1
|
Jayasinghe SAL, Sainburg RL, Sarlegna FR. Role of proprioception in corrective visually-guided movements: larger movement errors in both arms of a deafferented individual compared to control participants. Exp Brain Res 2024; 242:2329-2340. [PMID: 39110161 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-024-06901-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2024] [Accepted: 07/23/2024] [Indexed: 09/26/2024]
Abstract
Proprioception plays an important role in both feedforward and feedback processes underlying movement control. This has been shown with individuals who suffered a profound proprioceptive loss and use vision to partially compensate for the sensory loss. The purpose of this study was to specifically examine the role of proprioception in feedback motor responses to visual perturbations by examining voluntary arm movements in an individual with a rare case of selective peripheral deafferentation (GL). We compared her left and right hand movements with those of age-matched female control participants (70.0 years ± 0.2 SEM) during a reaching task. Participants were asked to move their unseen hand, represented by a cursor on the screen, quickly and accurately to reach a visual target. A visual perturbation could be pseudorandomly applied, at movement onset, to either the target position (target jump) or the cursor position (cursor jump). Results showed that despite the continuous visual feedback that was provided, GL produced larger errors in final position accuracy compared to control participants, with her left nondominant hand being more erroneous after a cursor jump. We also found that the proprioceptively-deafferented individual produced less spatially efficient movements than the control group. Overall, these results provide evidence of a heavier reliance on proprioceptive feedback for movements of the nondominant hand relative to the dominant hand, supporting the view of a lateralization of the feedback processes underlying motor control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shanie A L Jayasinghe
- Division of Physical Therapy and Rehabilitation Science, Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA.
| | - Robert L Sainburg
- Department of Neurology, Pennsylvania State University College of Medicine, Hershey, PA, USA
- Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University, State College, University Park, PA, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Tanis D, Kurtzer I. Superior performance by two new methods in identifying the online reaction time of reaching movements. J Neurophysiol 2024; 132:906-921. [PMID: 39110518 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00379.2023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2023] [Revised: 07/08/2024] [Accepted: 08/05/2024] [Indexed: 09/11/2024] Open
Abstract
Reaching movements can be redirected during their progress to handle unexpected visual changes, such as a change in target location. It is important to know when these redirections start, i.e., the online reaction time (oRT), but this information is not readily evident since redirections are embedded within a time-varying baseline movement that differs from trial to trial. The one previous study that evaluated the performance of different oRT identification methods utilized simulated redirections with the exact same onset, rather than a range of onsets as would be typically encountered. We addressed this gap by utilizing batches of "hybrid" trials with temporal spread in their oRTs. Each hybrid trial combined a sampled baseline movement with an idealized corrective response. Two new methods had the most accurate identification of online reaction times: 1) a threshold-aligned grand mean regression, and 2) a template-based approach we term the canonical correction search. The threshold-aligned grand mean regression is simple to implement and effective. The canonical correction search is a more complex procedure but arguably better linked to the underlying response. Applying the two methods to a published dataset revealed more delayed oRTs than was previously reported along with new information such as the width of oRT distributions. Taken together, our results demonstrate the utility of two new methods for dissecting corrective action from ongoing movement.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Advancing our understanding of visual feedback control requires methods that accurately identify the onset of corrective action. We developed a modified regression approach and a template-based approach to identify the online reaction time of single-reaching movements. Both outperform previous methods when challenged by temporal jitter in the response onset and increased background noise.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Tanis
- Department of Biomedical ScienceNew York Institute of Technology-College of Osteopathic MedicineOld WestburyNew YorkUnited States
| | - Isaac Kurtzer
- Department of Biomedical ScienceNew York Institute of Technology-College of Osteopathic MedicineOld WestburyNew YorkUnited States
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Takagi A, Burdet E, Koike Y. The control of the arm's equilibrium position. J Neurophysiol 2024; 131:750-756. [PMID: 38507295 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00011.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2024] [Revised: 03/15/2024] [Accepted: 03/18/2024] [Indexed: 03/22/2024] Open
Abstract
To generate a force, the brain activates muscles that act like springs to pull the arm toward a new equilibrium position. The equilibrium position (EP) is central to our understanding of the biological control of viscoelastic muscles. Although there is evidence of the EP during the control of limb posture, EPs have not been directly identified when the limb exerts a force against the environment. Here, we asked participants to apply a constant force in one of eight directions against a point-like constraint. This constraint was released abruptly to observe the final position to which the arm converged. Importantly, the same force magnitude was maintained while changing the arm's stiffness by modulating the strength of the hand's power grasp. The final position moved further away from the constraint as the arm became less stiff and was inversely proportional to the arm's stiffness, thereby confirming that the final position was the arm's EP. These results demonstrate how the EP changes with the arm's stiffness to produce a desired force in different directions.NEW & NOTEWORTHY According to numerous theories, the brain controls posture and movement by activating muscles that attract the limb toward a so-called equilibrium position, but the universality of this mechanism has not been shown for different motor behaviors. Here, we show that even when pushing or pulling against the environment, the brain achieves the desired force through an equilibrium position that lies beyond the physical constraint.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Atsushi Takagi
- NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Atsugi, Japan
- Institute of Innovative Research, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Yokohama, Japan
| | - Etienne Burdet
- Imperial College of Science, Technology and Medicine, London, UK
| | - Yasuharu Koike
- Institute of Innovative Research, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Yokohama, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Bloechle JL, Audiffren J, Le Naour T, Alli A, Simoni D, Wüthrich G, Bresciani JP. It's not all in your feet: Improving penalty kick performance with human-avatar interaction and machine learning. Innovation (N Y) 2024; 5:100584. [PMID: 38445019 PMCID: PMC10912701 DOI: 10.1016/j.xinn.2024.100584] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2023] [Accepted: 01/24/2024] [Indexed: 03/07/2024] Open
Abstract
Penalty kicks are increasingly decisive in major international football competitions. Yet, over 30% of shootout kicks are missed. The outcome of the kick often relies on the ability of the penalty taker to exploit anticipatory movements of the goalkeeper to redirect the kick toward the open side of the goal. Unfortunately, this ability is difficult to train using classical methods. We used an augmented reality simulator displaying an holographic goalkeeper to test and train penalty kick performance with 13 young elite players. Machine learning algorithms were used to optimize the learning rate by maintaining an optimal level of training difficulty. Ten training sessions of 20 kicks reduced the redirection threshold by 120 ms, which constituted a 28% reduction with respect to the baseline threshold. Importantly, redirection threshold reduction was observed for all trained players, and all things being equal, it corresponded to an estimated 35% improvement of the success rate.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Luc Bloechle
