206188. [PMID: 30462655 PMCID: PMC6248921 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206188] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2018] [Accepted: 10/07/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
National character stereotypes, or beliefs about the personality characteristics of the members of a nation, present a paradox. Such stereotypes have been argued to not be grounded in the actual personality traits of members of nations, yet they are also prolific and reliable. Stereotypes of Canadians and Americans exemplify the paradox; people in both nations strongly believe that the personality profiles of typical Canadians and Americans diverge, yet aggregated self-reports of personality profiles of Canadians and Americans show no reliable differences. We present evidence that the linguistic behavior of nations mirrors national character stereotypes. Utilizing 40 million tweets from the microblogging platform Twitter, in Study 1A we quantify the words and emojis diagnostic of Canadians and Americans. In Study 1B we explore the positivity of national language use. In Studies 2A and 2B, we present the 120 most nationally diagnostic words and emojis of each nation to naive participants, and ask them to assess personality of a hypothetical person who uses either diagnostically Canadian or American words and emojis. Personality profiles derived from the diagnostic words of each nation bear close resemblance to national character stereotypes. We therefore propose that national character stereotypes may be partially grounded in the collective linguistic behaviour of nations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bryor Snefjella
- Department of Linguistics and Languages, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
| | - Daniel Schmidtke
- Department of Linguistics and Languages, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
| | - Victor Kuperman
- Department of Linguistics and Languages, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Hřebíčková M, Mõttus R, Graf S, Jelínek M, Realo A. How Accurate Are National Stereotypes? A Test of Different Methodological Approaches. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/per.2146] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
We compared different methodological approaches in research on the accuracy of national stereotypes that use aggregated mean scores of real people's personality traits as criteria for stereotype accuracy. Our sample comprised 16,713 participants from the Central Europe and 1,090 participants from the Baltic Sea region. Participants rated national stereotypes of their own country using the National Character Survey (NCS) and their personality traits using either the Revised NEO Personality Inventory or the NCS. We examined the effects of different (i) methods for rating of real people (Revised NEO Personality Inventory vs. NCS) and national stereotypes (NCS); (ii) norms for converting raw scores into T–scores (Russian vs. international norms); and (iii) correlation techniques (intraclass correlations vs. Pearson correlations vs. rank–order correlations) on the resulting agreement between the ratings of national stereotypes and real people. We showed that the accuracy of national stereotypes depended on the employed methodology. The accuracy was the highest when ratings of real people and national stereotypes were made using the same method and when rank order correlations were used to estimate the agreement between national stereotypes and personality profiles of real people. We propose a new statistical procedure for determining national stereotype accuracy that overcomes limitations of past studies. We provide methodological recommendations applicable to a wider range of cross national stereotype accuracy studies. Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - René Mõttus
- Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Tartu, Estonia
| | - Sylvie Graf
- Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Czech Republic
- Institute of Psychology, University of Bern
| | - Martin Jelínek
- Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Czech Republic
| | - Anu Realo
- Department of Psychology, University of Tartu, Estonia
- Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, UK
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Wei W, Lu JG, Galinsky AD, Wu H, Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Yuan W, Zhang Q, Guo Y, Zhang M, Gui W, Guo XY, Potter J, Wang J, Li B, Li X, Han YM, Lv M, Guo XQ, Choe Y, Lin W, Yu K, Bai Q, Shang Z, Han Y, Wang L. Regional ambient temperature is associated with human personality. Nat Hum Behav 2017; 1:890-895. [PMID: 31024181 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-017-0240-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2017] [Accepted: 10/11/2017] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Human personality traits differ across geographical regions 1-5 . However, it remains unclear what generates these geographical personality differences. Because humans constantly experience and react to ambient temperature, we propose that temperature is a crucial environmental factor that is associated with individuals' habitual behavioural patterns and, therefore, with fundamental dimensions of personality. To test the relationship between ambient temperature and personality, we conducted two large-scale studies in two geographically large yet culturally distinct countries: China and the United States. Using data from 59 Chinese cities (N = 5,587), multilevel analyses and machine learning analyses revealed that compared with