1
|
Schroyens N, Bender CL, Alfei JM, Molina VA, Luyten L, Beckers T. Post-weaning housing conditions influence freezing during contextual fear conditioning in adult rats. Behav Brain Res 2018; 359:172-180. [PMID: 30391556 PMCID: PMC6314464 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.10.040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2018] [Revised: 10/30/2018] [Accepted: 10/30/2018] [Indexed: 01/14/2023]
Abstract
The present study aimed to investigate the influence of housing conditions on contextual fear memory malleability. Male Wistar rats were housed in enriched, standard, or impoverished conditions after weaning and remained in these conditions throughout the entire experiment. After six weeks into those housing conditions, all animals underwent a 3-day protocol including contextual fear conditioning (day 1), memory reactivation followed by systemic administration of midazolam or vehicle (day 2), and a retention test (day 3). Percentage freezing was used as a behavioral measure of contextual fear. There was no evidence for an effect of housing conditions on the sensitivity of contextual fear memory to amnestic effects of post-reactivation midazolam administration, and no indication for amnestic effects of post-reactivation midazolam overall (including in the standard group). The inability to replicate previous demonstrations of post-reactivation amnesia using the same protocol underscores the subtle nature of post-reactivation pharmacological memory interference. Notably, impoverished housing resulted in a decrease in contextual freezing during contextual fear conditioning, reactivation and retention testing, compared to enriched and standard housing conditions. This observation warrants caution when interpreting the results from experiments regarding effects of housing on fear memory processes, particularly when freezing is used as a measure of fear.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natalie Schroyens
- Centre for the Psychology of Learning and Experimental Psychopathology, Department of Psychology, KU Leuven, Tiensestraat 102 bus 3712, 3000 Leuven, Belgium.
| | - Crhistian Luis Bender
- Instituto de Farmacología Experimental de Córdoba-CONICET, Departamento de Farmacología, Facultad de Ciencias Químicas, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina.
| | - Joaquín Matias Alfei
- Centre for the Psychology of Learning and Experimental Psychopathology, Department of Psychology, KU Leuven, Tiensestraat 102 bus 3712, 3000 Leuven, Belgium.
| | - Victor Alejandro Molina
- Instituto de Farmacología Experimental de Córdoba-CONICET, Departamento de Farmacología, Facultad de Ciencias Químicas, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina.
| | - Laura Luyten
- Centre for the Psychology of Learning and Experimental Psychopathology, Department of Psychology, KU Leuven, Tiensestraat 102 bus 3712, 3000 Leuven, Belgium.
| | - Tom Beckers
- Centre for the Psychology of Learning and Experimental Psychopathology, Department of Psychology, KU Leuven, Tiensestraat 102 bus 3712, 3000 Leuven, Belgium; Leuven Brain Institute, 3000 Leuven, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bae SE, Richardson R. Behavioral tagging in infant rats. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018; 25:580-586. [PMID: 30322891 PMCID: PMC6191016 DOI: 10.1101/lm.047605.118] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2018] [Accepted: 08/28/2018] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Recent studies have shown that exposure to a novel environment may stabilize the persistence of weak memories, a phenomenon often attributed to a process referred to as “behavioral tagging.” While this phenomenon has been repeatedly demonstrated in adult animals, no studies to date have examined whether it occurs in infant animals, which is surprising given that infants exhibit an impaired ability to form long-term memories (LTMs). In the present study, infant (i.e., postnatal day (P) 17) rats were placed in a context and repeatedly shocked. Infant rats given brief open field exposure 1 h, but not 2 h, prior to conditioning exhibited enhanced retention when tested 1 d later (Experiments 1 and 2), but comparable retention when tested shortly after training (Experiment 2). Thus, exploration of an open field facilitates subsequent context fear memories by enhancing the persistence of the memory rather than strengthening the context-shock association at encoding. While exploration of an open field did not lead to better memory when animals were tested 3 d later (Experiment 3), a brief pretest shock led to a more pronounced reinstatement effect in rats exposed to the open field 1 h before conditioning (Experiment 4). Finally, unlike what has been reported in adults, Experiments 5 and 6 suggest that familiarization of the open field before subsequent exposure does not abolish the behavioral tagging effect in infants. Overall, while these findings suggest that similar behavioral tagging mechanisms to those reported in adults might be involved in the formation of LTMs in infant rats, they also suggest that there may be developmental differences in the retention of familiarization experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah E Bae