- Control and Perception Laboratory, University of Fribourg, Bd Perolles 90, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland
| | - Julien Audiffren
- Control and Perception Laboratory, University of Fribourg, Bd Perolles 90, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland
| | - Thibaut Le Naour
- Motion-up, Le Prisme, Place Albert Einstein, 56000 Vannes, France
| | - Andrea Alli
- Control and Perception Laboratory, University of Fribourg, Bd Perolles 90, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland
| | - Dylan Simoni
- Control and Perception Laboratory, University of Fribourg, Bd Perolles 90, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland
| | | | - Jean-Pierre Bresciani
- Control and Perception Laboratory, University of Fribourg, Bd Perolles 90, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
McManus M, Schütz I, Voudouris D, Fiehler K. How visuomotor predictability and task demands affect tactile sensitivity on a moving limb during object interaction in a virtual environment. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2023; 10:231259. [PMID: 38094265 PMCID: PMC10716662 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.231259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2023] [Accepted: 11/16/2023] [Indexed: 01/11/2024]
Abstract
Tactile sensitivity is decreased on a moving limb compared to the same static limb. This tactile suppression likely reflects an interplay between sensorimotor predictions and sensory feedback. Here, we examined how visuomotor predictability influences tactile suppression. Participants were instructed to hit an approaching virtual object, with the object either never rotating, or always rotating, or rotating unpredictably, prompting related movement adjustments. We probed tactile suppression by delivering a vibrotactile stimulus of varying intensities to the moving hand briefly after the object's rotation and asked participants to indicate if they had felt a vibration. We hypothesized that Unpredictable Rotations would require upweighting of somatosensory feedback from the hand and therefore decrease suppression. Instead, we found stronger suppression with unpredictable than Predictable Rotations. This finding persisted even when visual input from the moving hand was removed and participants had to rely solely on somatosensory feedback of their hand. Importantly, we found a correlation between task demand and tactile suppression in both experiments, indicating that task load can amplify tactile suppression, possibly by downweighting task-irrelevant somatosensory feedback signals to allow for successful task performance when visuomotor task demands are high.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meaghan McManus
- Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Hessen, Germany
| | - Immo Schütz
- Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Hessen, Germany
| | - Dimitris Voudouris
- Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Hessen, Germany
| | - Katja Fiehler
- Experimental Psychology, Justus Liebig University, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394, Giessen, Hessen, Germany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University, Giessen, Hessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Fooken J, Baltaretu BR, Barany DA, Diaz G, Semrau JA, Singh T, Crawford JD. Perceptual-Cognitive Integration for Goal-Directed Action in Naturalistic Environments. J Neurosci 2023; 43:7511-7522. [PMID: 37940592 PMCID: PMC10634571 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1373-23.2023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/21/2023] [Revised: 08/15/2023] [Accepted: 08/18/2023] [Indexed: 11/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Real-world actions require one to simultaneously perceive, think, and act on the surrounding world, requiring the integration of (bottom-up) sensory information and (top-down) cognitive and motor signals. Studying these processes involves the intellectual challenge of cutting across traditional neuroscience silos, and the technical challenge of recording data in uncontrolled natural environments. However, recent advances in techniques, such as neuroimaging, virtual reality, and motion tracking, allow one to address these issues in naturalistic environments for both healthy participants and clinical populations. In this review, we survey six topics in which naturalistic approaches have advanced both our fundamental understanding of brain function and how neurologic deficits influence goal-directed, coordinated action in naturalistic environments. The first part conveys fundamental neuroscience mechanisms related to visuospatial coding for action, adaptive eye-hand coordination, and visuomotor integration for manual interception. The second part discusses applications of such knowledge to neurologic deficits, specifically, steering in the presence of cortical blindness, impact of stroke on visual-proprioceptive integration, and impact of visual search and working memory deficits. This translational approach-extending knowledge from lab to rehab-provides new insights into the complex interplay between perceptual, motor, and cognitive control in naturalistic tasks that are relevant for both basic and clinical research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jolande Fooken
- Centre for Neuroscience, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L3N6, Canada
| | - Bianca R Baltaretu
- Department of Psychology, Justus Liebig University, Giessen, 35394, Germany
| | - Deborah A Barany
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Georgia, and Augusta University/University of Georgia Medical Partnership, Athens, Georgia 30602
| | - Gabriel Diaz
- Center for Imaging Science, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, New York 14623
| | - Jennifer A Semrau
- Department of Kinesiology and Applied Physiology, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware 19713
| | - Tarkeshwar Singh
- Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802
| | - J Douglas Crawford
- Centre for Integrative and Applied Neuroscience, York University, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Bapna T, Valles J, Leng S, Pacilli M, Nataraja RM. Eye-tracking in surgery: a systematic review. ANZ J Surg 2023; 93:2600-2608. [PMID: 37668263 DOI: 10.1111/ans.18686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2023] [Revised: 08/20/2023] [Accepted: 08/22/2023] [Indexed: 09/06/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Surgery is constantly evolving with the assistance of rapidly developing novel technology. Eye-tracking devices provide opportunities to monitor the acquisition of surgical skills, gain insight into performance, and enhance surgical practice. The aim of this review was to consolidate the available evidence for the use of eye-tracking in the surgical disciplines. METHODS A systematic literature review was conducted in accordance with PRISMA guidelines. A search of OVID Medline, EMBASE, Cochrane library, Scopus, and Science Direct was conducted January 2000 until December 2022. Studies involving eye-tracking in surgical training, assessment and technical innovation were included in the review. Non-surgical procedures, animal studies, and studies not involving surgical participants were excluded from the review. RESULTS The search returned a total of 12 054 articles, 80 of which were included in the final analysis and review. Seventeen studies involved eye-tracking in surgical training, 48 surgical assessment, and 20 were focussing on technical aspects of this technology. Twenty-six different eye-tracking devices were used in the included studies. Metrics such as the number of fixations, duration of fixations, dwell time, and cognitive workload were able to differentiate between novice and expert performance. Eight studies demonstrated the effectiveness of gaze-training for improving surgical skill. CONCLUSION The current literature shows a broad range of utility for a variety of eye-tracking devices in surgery. There remains a lack of standardization for metric parameters and gaze analysis techniques. Further research is required to validate its use to establish reliability and create uniform practices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tanay Bapna