individuals who grew up in regions with less clement temperatures, individuals who grew up in regions with more clement temperatures (that is, closer to 22 °C) scored higher on personality factors related to socialization and stability (agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability) and personal growth and plasticity (extraversion and openness to experience). These relationships between temperature clemency and personality factors were replicated in a larger dataset of 12,499 ZIP-code level locations (the lowest geographical level feasible) in the United States (N = 1,660,638). Taken together, our findings provide a perspective on how and why personalities vary across geographical regions beyond past theories (subsistence style theory, selective migration theory and pathogen prevalence theory). As climate change continues across the world, we may also observe concomitant changes in human personality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wenqi Wei
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China.,Center for Biomedical Informatics, College of Medicine, Texas A&M University Health Science Center, Bryan, TX, USA
| | - Jackson G Lu
- Columbia Business School, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Adam D Galinsky
- Columbia Business School, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Han Wu
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Samuel D Gosling
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.,School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Peter J Rentfrow
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Wenjie Yuan
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Qi Zhang
- School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China
| | - Yongyu Guo
- School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China
| | - Ming Zhang
- Department of Psychology, Soochow University, Suzhou, China
| | - Wenjing Gui
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiao-Yi Guo
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | | | | | - Bingtan Li
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiaojie Li
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Yang-Mei Han
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Meizhen Lv
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiang-Qing Guo
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Yera Choe
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Weipeng Lin
- Department of Human Resource Management, Business School, Nankai University, Tianjin, China
| | - Kun Yu
- School of Labor and Human Resources, Renmin University of China, Beijing, China
| | - Qiyu Bai
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Zhe Shang
- Guanghua School of Management, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Ying Han
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Lei Wang
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Allik J, Church AT, Ortiz FA, Rossier J, Hřebíčková M, de Fruyt F, Realo A, McCrae RR. Mean Profiles of the NEO Personality Inventory. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022117692100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and its latest version, the NEO-PI-3, were designed to measure 30 distinctive personality traits, which are grouped into Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness domains. The mean self-rated NEO-PI-R scores for 30 subscales have been reported for 36 countries or cultures in 2002. As a follow-up, this study reports the mean scores of the NEO-PI-R/3 for 71,870 participants from 76 samples and 62 different countries or cultures and 37 different languages. Mean differences in personality traits across countries and cultures were about 8.5 times smaller than differences between any two individuals randomly selected from these samples. Nevertheless, a multidimensional scaling of similarities and differences in the mean profile shape showed a clear clustering into distinctive groups of countries or cultures. This study provides further evidence that country/culture mean scores in personality are replicable and can provide reliable information about personality dispositions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jüri Allik
- University of Tartu, Estonia
- Estonian Academy of Sciences, Tallinn, Estonia
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Anu Realo
- University of Tartu, Estonia
- University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Hřebíčková M, Graf S, Tegdes T, Brezina I. We are the opposite of you! Mirroring of national, regional and ethnic stereotypes. The Journal of Social Psychology 2017; 157:703-719. [PMID: 28129071 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2017.1284738] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The content of stereotypes can be shaped by multiple mechanisms, one of them possibly being the "mirroring effect." Mirroring describes a phenomenon whereby people rate their ingroup characteristics as opposite to characteristics typical of a relevant outgroup. The aim of our study was to explore mirroring in three intergroup contexts-in national, regional, and ethnic stereotypes. In Study 1, 2,241 participants rated national ingroup stereotype and outgroup stereotypes of five Central European countries. In Study 2, 741 Czech participants rated regional ingroup and outgroup stereotypes of people living in two distinct parts of the Czech Republic. In Study 3, 463 majority and Hungarian minority participants in Slovakia rated ethnic ingroup and outgroup stereotypes. The results showed a clear presence of mirroring in all three contexts.