- School of Psychology, UNSW Sydney, New South Wales 2052, Australia
| | - Rick Richardson
- School of Psychology, UNSW Sydney, New South Wales 2052, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Chan D, Baker KD, Richardson R. Relearning a context-shock association after forgetting is an NMDAr-independent process. Physiol Behav 2014; 148:29-35. [PMID: 25446198 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2014] [Revised: 10/31/2014] [Accepted: 11/03/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Infantile amnesia (i.e., the rapid rate of forgetting in young animals) is at least partially due to a memory retrieval, rather than a storage, failure as studies have shown that these engrams can continue to influence later behavior. For example, prior conditioning affects the neural mechanisms underlying future learning. In adult animals, the initial learning of a context-shock association depends upon N-Methyl-D-Aspartate (NMDA) receptors, but this conditioning renders subsequent learning to a similar context NMDAr-independent. In the present study, we examined whether this transition from NMDAr-dependent to NMDAr-independent context conditioning occurs even after infantile amnesia. Experiment 1 demonstrated that infant (i.e., postnatal day 17) rats acquire a context-shock association when trained with multiple shocks, as assessed by context freezing one day later. However, they exhibit significant forgetting of this association 10days later. Experiments 2 and 3 showed that even when animals had forgotten the initial learning experience, future conditioning to the same context was NMDAr-independent. There was evidence of a transition to NMDAr-independent context fear learning in animals exposed only to the foot shock in infancy (Experiment 3) or only to the context in infancy (Experiment 3 but not Experiment 2). These latter results suggest that animals do not have to be exposed to the entire conditioning procedure at postnatal day 17 to show a transition to NMDAr-independent context learning. These experiments add to a growing body of evidence that forgotten infant memories can continue to affect later behavior by demonstrating that prior experience alters the mechanisms of future learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Diana Chan
- School of Psychology, The University of New South Wales, Sydney 2052, Australia.
| | - Kathryn D Baker
- School of Psychology, The University of New South Wales, Sydney 2052, Australia.
| | - Rick Richardson
- School of Psychology, The University of New South Wales, Sydney 2052, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Burman MA, Erickson KJ, Deal AL, Jacobson RE. Contextual and auditory fear conditioning continue to emerge during the periweaning period in rats. PLoS One 2014; 9:e100807. [PMID: 24977415 PMCID: PMC4076234 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100807] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2014] [Accepted: 05/29/2014] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Anxiety disorders often emerge during childhood. Rodent models using classical fear conditioning have shown that different types of fear depend upon different neural structures and may emerge at different stages of development. For example, some work has suggested that contextual fear conditioning generally emerges later in development (postnatal day 23–24) than explicitly cued fear conditioning (postnatal day 15–17) in rats. This has been attributed to an inability of younger subjects to form a representation of the context due to an immature hippocampus. However, evidence that contextual fear can be observed in postnatal day 17 subjects and that cued fear conditioning continues to emerge past this age raises questions about the nature of this deficit. The current studies examine this question using both the context pre-exposure facilitation effect for immediate single-shock contextual fear conditioning and traditional cued fear conditioning using Sprague-Dawley rats. The data suggest that both cued and contextual fear conditioning are continuing to develop between PD 17 and 24, consistent with development occurring the in essential fear conditioning circuit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A. Burman
- Department of Psychology, Center for Excellence in the Neurosciences, University of New England, Biddeford, Maine, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Kristen J. Erickson
- Department of Psychology, Center for Excellence in the Neurosciences, University of New England, Biddeford, Maine, United States of America
| | - Alex L. Deal
- Department of Psychology, Center for Excellence in the Neurosciences, University of New England, Biddeford, Maine, United States of America
| | - Rose E. Jacobson