- Department of Paediatric Surgery & Surgical Simulation, Monash Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - John Valles
- Department of Paediatric Surgery & Surgical Simulation, Monash Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Samantha Leng
- Department of Paediatric Surgery & Surgical Simulation, Monash Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Maurizio Pacilli
- Department of Paediatric Surgery & Surgical Simulation, Monash Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
- Departments of Paediatrics & Surgery, School of Clinical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Ramesh Mark Nataraja
- Department of Paediatric Surgery & Surgical Simulation, Monash Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
- Departments of Paediatrics & Surgery, School of Clinical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Le Naour T, Papinutto M, Lobier M, Bresciani JP. Controlling the trajectory of a moving object substantially shortens the latency of motor responses to visual stimuli. iScience 2023; 26:106838. [PMID: 37250785 PMCID: PMC10212987 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.106838] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2022] [Revised: 03/01/2023] [Accepted: 05/04/2023] [Indexed: 05/31/2023] Open
Abstract
Motor responses to visual stimuli have shorter latencies for controlling than for initiating movement. The shorter latencies observed for movement control are notably believed to reflect the involvement of forward models when controlling moving limbs. We assessed whether controlling a moving limb is a "requisite" to observe shortened response latencies. The latency of button-press responses to a visual stimulus was compared between conditions involving or not involving the control of a moving object, but never involving any actual control of a body segment. When the motor response controlled a moving object, response latencies were significantly shorter and less variable, probably reflecting a faster sensorimotor processing (as assessed fitting a LATER model to our data). These results suggest that when the task at hand entails a control component, the sensorimotor processing of visual information is hastened, and this even if the task does not require to actually control a moving limb.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thibaut Le Naour
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
- Motion-up, Vannes, France
| | - Michael Papinutto
- Department of Psychology, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
| | | | - Jean-Pierre Bresciani
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
- Grenoble-Alpes University, Grenoble, France
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Crowe EM, Smeets JBJ, Brenner E. Online updating of obstacle positions when intercepting a virtual target. Exp Brain Res 2023:10.1007/s00221-023-06634-5. [PMID: 37244877 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-023-06634-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2022] [Accepted: 05/11/2023] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
People rely upon sensory information in the environment to guide their actions. Ongoing goal-directed arm movements are constantly adjusted to the latest estimate of both the target and hand's positions. Does the continuous guidance of ongoing arm movements also consider the latest visual information of the position of obstacles in the surrounding? To find out, we asked participants to slide their finger across a screen to intercept a laterally moving virtual target while moving through a gap that was created by two virtual circular obstacles. At a fixed time during each trial, the target suddenly jumped slightly laterally while still continuing to move. In half the trials, the size of the gap changed at the same moment as the target jumped. As expected, participants adjusted their movements in response to the target jump. Importantly, the magnitude of this response depended on the new size of the gap. If participants were told that the circles were irrelevant, changing the gap between them had no effect on the responses. This shows that obstacles' instantaneous positions can be considered when visually guiding goal-directed movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily M Crowe
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Institute of Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Amsterdam Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham, NG7 2RD, UK.
| | - Jeroen B J Smeets
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Institute of Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Amsterdam Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Eli Brenner
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Institute of Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Amsterdam Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Yokosaka T, Ujitoko Y, Kawabe T. Force illusion induced by visual illusion: Illusory curve in cursor path is interpreted as unintended force. J Vis 2023; 23:5. [PMID: 37052903 PMCID: PMC10108736 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.4.5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/14/2023] Open
Abstract
Discrepancies between expected and actual visual outcomes of motor action can produce an illusory sensation of unintended force. In the present study, we addressed whether the force illusion could be induced even when the discrepancy was brought about by the illusory appearance of the actual outcome. Specifically, the apparent path of a cursor controlled by the participants was modulated by the direction of noise motion presented inside the cursor. We showed that a greater noise motion inside the cursor caused a greater apparent curve of the cursor path and, also, higher rating scores for an unintended force. We also found that the unintended force was influenced strongly by the visibility of the cursor, suggesting that the apparent curve of the cursor path was a more important factor in generating the unintended force than the noise motion itself inside the cursor. Our results suggest that the illusory force, which is mediated by cross-modal mechanisms susceptible to visual illusion, can be exploited in extended reality systems as a novel technique for giving users a sensation of force.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Takumi Yokosaka
- NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation, Atsugi, Japan
- http://www.kecl.ntt.co.jp/people/yokosaka.takumi/index.html
| | - Yusuke Ujitoko
- NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation, Atsugi, Japan
| | - Takahiro Kawabe
- NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation, Atsugi, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Weber BM, Schätzle S, Stelzer M. Aiming performance during spaceflight: Individual adaptation to microgravity and the benefits of haptic support. APPLIED ERGONOMICS 2022; 103:103791. [PMID: 35588558 DOI: 10.1016/j.apergo.2022.103791] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2021] [Revised: 04/27/2022] [Accepted: 04/28/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
Sensorimotor performance is known to deteriorate during spaceflight. Prior research for instance documented that targeted arm motions are performed slower and less precise in microgravity conditions. This article describes an experiment on aiming performance during different stages of a space mission. Moreover, the influence of different haptic settings of the human-machine interface (HMI) was explored. Two separate studies are presented in which the same aiming tasks were performed with a force feedback joystick: 1) A terrestrial study (N = 20) to explore time and haptic setting effects and 2) a space experiment (N = 3) with a pre-mission session, three mission sessions on board the ISS (2, 4, and 6 weeks in space), and a post-mission session. Results showed that sensorimotor performance was mainly affected in the initial phase of exposure to microgravity and this effect was moderated by astronauts' sensorimotor skills. Providing low stiffness at the HMI, however, proved to be an effective measure to maintain aiming precision in microgravity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bernhard M Weber
- German Aerospace Center, Institute of Robotics and Mechatronics, 82234, Wessling, Germany.