Collapse
|
16
|
Foschi R, Lauriola M. Do Amoral Familism and Political Distrust Really Affect North–South Differences in Italy? JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022116644986] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Especially in southern Italy, Banfield’s amoral familism is considered an obstacle to the formation of associations and growth of political participation. This article discusses Banfield’s concept, showing that it has been vulgarized merely as familism and, in particular, demonstrates that Banfield intended amoral familism to be understood in terms of political distrust. We investigated whether amoral familism or political distrust, operationalized as an individual difference variable, mediated the relationships between personality traits, personal values, and conventional and unconventional political acts, controlling for differences in political attitude. We recruited 405 participants, distributed across north, central, and southern Italy, to complete a questionnaire on political participation that also assessed Big Five personality factors, values, sociability and political attitude (expertise, interest, self-efficacy), and a new scale assessing amoral familism as a form of political distrust. Regression analyses were used to identify the best predictors of political acts, then structural equation modeling was used to test a model of political participation. Like political attitudes, familism mediated the relationships between personality traits, especially “openness to experience” and “taking conventional and unconventional political acts.” However, our data do not confirm the stereotype that northern and southern Italians differ in their tendency to amoral familism as defined by Banfield.
Collapse
|
17
|
Lönnqvist JE, Konstabel K, Lönnqvist N, Verkasalo M. Accuracy, consensus, in-group bias, and cultural frame shifting in the context of national character stereotypes. The Journal of Social Psychology 2014; 154:40-58. [PMID: 24689336 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2013.843500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
We examined Finns' and bilingual Swedish-Finns' stereotypes regarding personality differences between Finns and Swedish-Finns and compared them with their respective self-ratings. Stereotype ratings by both groups converged on depicting Swedish-Finns as having a more desirable personality. In-group bias also influenced stereotypes. Contrary to predictions based on the Stereotype Content Model, out-group stereotypes were not compensatory. Consistent with the kernel of truth hypothesis of national stereotypes, Swedish-Finns' aggregate self-ratings resembled their stereotype of personality differences between the two groups, and their personality self-ratings were more desirable than Finns' self-ratings. Tentatively suggesting the occurrence of cultural frame shifting, the resemblance between Swedish-Finns' self-ratings and their stereotype of Swedish-Finns was, although only marginally statistically significantly, somewhat stronger when the self-ratings were provided in Swedish.
Collapse
|
18
|
HŘebÍČková M, Graf S. Accuracy of National Stereotypes in Central Europe: Outgroups are not Better than Ingroup in considering Personality Traits of Real People. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY 2014. [DOI: 10.1002/per.1904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
In a study on national stereotypes in central Europe—composed of Austria, the Czech Republic, Germany, Poland and Slovakia—2241 participants rated their autostereotype (a typical representative of their own country) and heterostereotypes (typical representatives of the other countries) by using National Character Survey (NCS). Existing data from 17377 participants including self–reports or observer ratings on Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NCS were compared with the national autostereotypes and heterostereotypes. Although national autostereotypes converged with personality traits of real people in Poland and an adult subsample in the Czech Republic, national heterostereotypes did not correspond to personality traits of real people in any of the studied countries. National stereotypes were shared within as well as across countries. In heterostereotypes, raters from similar cultural backgrounds speaking similar languages agreed better as compared with raters from more distant cultures. Target country played a role in agreement of raters from different countries, showed in the highest convergence between autostereotypes and heterostereotypes of a typical German. Sharing of national stereotypes is influenced by political and economic significance of the target country. Although national autostereotypes clearly differentiated between typical representatives of central European countries, the comparison of personality profiles of their inhabitants showed remarkable resemblance. Copyright © 2013 European Association of Personality Psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina HŘebÍČková
- Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Czech Republic
| | - Sylvie Graf
- Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Montesquieu hypothesis and football: players from hot countries are more expressive after scoring a goal. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013. [DOI: 10.2478/ppb-2013-0045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Analysis of sportsmen behavior enabled the authors to conduct simultaneous analysis of emotional expression of people from many distinct countries and cultures. In the study, participants from Nigeria and Poland watched all the goals scored in group matches of the 1998, 2002 and 2006 World Cups and assessed the emotions players expressed after scoring each goal on three scales (happiness, anger, and excitement). Based on the assessment of the participants, emotional expression of football players from 51 countries was analyzed. Basing on “Montesquieu hypothesis”, it was shown that players born in warmer climates (controlling for HDI of their country) express more excitement and happiness after scoring a goal. Further cross-cultural differences were also found. The results are discussed in context of previous cross-cultural studies regarding emotional expression
Collapse
|
20
|