- Department of Biology, University of New England, Biddeford, Maine, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Akers KG, Arruda-Carvalho M, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW. Ontogeny of contextual fear memory formation, specificity, and persistence in mice. Learn Mem 2012; 19:598-604. [PMID: 23161449 DOI: 10.1101/lm.027581.112] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Pinpointing the precise age when young animals begin to form memories of aversive events is valuable for understanding the onset of anxiety and mood disorders and for detecting early cognitive impairment in models of childhood-onset disorders. Although these disorders are most commonly modeled in mice, we know little regarding the development of learning and memory in this species because most previous studies have been restricted to rats. Therefore, in the present study, we constructed an ontogenetic timeline of contextual fear memory ranging from infancy to adulthood in mice. We found that the ability of mice to form long-term context-shock associations emerged ∼13-14 d of age, which is several days earlier than previously reported for rats. Although the ability to form contextual fear memories remained stable from infancy into adulthood, infant mice had shorter-lasting memories than adolescent and adult mice. Furthermore, we found that mice subjected to fetal alcohol exposure showed a delay in the developmental emergence of contextual fear memory, illustrating the utility of this ontogenetic approach in detecting developmental delays in cognitive function stemming from maladaptive early life experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine G Akers
- Neurosciences and Mental Health, Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada, M5G 1X8
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Graham BM, Richardson R. Acute systemic fibroblast growth factor-2 enhances long-term memory in developing rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2009; 91:424-30. [DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2008] [Revised: 12/08/2008] [Accepted: 12/09/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
7
|
Yap CSL, Richardson R. Extinction in the developing rat: an examination of renewal effects. Dev Psychobiol 2007; 49:565-75. [PMID: 17680605 DOI: 10.1002/dev.20244] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
In the present series of experiments the context-specificity of extinction was examined from a developmental perspective. For postnatal day (PN) 23 rats, renewal of freezing to an aversive odor conditioned stimulus (CS) was observed when rats were conditioned in Context A, extinguished in Context B, and tested in Context A (i.e., ABA renewal). This effect was not observed in PN16 rats, which is consistent with previous studies suggesting that rats < approximately PN20 are impaired in encoding contextual information [i.e., Carew and Rudy [1991]. Developmental Psychobiology, 24, 191-209]. Subsequent experiments demonstrated that for rats conditioned at PN16 and tested at PN23, contextual regulation of extinction performance depended on the age at which extinction occurred. Specifically, ABA renewal was observed in rats given extinction training at PN22 but not in rats given extinction training at PN17. These latter results show that whether or not context regulates the expression of an ambiguous memory is determined by the animal's age when the memory becomes ambiguous.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carol S L Yap
- School of Psychology, The University of New South Wales, Sydney 2052, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Anticipatory activity and stereotypical behaviour in American mink (Mustela vison) in three housing systems differing in the amount of enrichments. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2004. [DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
9
|
Schulz B, Fendt M, Richardson R, Schnitzler HU. Temporary inactivation of the perirhinal cortex by muscimol injections block acquisition and expression of fear-potentiated startle. Eur J Neurosci 2004; 19:713-20. [PMID: 14984422 DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03180.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined the role of the perirhinal cortex (PRh) in aversive information processing and emotional learning. Specifically, we studied the effects of temporary inactivation of the PRh on acquisition and expression of conditioned fear as measured by fear-potentiated startle in rats, as well as on shock sensitization of startle. Temporary inactivation of the PRh was induced by local injections of the GABAA agonist muscimol (0.0, 1.1, 2.2, 4.4 nmol/0.5 micro L). Muscimol injections into the PRh blocked both the expression and acquisition of fear-potentiated startle, as well as shock sensitization of startle. Shock sensitivity was not affected by muscimol injections, indicating that the observed blockade of acquisition and shock sensitization was not caused by a disruption in the perception of shock. Taken together, the present data show that the PRh is critical for the processing of aversive information and is necessary for the expression of emotional learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brigitte Schulz
- Animal Physiology, University of Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, D-72076 Tübingen, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|