| | - Simon Schätzle
- Sensor-Technik Wiedemann GmbH, 87600, Kaufbeuren, Germany
| | - Martin Stelzer
- German Aerospace Center, Institute of Robotics and Mechatronics, 82234, Wessling, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
de Brouwer AJ, Spering M. Eye-hand coordination during online reach corrections is task dependent. J Neurophysiol 2022; 127:885-895. [PMID: 35294273 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00270.2021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
To produce accurate movements, the human motor system needs to deal with errors that can occur due to inherent noise, changes in the body, or disturbances in the environment. Here, we investigated the temporal coupling of rapid corrections of the eye and hand in response to a change in visual target location during the movement. In addition to a "classic" double-step task in which the target stepped to a new position, participants performed a set of modified double-step tasks in which the change in movement goal was indicated by the appearance of an additional target, or by a spatial or symbolic cue. We found that both the absolute correction latencies of the eye and hand and the relative eye-hand correction latencies were dependent on the visual characteristics of the target change, with increasingly longer latencies in tasks that required more visual and cognitive processing. Typically, the hand started correcting slightly earlier than the eye, especially when the target change was indicated by a symbolic cue, and in conditions where visual feedback of the hand position was provided during the reach. Our results indicate that the oculomotor and limb-motor system can be differentially influenced by processing requirements of the task and emphasize that temporal eye-hand coupling is flexible rather than rigid.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Eye movements support hand movements in many situations. Here, we used variations of a double-step task to investigate temporal coupling of corrective hand and eye movements in response to target displacements. Correction latency coupling depended on the visual and cognitive processing demands of the task. The hand started correcting before the eye, especially when the task required decoding a symbolic cue. These findings highlight the flexibility and task dependency of eye-hand coordination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anouk J de Brouwer
- Department of Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Miriam Spering
- Department of Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.,Institute for Computing, Information and Cognitive Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.,Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Training visual attention improves basketball three-point shot performance under pressure. SPORT SCIENCES FOR HEALTH 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s11332-021-00866-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
14
|
Reichenbach A, Urgen BM, Apostolakis S, Michlin L, Diedrichsen J. Factors governing the assignment of visual consequence to the corresponding action. J Neurophysiol 2022; 127:756-766. [PMID: 35171748 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00450.2021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
To achieve fast feedback control of voluntary movements, the visual consequences of our motor commands need to be quickly identified and analyzed by the motor control processes in the brain. These processes work remarkably well even in complex visual environments and in the face of discrepancies between physical actuator and visually perceived effect, e.g. when moving a computer mouse on a visually crowded screen. Here we use an ambiguous situation in which a single cursor could be controlled by either the left or the right hand to determine the visual and cognitive factors that determine the assignment of a visual stimulus to the corresponding motor command. Our results demonstrate that the visuomotor system is exquisitely sensitive to the spatio-temporal correlation between cursor and hands, learning the appropriate mapping implicitly within several minutes. In contrast, spatial proximity between end effector and visual consequence has an immediate but only transient effect on the assignment process. Finally, an explicit instruction about which hand controls the cursor only has a minor influence when the instruction is presented first. These findings provide insight into the relative importance of the factors that determine the binding of visual information to the corresponding motor structures to enable fast feedback control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Reichenbach
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,Faculty of Computer Science, Heilbronn University, Heilbronn, Germany.,Center for Machine Learning, Heilbronn University, Heilbronn, Germany
| | - Buse Merve Urgen
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,National Magnetic Resonance Research Center (UMRAM), Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Sotirios Apostolakis
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Liora Michlin
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Jörn Diedrichsen
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,The Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
De Comite A, Crevecoeur F, Lefèvre P. Online modification of goal-directed control in human reaching movements. J Neurophysiol 2021; 125:1883-1898. [PMID: 33852821 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00536.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans are able to perform very sophisticated reaching movements in a myriad of contexts based on flexible control strategies influenced by the task goal and environmental constraints such as obstacles. However, it remains unknown whether these control strategies can be adjusted online. The objective of this study was to determine whether the factors that determine control strategies during planning also modify the execution of an ongoing movement following sudden changes in task demand. More precisely, we investigated whether, and at which latency, feedback responses to perturbation loads followed the change in the structure of the goal target or environment. We changed the target width (square or rectangle) to alter the task redundancy, or the presence of obstacles to induce different constraints on the reach path, and assessed based on surface electromyography (EMG) recordings when the change in visual display altered the feedback response to mechanical perturbations. Task-related EMG responses were detected within 150 ms of a change in target shape. Considering visuomotor delays of ∼ 100 ms, these results suggest that it takes 50 ms to change control policy within a trial. An additional 30-ms delay was observed when the change in context involved sudden appearance or disappearance of obstacles. Overall, our results demonstrate that the control policy within a reaching movement is not static: contextual factors that influence movement planning also influence movement execution at surprisingly short latencies. Moreover, the additional 30 ms associated with obstacles suggests that these two types of changes may be mediated via distinct processes.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The present work demonstrates that the control strategies used to perform reaching movements are adjusted online when the structure of the target or the presence of obstacles are altered during movements. Thus, the properties of goal-directed reaching control are not simply selected during the planning stage of a movement prior to execution. Rather, they are dynamically and rapidly adjusted online, within ∼150 ms, according to changes in environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antoine De Comite
- Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics, Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Ottignies-Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Frédéric Crevecoeur
- Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics, Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Ottignies-Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Philippe Lefèvre
- Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics, Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Ottignies-Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Cámara C, López-Moliner J, Brenner E, de la Malla C. Looking away from a moving target does not disrupt the way in which the movement toward the target is guided. J Vis 2021; 20:5. [PMID: 32407436 PMCID: PMC7409596 DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.5.5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
People usually follow a moving object with their gaze if they intend to interact with it. What would happen if they did not? We recorded eye and finger movements while participants moved a cursor toward a moving target. An unpredictable delay in updating the position of the cursor on the basis of that of the invisible finger made it essential to use visual information to guide the finger's ongoing movement. Decreasing the contrast between the cursor and the background from trial to trial made it difficult to see the cursor without looking at it. In separate experiments, either participants were free to hit the target anywhere along its trajectory or they had to move along a specified path. In the two experiments, participants tracked the cursor rather than the target with their gaze on 13% and 32% of the trials, respectively. They hit fewer targets when the contrast was low or a path was imposed. Not looking at the target did not disrupt the visual guidance that was required to deal with the delays that we imposed. Our results suggest that peripheral vision can be used to guide one item to another, irrespective of which item one is looking at.