McCrae RR, Chan W, Jussim L, De Fruyt F, Löckenhoff CE, De Bolle M, Costa PT, Hřebíčková M, Graf S, Realo A, Allik J, Nakazato K, Shimonaka Y, Yik M, Ficková E, Brunner-Sciarra M, Reátigui N, de Figueora NL, Schmidt V, Ahn CK, Ahn HN, Aguilar-Vafaie ME, Siuta J, Szmigielska B, Cain TR, Crawford JT, Mastor KA, Rolland JP, Nansubuga F, Miramontez DR, Benet-Martínez V, Rossier J, Bratko D, Marušić I, Halberstadt J, Yamaguchi M, Knežević G, Purić D, Martin TA, Gheorghiu M, Smith PB, Barbaranelli C, Wang L, Shakespeare-Finch J, Lima MP, Klinkosz W, Sekowski A, Alcalay L, Simonetti F, Avdeyeva TV, Pramila VS, Terracciano A. The Inaccuracy of National Character Stereotypes. JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN PERSONALITY 2013; 47:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.08.006. [PMID: 24187394 PMCID: PMC3811946 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Consensual stereotypes of some groups are relatively accurate, whereas others are not. Previous work suggesting that national character stereotypes are inaccurate has been criticized on several grounds. In this article we (a) provide arguments for the validity of assessed national mean trait levels as criteria for evaluating stereotype accuracy; and (b) report new data on national character in 26 cultures from descriptions (N=3,323) of the typical male or female adolescent, adult, or old person in each. The average ratings were internally consistent and converged with independent stereotypes of the typical culture member, but were weakly related to objective assessments of personality. We argue that this conclusion is consistent with the broader literature on the inaccuracy of national character stereotypes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Wayne Chan
- National Institute on Aging, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
| | - Lee Jussim
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey, USA
| | - Filip De Fruyt
- Department of Developmental, Personality, and Social Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | | | - Marleen De Bolle
- Department of Developmental, Personality, and Social Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Paul T. Costa
- National Institute on Aging, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
| | - Martina Hřebíčková
- Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Brno, Czech Republic
| | - Sylvie Graf
- Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Brno, Czech Republic
| | - Anu Realo
- Department of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
| | - Jüri Allik
- Department of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
- Estonian Academy of Sciences, Tallinn, Estonia
| | | | - Yoshiko Shimonaka
- Department of Human Studies, Bunkyo Gakuin University, Bunkyo, Japan
| | - Michelle Yik
- Division of Social Science, Hong Kong University of Science and Technology, Kowloon, Hong Kong
| | - Emília Ficková
- Institute of Experimental Psychology, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovak Republic
| | | | - Norma Reátigui
- Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Peruana Cayetano Heredia, Lima, Peru
| | | | - Vanina Schmidt
- Department of Psychology, University of Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Chang-kyu Ahn
- Department of Education, Pusan National University, Busan, South Korea
| | - Hyun-nie Ahn
- Department of Psychology, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, South Korea
| | | | - Jerzy Siuta
- Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
| | | | - Thomas R. Cain
- School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, Amherst, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Jarret T. Crawford
- Department of Psychology, The College of New Jersey, Ewing, New Jersey, USA
| | - Khairul Anwar Mastor
- Personality Research Group, University Kebangsaan Malaysia, Bangi, Selangor, Malaysia
| | | | | | - Daniel R. Miramontez
- Office of Institutional Research and Planning, San Diego Community College District, San Diego, California, USA
| | - Veronica Benet-Martínez
- ICREA and Department of Political and Social Sciences, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Jérôme Rossier
- Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Denis Bratko
- Department of Psychology, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia
| | - Iris Marušić
- Institute for Social Research in Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia
| | | | - Mami Yamaguchi
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Goran Knežević
- Department of Psychology, Belgrade University, Belgrade, Serbia
| | - Danka Purić
- Department of Psychology, Belgrade University, Belgrade, Serbia
| | - Thomas A. Martin
- Department of Psychology, Susquehanna University, Selinsgrove, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Mirona Gheorghiu
- School of Psychology, Queens University, Belfast, United Kingdom
| | - Peter B. Smith
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, United Kingdom
| | | | - Lei Wang
- Department of Psychology, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Jane Shakespeare-Finch
- School of Psychology and Counseling, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Margarida P. Lima
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Waldemar Klinkosz
- Department of Psychology, John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
| | - Andrzej Sekowski
- Department of Psychology, John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
| | - Lidia Alcalay
- Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
| | - Franco Simonetti
- Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
| | - Tatyana V. Avdeyeva
- Graduate School of Professional Psychology, University of St. Thomas, Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA
| | | | - Antonio Terracciano
- Department of Geriatrics, Florida State University College of Medicine, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Personality traits, national character stereotypes, and climate-economic conditions. Behav Brain Sci 2013; 36:501-2; discussion 503-21. [PMID: 23985221 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x1300023x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Cross-cultural personality research suggests that individuals from wealthier countries tend to be more open-minded. This openness to values may support more democratic governments and the expansion of fundamental freedoms. The link between wealth and freedom is evident in cold-to-temperate climates, but not across wealthy nations in hot climates. Furthermore, temperature and economic conditions shape perceptions of national character stereotypes.