Collapse
|
17
|
O'Rielly JL, Ma-Wyatt A. Saccade dynamics during an online updating task change with healthy aging. J Vis 2020; 20:2. [PMID: 33270828 PMCID: PMC7718816 DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.13.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2019] [Accepted: 08/01/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Goal-directed movements rely on the integration of both visual and motor information, especially during the online control of movement, to fluidly and flexibly control coordinated action. Eye-hand coordination typically plays an important role in goal-directed movements. As people age, various aspects of motor control and visual performance decline (Haegerstrom-Portnoy, Schneck, & Brabyn, 1999; Seidler et al., 2010), including an increase in saccade latencies (Munoz, Broughton, Goldring, & Armstrong, 1998). However, there is limited insight into how age-related changes in saccadic performance impact eye-hand coordination during online control. We investigated this question through the use of a target perturbation paradigm. Older and younger participants completed a perturbation task where target perturbations could occur either early (0 ms) or later (200 ms) after reach onset. We analyzed reach correction latencies and the frequency of the reach correction, coupled with analyses of saccades across all stages of movement. Older participants had slower correction latencies and initiated corrections less frequently compared to younger participants, with this trend being exacerbated in the later (200 ms) target perturbation condition. Older participants also produced slower saccade latencies toward both the initial target and the perturbed target. For trials in which a correction occurred to a late perturbation, touch responses were more accurate when there was more time between the saccade landing and the touch. Altogether, our results suggest that these age-related effects may be due to the delayed acquisition of visual and oculomotor information used to inform the reaching movement, stemming from the increase in saccade latencies before and after target perturbation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica L O'Rielly
- School of Psychology, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
| | - Anna Ma-Wyatt
- School of Psychology, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Chomienne L, Blouin J, Bringoux L. Online corrective responses following target jump in altered gravitoinertial force field point to nested feedforward and feedback control. J Neurophysiol 2020; 125:154-165. [PMID: 33174494 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00268.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Studies on goal-directed arm movements have shown a close link between feedforward and feedback control in protocols where both planning and online control processes faced a similar type of perturbation, either mechanical or visual. This particular context might have facilitated the use of an adapted internal model by feedforward and feedback control. Here, we considered this link in a context where, after feedforward control was adapted through proprioception-based processes, feedback control was tested under visual perturbation. We analyzed the response of the reaching hand to target displacements following adaptation to an altered force field induced by rotating participants at constant velocity. Reaching corrections were assessed through variables related to the accuracy (lateral and longitudinal end point errors) and kinematics (movement time, peak velocity) of the corrective movements. The electromyographic activity of different arm muscles (pectoralis, posterior deltoid, biceps brachii, and triceps brachii) was analyzed. Statistical analyses revealed that accuracy and kinematics of corrective movements were strikingly alike between normal and altered gravitoinertial force fields. However, pectoralis and biceps muscle activities recorded during corrective movements were significantly modified to counteract the effect of rotation-induced Coriolis and centrifugal forces on the arm. Remarkably, feedback control was functional from the very first time participants encountered a target jump in the altered force field. Overall, the present results demonstrate that feedforward control enables immediate functional feedback control even when applied to distinct sensorimotor processes.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We investigated the link between feedforward and feedback control when applying a double-step perturbation (visual target jump) during reaching movements performed in modified gravitoinertial environments. Altogether, kinematics and EMG analyses showed that movement corrections were highly effective in the different force fields, suggesting that, although feedforward and feedback control were driven by different sensory inputs, feedback control was remarkably functional from the very first time participants encountered a target jump in the altered force field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Chomienne
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| | - J Blouin
- Aix-Marseille Univ, CNRS, LNC, Marseille, France
| | - L Bringoux
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Priot AE, Revol P, Sillan O, Prablanc C, Gaveau V. Sensory Prediction of Limb Movement Is Critical for Automatic Online Control. Front Hum Neurosci 2020; 14:549537. [PMID: 33132873 PMCID: PMC7575915 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.549537] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2020] [Accepted: 08/27/2020] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Fast, online control of movement is an essential component of human motor skills, as it allows automatic correction of inaccurate planning. The present study explores the role of two types of concurrent signals in error correction: predicted visual reafferences coming from an internal representation of the hand, and actual visual feedback from the hand. While the role of sensory feedback in these corrections is well-established, much less is known about sensory prediction. The relative contributions of these two types of signals remain a subject of debate, as they are naturally interconnected. We address the issue in a study that compares online correction of an artificially induced, undetected planning error. Two conditions are tested, which only differ with respect to the accuracy of predicted visual reafferences. In the first, “Prism” experiment, a planning error is introduced by prisms that laterally displace the seen hand prior to hand movement onset. The prism-induced conflict between visual and proprioceptive inputs of the hand also generates an erroneous prediction of visual reafferences of the moving hand. In the second, “Jump” experiment, a planning error is introduced by a jump in the target position, during the orienting saccade, prior to hand movement onset. In the latter condition, predicted reafferences of the hand remained intact. In both experiments, after hand movement onset, the hand was either visible or hidden, which enabled us to manipulate the presence (or absence) of visual feedback during movement execution. The Prism experiment highlighted late and reduced correction of the planning error, even when natural visual feedback of the moving hand was available. In the Jump experiment, early and automatic corrections of the planning error were observed, even in the absence of visual feedback from the moving hand. Therefore, when predicted reafferences were accurate (the Jump experiment), visual feedback was processed rapidly and automatically. When they were erroneous (the Prism experiment), the same visual feedback was less efficient, and required voluntary, and late, control. Our study clearly demonstrates that in natural environments, reliable prediction is critical in the preprocessing of visual feedback, for fast and accurate movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne-Emmanuelle Priot
- Department of Neurosciences and Cognitive Sciences, Perception Team, Institut de Recherche Biomédicale des Armées, Brétigny-sur-Orge, France.,Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct/Trajectoires Team, Bron, France
| | - Patrice Revol
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct/Trajectoires Team, Bron, France.,Platform 'Mouvement et Handicap', Hôpital Henry Gabrielle, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Saint-Genis-Laval, France
| | - Olivier Sillan
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct/Trajectoires Team, Bron, France
| | - Claude Prablanc
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct/Trajectoires Team, Bron, France
| | - Valérie Gaveau
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct/Trajectoires Team, Bron, France.,Institute of Rehabilitation Sciences and Techniques (ISTR), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Lyon, France
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Kurtzer IL, Muraoka T, Singh T, Prasad M, Chauhan R, Adhami E. Reaching movements are automatically redirected to nearby options during target split. J Neurophysiol 2020; 124:1013-1028. [PMID: 32783570 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00336.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Motor behavior often occurs in environments with multiple goal options that can vary during the ongoing action. We explored this situation by requiring subjects to select between different target options during an ongoing reach. During split trials the original target was replaced with a left and a right flanking target, and participants had to select between them. This contrasted with the standard jump trials, where the original target would be replaced with a single flanking target, left or right. When participants were instructed to follow their natural tendency, they all tended to select the split target nearest the original. The near-target preference was more prominent with increased spatial disparity between the options and when participants could preview the potential options. Moreover, explicit instruction to obtain the "far" target during split trials resulted many errors compared with a "near" instruction, ~50% vs. ~15%. Online reaction times to target change were delayed in split trials compared with jump trials, ~200 ms vs. ~150 ms, but also highly automatic. Trials in which the instructed far target was correctly obtained were delayed by a further ~50 ms, unlike those in which the near target was incorrectly obtained. We also observed nonspecific responses from arm muscles at the jump trial latency during split trials. Taken together, our results indicate that online selection of reach targets is automatically linked to the spatial distribution of the options, though at greater delays than redirecting to a single target.NEW & NOTEWORTHY This work demonstrates that target selection during an ongoing reach is automatically linked to the option nearest a voided target. Online reaction times for two options are longer than redirection to a single option. Attempts to override the near-target tendency result in a high number of errors at the normal delay and further delays when the attempt is successful.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Isaac L Kurtzer
- Department of Biomedical Science, College of Osteopathic Medicine, New York Institute of Technology, Old Westbury, New York
| | - Tetsuro Muraoka
- College of Economics, Nihon University, Chiyoda City, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Tarkeshwar Singh
- Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania
| | - Mark Prasad
- Department of Biomedical Science, College of Osteopathic Medicine, New York Institute of Technology, Old Westbury, New York
| | - Riddhi Chauhan
- Department of Biomedical Science, College of Osteopathic Medicine, New York Institute of Technology, Old Westbury, New York
| | - Elan Adhami
- Department of Biomedical Science, College of Osteopathic Medicine, New York Institute of Technology, Old Westbury, New York
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Bringoux L, Macaluso T, Sainton P, Chomienne L, Buloup F, Mouchnino L, Simoneau M, Blouin J. Double-Step Paradigm in Microgravity: Preservation of Sensorimotor Flexibility in Altered Gravitational Force Field. Front Physiol 2020; 11:377. [PMID: 32390872 PMCID: PMC7193114 DOI: 10.3389/fphys.2020.00377] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2019] [Accepted: 03/30/2020] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
The way we can correct our ongoing movements to sudden and unforeseen perturbations is key to our ability to rapidly adjust our behavior to novel environmental demands. Referred to as sensorimotor flexibility, this ability can be assessed by the double-step paradigm in which participants must correct their ongoing arm movements to reach targets that unexpectedly change location (i.e., target jump). While this type of corrections has been demonstrated in normogravity in the extent of reasonable spatiotemporal constraints underpinning the target jumps, less is known about sensorimotor flexibility in altered gravitational force fields. We thus aimed to assess sensorimotor flexibility by comparing online arm pointing corrections observed during microgravity episodes of parabolic flights with normogravity standards. Seven participants were asked to point as fast and as accurately as possible toward one of two visual targets with their right index finger. The targets were aligned vertically in the mid-sagittal plane and were separated by 10 cm. In 20% of the trials, the initially illuminated lower target was switched off at movement onset while the upper target was concomitantly switched on prompting participants to change the trajectory of their ongoing movements. Results showed that, both in normogravity and microgravity, participants successfully performed the pointing task including when the target jumped unexpectedly (i.e., comparable success rate). Most importantly, no significant difference was found in target jump trials regarding arm kinematics between both gravitational environments, neither in terms of peak velocity, relative deceleration duration, peak acceleration or time to peak acceleration. Using inverse dynamics based on experimental and anthropometrical data, we demonstrated that the shoulder torques for accelerating and decelerating the vertical arm movements substantially differed between microgravity and normogravity. Our data therefore highlight the capacity of the central nervous system to perform very fast neuromuscular adjustments that are adapted to the gravitational constraints. We discuss our findings by considering the contribution of feedforward and feedback mechanisms in the online control of arm pointing movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Bringoux
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| | - T Macaluso
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| | - P Sainton
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| | - L Chomienne
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| | - F Buloup
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| | - L Mouchnino
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LNC, Marseille, France
| | - M Simoneau
- Département de Kinésiologie, Faculté de Médecine, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada.,Centre Interdisciplinaire de Recherche en Réadaptation et Intégration Sociale (CIRRIS) du CIUSSS de la Capitale Nationale, Quebec, QC, Canada
| | - J Blouin
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LNC, Marseille, France
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Time-to-Target Simplifies Optimal Control of Visuomotor Feedback Responses. eNeuro 2020; 7:ENEURO.0514-19.2020. [PMID: 32213555 PMCID: PMC7189480 DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0514-19.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2019] [Revised: 02/13/2020] [Accepted: 03/01/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Visuomotor feedback responses vary in intensity throughout a reach, commonly explained by optimal control. Here, we show that the optimal control for a range of movements with the same goal can be simplified to a time-to-target dependent control scheme. We measure our human participants’ visuomotor responses in five reaching conditions, each with different hand or cursor kinematics. Participants only produced different feedback responses when these kinematic changes resulted in different times-to-target. We complement our experimental data with a range of finite and non-finite horizon optimal feedback control (OFC) models, finding that the model with time-to-target as one of the input parameters best replicates the experimental data. Overall, this suggests that time-to-target is a critical control parameter in online feedback control. Moreover, we propose that for a specific task and known dynamics, humans can instantly produce a control signal without any additional online computation allowing rapid response onset and close to optimal control.