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
Although Van de Vliert presented an entertaining story containing several original observations, an implicit assumption that climate affects human society identically through the history is not realistic. If almost everything is explained by cold winters or hot summers, then nothing is explained. Ignoring rival explanations does not make the proposed theory more convincing.
Collapse
|
23
|
Abstract
AbstractForty-nine commentators have reviewed the theory that needs-based stresses and freedoms are shaped differently in threatening, comforting, and challenging climato-economic habitats. Their commentaries cover the white domain, where the theory does apply (e.g., happiness, collectivism, and democracy), the gray domain, where it may or may not apply (e.g., personality traits and creativity), and the black domain, where it does not apply (e.g., human intelligence and gendered culture). This response article provides clarifications, recommendations, and expectations.
Collapse
|
24
|
Abstract
Socioecological psychology investigates humans' cognitive, emotional, and behavioral adaption to physical, interpersonal, economic, and political environments. This article summarizes three types of socioecological psychology research: (a) association studies that link an aspect of social ecology (e.g., population density) with psychology (e.g., prosocial behavior), (b) process studies that clarify why there is an association between social ecology and psychology (e.g., residential mobility → anxiety → familiarity seeking), and (c) niche construction studies that illuminate how psychological states give rise to the creation and maintenance of a social ecology (e.g., familiarity seeking → dominance of national chain stores). Socioecological psychology attempts to bring the objectivist perspective to psychological science, investigating how objective social and physical environments, not just perception and construal of the environments, affect one's thinking, feeling, and behaviors, as well as how people's thinking, feeling, and behaviors give rise to social and built environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shigehiro Oishi
- Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia 22904;
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Abstract
I present a very broad overview of what I have learned about personality trait assessment at different levels and offer some views on future directions for research and clinical practice. I review some basic principles of scale development and argue that internal consistency has been overemphasized; more attention to retest reliability is needed. Because protocol validity is crucial for individual assessment and because validity scales have limited utility, I urge combining assessments from multiple informants, and I present some statistical tools for that purpose. As culture-level traits, I discuss ethos, national character stereotypes, and aggregated personality traits, and summarize evidence for the validity of the latter. Our understanding of trait profiles of cultures is limited, but it can guide future exploration.
Collapse
|
26
|
Chan W, Mccrae RR, De Fruyt F, Jussim L, Löckenhoff CE, De Bolle M, Costa PT, Sutin AR, Realo A, Allik J, Nakazato K, Shimonaka Y, Hřebíčková M, Graf S, Yik M, Brunner-Sciarra M, De Figueroa NL, Schmidt V, Ahn CK, Ahn HN, Aguilar-Vafaie ME, Siuta J, Szmigielska B, Cain TR, Crawford JT, Mastor KA, Rolland JP, Nansubuga F, Miramontez DR, Benet-Martínez V, Rossier J, Bratko D, Marušić I, Halberstadt J, Yamaguchi M, Knežević G, Martin TA, Gheorghiu M, Smith PB, Barbaranelli C, Wang L, Shakespeare-Finch J, Lima MP, Klinkosz W, Sekowski A, Alcalay L, Simonetti F, Avdeyeva TV, Pramila VS, Terracciano A. Stereotypes of age differences in personality traits: universal and accurate? J Pers Soc Psychol 2012; 103:1050-1066. [PMID: 23088227 PMCID: PMC3514646 DOI: 10.1037/a0029712] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Age trajectories for personality traits are known to be similar across cultures. To address whether stereotypes of age groups reflect these age-related changes in personality, we asked participants in 26 countries (N = 3,323) to rate typical adolescents, adults, and old persons in their own country. Raters across nations tended to share similar beliefs about different age groups; adolescents were seen as impulsive, rebellious, undisciplined, preferring excitement and novelty, whereas old people were consistently considered lower on impulsivity, activity, antagonism, and Openness. These consensual age group stereotypes correlated strongly with published age differences on the five major dimensions of personality and most of 30 specific traits, using as criteria of accuracy both self-reports and observer ratings, different survey methodologies, and data from up to 50 nations. However, personal stereotypes were considerably less accurate, and consensual stereotypes tended to exaggerate differences across age groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Filip De Fruyt
- Department of Developmental, Personality, and Social Psychology, Ghent University
| | - Lee Jussim
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers University
| | | | - Marleen De Bolle
- Department of Developmental, Personality, and Social Psychology, Ghent University
| | | | | | - Anu Realo
- Department of Psychology, University of Tartu
| | - Jüri Allik
- Department of Psychology, University of Tartu
| | | | | | | | - Sylvie Graf
- Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic
| | - Michelle Yik
- Division of Social Science, Hong Kong University of Science and Technology
| | | | | | | | | | - Hyun-Nie Ahn
- Department of Psychology, Ewha Womans University
| | | | - Jerzy Siuta
- Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Daniel R Miramontez
- Office of Institutional Research and Planning, San Diego Community College District
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Lei Wang
- Department of Psychology, Peking University
| | | | - Margarida P Lima
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Coimbra
| | - Waldemar Klinkosz
- Department of Psychology, John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin
| | - Andrzej Sekowski
- Department of Psychology, John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin
| | - Lidia Alcalay
- Escuela de Psicologia, Pontificia Universidad Catolica de Chile
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Lönnqvist JE, Jasinskaja-Lahti I, Verkasalo M. Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022112466592] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
In a 4-year longitudinal study, we investigated stereotype change in the context of increased intergroup contact. Specifically, using one pre- and two postmigration measurement points, we followed some 200 Ingrian Finns and their families migrating from Russia to Finland. Stereotypes of a typical Finn were conceptualized within the framework provided by Schwartz’s values theory. At the group level, migrants’ stereotype profiles were consensual, similar to Finns’s autostereotypes, somewhat accurate, and highly stable. However, mean-level changes indicated a process of disillusionment: Finns were increasingly perceived as less benevolent and more hedonistic. We argue that personal contact changes aspects of stereotypes related to communal characteristics, whereas contact with cultural institutions influences perceptions of conservativeness. Probably due to political climate, Finns were increasingly perceived as adhering to tradition and security values over stimulation. Although individual-level stereotypes were only moderately stable and stereotype change was heterogeneous, we could not predict individual-level changes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Markku Verkasalo
- Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
|
29
|
Chan W, McCrae RR, Rogers DL, Weimer AA, Greenberg DM, Terracciano A. Rater Wealth Predicts Perceptions of Outgroup Competence. JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN PERSONALITY 2011; 45:597-603. [PMID: 22379232 PMCID: PMC3286797 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
National income has a pervasive influence on the perception of ingroup stereotypes, with high status and wealthy targets perceived as more competent. In two studies we investigated the degree to which economic wealth of raters related to perceptions of outgroup competence. Raters' economic wealth predicted trait ratings when 1) raters in 48 other cultures rated Americans' competence and 2) Mexican Americans rated Anglo Americans' competence. Rater wealth also predicted ratings of interpersonal warmth on the culture level. In conclusion, raters' economic wealth, either nationally or individually, is significantly associated with perception of outgroup members, supporting the notion that ingroup conditions or stereotypes function as frames of reference in evaluating outgroup traits.