Collapse
|
23
|
Endpoint stiffness magnitude increases linearly with a stronger power grasp. Sci Rep 2020; 10:379. [PMID: 31941998 PMCID: PMC6962455 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-57267-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2019] [Accepted: 12/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans can increase the endpoint stiffness of their arm to reduce self-generated movement variability and to reject unpredictable perturbations from the environment, like during handheld drilling, thereby increasing movement precision. Existing methods to estimate changes in the endpoint stiffness use robotic interfaces to apply position or force perturbations to measure the arm's dynamic response. We propose an alternative method of measuring changes in the power grasp force to estimate adaptations in the magnitude of the arm's endpoint stiffness. To validate our method, we examined how the strength of the power grasp, when holding onto a robotic manipulandum, affected the arm's endpoint stiffness in three different locations of the workspace. The endpoint stiffness magnitude increased linearly with the grasp force, and this linear relationship did not depend on the arm's posture or position in the workspace. The endpoint stiffness may have increased as a combination of greater grasp stiffness and greater arm stiffness, since larger co-contraction was observed in the elbow and shoulder with a stronger grasp. Changes in the grasp force could serve as a metric in assessing how humans adapt their endpoint stiffness magnitude.
Collapse
|
24
|
Lu Z, Fiehler K. Spatial updating of allocentric landmark information in real-time and memory-guided reaching. Cortex 2020; 125:203-214. [PMID: 32006875 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/12/2019] [Revised: 09/16/2019] [Accepted: 12/12/2019] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
The 2-streams model of vision suggests that egocentric and allocentric reference frames are utilized by the dorsal and the ventral stream for real-time and memory-guided movements, respectively. Recent studies argue against such a strict functional distinction and suggest that real-time and memory-guided movements recruit the same spatial maps. In this study we focus on allocentric spatial coding and updating of targets by using landmark information in real-time and memory-guided reaching. We presented participants with a naturalistic scene which consisted of six objects on a table that served as potential reach targets. Participants were informed about the target object after scene encoding, and were prompted by a go cue to reach to its position. After target identification a brief air-puff was applied to the participant's right eye inducing an eye blink. During the blink the target object disappeared from the scene, and in half of the trials the remaining objects, that functioned as landmarks, were shifted horizontally in the same direction. We found that landmark shifts systematically influenced participants' reaching endpoints irrespective of whether the movements were controlled online based on available target information (real-time movement) or memory-guided based on remembered target information (memory-guided movement). Overall, the effect of landmark shift was stronger for memory-guided than real-time reaching. Our findings suggest that humans can encode and update reach targets in an allocentric reference frame for both real-time and memory-guided movements and show stronger allocentric coding when the movement is based on memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zijian Lu
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus-Liebig-University, Giessen, Germany.