Collapse
|
30
|
Abstract
Based on research with Bond’s Chinese Values Survey (CVS) across 23 countries, Hofstede added a fifth dimension, Long- versus Short-Term Orientation (LTO), to his earlier four IBM-based dimensions of national cultures. The authors attempted to replicate this dimension by analyzing World Values Survey (WVS) items that seemed to capture the concept of LTO. Their factor analysis of 10 such items across 38 countries resulted in two factors. One was strongly correlated with the original LTO, whereas the other resembled Hofstede’s individualism dimension. The first factor’s nomological network was identical to that of the CVS-based LTO: It predicted national economic growth and national school success in mathematics. These findings show that a dimension very similar to the original LTO can be derived from the WVS and that Chinese and Western research instruments can produce similar dimensions of culture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Minkov
- International University College, Sofia, Bulgaria
- Department of Germanic and Scandinavian Studies, Sofia University Saint Kliment Ohridski, Sofia, Bulgaria
| | - Geert Hofstede
- Emeritus, University of Maastricht, Maastricht, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
|
32
|
McCrae RR, Terracciano A, De Fruyt F, De Bolle M, Gelfand MJ, Costa PT. The validity and structure of culture-level personality scores: data from ratings of young adolescents. J Pers 2010; 78:815-38. [PMID: 20573127 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00634.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
We examined properties of culture-level personality traits in ratings of targets (N=5,109) ages 12 to 17 in 24 cultures. Aggregate scores were generalizable across gender, age, and relationship groups and showed convergence with culture-level scores from previous studies of self-reports and observer ratings of adults, but they were unrelated to national character stereotypes. Trait profiles also showed cross-study agreement within most cultures, 8 of which had not previously been studied. Multidimensional scaling showed that Western and non-Western cultures clustered along a dimension related to Extraversion. A culture-level factor analysis replicated earlier findings of a broad Extraversion factor but generally resembled the factor structure found in individuals. Continued analysis of aggregate personality scores is warranted.
Collapse
|
33
|
Löckenhoff CE, De Fruyt F, Terracciano A, McCrae RR, De Bolle M, Costa PT, Aguilar-Vafaie ME, Ahn CK, Ahn HN, Alcalay L, Allik J, Avdeyeva TV, Barbaranelli C, Benet-Martinez V, Blatný M, Bratko D, Cain TR, Crawford JT, Lima MP, Ficková E, Gheorghiu M, Halberstadt J, Hrebícková M, Jussim L, Klinkosz W, Knezević G, de Figueroa NL, Martin TA, Marusić I, Mastor KA, Miramontez DR, Nakazato K, Nansubuga F, Pramila VS, Realo A, Rolland JP, Rossier J, Schmidt V, Sekowski A, Shakespeare-Finch J, Shimonaka Y, Simonetti F, Siuta J, Smith PB, Szmigielska B, Wang L, Yamaguchi M, Yik M. Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and their culture-level associates. Psychol Aging 2010; 24:941-54. [PMID: 20025408 DOI: 10.1037/a0016901] [Citation(s) in RCA: 179] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
College students (N=3,435) in 26 cultures reported their perceptions of age-related changes in physical, cognitive, and socioemotional areas of functioning and rated societal views of aging within their culture. There was widespread cross-cultural consensus regarding the expected direction of aging trajectories with (a) perceived declines in societal views of aging, physical attractiveness, the ability to perform everyday tasks, and new learning; (b) perceived increases in wisdom, knowledge, and received respect; and (c) perceived stability in family authority and life satisfaction. Cross-cultural variations in aging perceptions were associated with culture-level indicators of population aging, education levels, values, and national character stereotypes. These associations were stronger for societal views on aging and perceptions of socioemotional changes than for perceptions of physical and cognitive changes. A consideration of culture-level variables also suggested that previously reported differences in aging perceptions between Asian and Western countries may be related to differences in population structure.
Collapse
|
34
|
Allik J, Mõttus R, Realo A. Does national character reflect mean personality traits when both are measured by the same instrument? JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN PERSONALITY 2010. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.10.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
35
|
Church AT. Current Controversies in the Study of Personality across Cultures. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2008. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00132.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|