| | - Katja Fiehler
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Justus-Liebig-University, Giessen, Germany; Center for Mind, Brain, and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus-Liebig University, Giessen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Mathew J, Flanagan JR, Danion FR. Gaze behavior during visuomotor tracking with complex hand-cursor dynamics. J Vis 2019; 19:24. [PMID: 31868897 DOI: 10.1167/19.14.24] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to track a moving target with the hand has been extensively studied, but few studies have characterized gaze behavior during this task. Here we investigate gaze behavior when participants learn a new mapping between hand and cursor motion, such that the cursor represented the position of a virtual mass attached to the grasped handle via a virtual spring. Depending on the experimental condition, haptic feedback consistent with mass-spring dynamics could also be provided. For comparison a simple one-to-one hand-cursor mapping was also tested. We hypothesized that gaze would be drawn, at times, to the cursor in the mass-spring conditions, especially in the absence of haptic feedback. As expected hand tracking performance was less accurate under the spring mapping, but gaze behavior was virtually unaffected by the spring mapping, regardless of whether haptic feedback was provided. Specifically, relative gaze position between target and cursor, rate of saccades, and gain of smooth pursuit were similar under both mappings and both haptic feedback conditions. We conclude that even when participants are exposed to a challenging hand-cursor mapping, gaze is primarily concerned about ongoing target motion suggesting that peripheral vision is sufficient to monitor cursor position and to update hand movement control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James Mathew
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, Marseille, France.,Current affiliation: Institute of Neuroscience, Institute of Communication & Information Technologies, Electronics & Applied Mathematics, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-neuve, Belgium
| | - J Randall Flanagan
- Department of Psychology and Centre for Neurosciences Studies, Queens University, Ontario, Canada
| | - Frederic R Danion
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, Marseille, France
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
de Mathelin M, Nageotte F, Zanne P, Dresp-Langley B. Sensors for Expert Grip Force Profiling: Towards Benchmarking Manual Control of a Robotic Device for Surgical Tool Movements. SENSORS 2019; 19:s19204575. [PMID: 31640204 PMCID: PMC6848933 DOI: 10.3390/s19204575] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2019] [Revised: 10/04/2019] [Accepted: 10/17/2019] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
STRAS (Single access Transluminal Robotic Assistant for Surgeons) is a new robotic system based on the Anubis® platform of Karl Storz for application to intra-luminal surgical procedures. Pre-clinical testing of STRAS has recently permitted to demonstrate major advantages of the system in comparison with classic procedures. Benchmark methods permitting to establish objective criteria for 'expertise' need to be worked out now to effectively train surgeons on this new system in the near future. STRAS consists of three cable-driven sub-systems, one endoscope serving as guide, and two flexible instruments. The flexible instruments have three degrees of freedom and can be teleoperated by a single user via two specially designed master interfaces. In this study, small force sensors sewn into a wearable glove to ergonomically fit the master handles of the robotic system were employed for monitoring the forces applied by an expert and a trainee (complete novice) during all the steps of surgical task execution in a simulator task (4-step-pick-and-drop). Analysis of grip-force profiles is performed sensor by sensor to bring to the fore specific differences in handgrip force profiles in specific sensor locations on anatomically relevant parts of the fingers and hand controlling the master/slave system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michel de Mathelin
- ICube Lab, UMR 7357 CNRS, Robotics Department, University of Strasbourg, 6700 Strasbourg, France.
| | - Florent Nageotte
- ICube Lab, UMR 7357 CNRS, Robotics Department, University of Strasbourg, 6700 Strasbourg, France.
| | - Philippe Zanne
- ICube Lab, UMR 7357 CNRS, Robotics Department, University of Strasbourg, 6700 Strasbourg, France.
| | - Birgitta Dresp-Langley
- ICube Lab, UMR 7357 CNRS, Robotics Department, University of Strasbourg, 6700 Strasbourg, France.
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Martel M, Fourneret P, Finos L, Schmitz C, Catherine Roy A. Highs and Lows in Motor Control Development. J Mot Behav 2019; 52:404-417. [PMID: 31339466 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2019.1643283] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Motor control is classically described as relying on two components: anticipatory control (feedforward processing) and online control (feedback processing). Here we aimed to unveil the developmental steps of both feedback and feedforward control in 5-10 years old children, using a simple and ecological task. We manipulated object's weight in a reach-to-displace paradigm. When the weight was known before lifting it, anticipatory processes were quantifiable during the reaching phase. Conversely, an unknown weight triggered online corrections during the displacing phase. Movement kinematics revealed that children anticipate this objet property as young as 5 y-o. This anticipation becomes adequate around 7 y-o and is paralleled by poor online corrections. This simple yet relevant paradigm should allow quantifying deviations from neurotypical patterns in disorders of motor control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marie Martel
- Laboratoire Dynamique Du Langage, Université Lyon 2, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France.,Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
| | - Pierre Fourneret
- Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France.,Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, Service de Psychopathologie du Développement, Hôpital Femme-Mère-Enfant, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Bron, France
| | - Livio Finos
- Department of Statistical Sciences, University of Padova, Padua, Italy
| | - Christina Schmitz
- Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France.,Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
| | - Alice Catherine Roy
- Laboratoire Dynamique Du Langage, Université Lyon 2, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. Continuously updating one’s predictions underlies successful interception. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:3257-3274. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00517.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
This paper reviews our understanding of the interception of moving objects. Interception is a demanding task that requires both spatial and temporal precision. The required precision must be achieved on the basis of imprecise and sometimes biased sensory information. We argue that people make precise interceptive movements by continuously adjusting their movements. Initial estimates of how the movement should progress can be quite inaccurate. As the movement evolves, the estimate of how the rest of the movement should progress gradually becomes more reliable as prediction is replaced by sensory information about the progress of the movement. The improvement is particularly important when things do not progress as anticipated. Constantly adjusting one’s estimate of how the movement should progress combines the opportunity to move in a way that one anticipates will best meet the task demands with correcting for any errors in such anticipation. The fact that the ongoing movement might have to be adjusted can be considered when determining how to move, and any systematic anticipation errors can be corrected on the basis of the outcome of earlier actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eli Brenner
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Jeroen B. J. Smeets
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
de Brouwer AJ, Gallivan JP, Flanagan JR. Visuomotor feedback gains are modulated by gaze position. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:2522-2531. [PMID: 30183472 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00182.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
During goal-directed reaching, people typically direct their gaze to the target before the start of the hand movement and maintain fixation until the hand arrives. This gaze strategy improves reach accuracy in two ways. It enables the use of central vision at the end of movement, and it allows the use of extraretinal information in guiding the hand to the target. Here we tested whether fixating the reach target further facilitates reach accuracy by optimizing the use of peripheral vision in detecting, and rapidly responding to, reach errors during the ongoing movement. We examined automatic visuomotor corrections in response to displacements of the cursor representing the hand position as a function of gaze fixation location during unimanual goal-directed reaching. Eight fixation targets were positioned either in line with, or at different angles relative to, the straight-ahead movement direction (manipulation of fixation angle), and at different distances from the location of the visual perturbation (manipulation of fixation distance). We found that corrections were fastest and strongest when gaze was directed at the reach target compared with when gaze was directed to a different location in the workspace. We found that the gain of the visuomotor response was strongly affected by fixation angle, and to a smaller extent by fixation distance, with lower gains as the angle or distance increased. We submit that fixating the reach target improves reach accuracy by facilitating rapid visuomotor responses to reach errors viewed in peripheral vision. NEW & NOTEWORTHY It is well known that directing gaze to the reach target allows the use of foveal visual feedback and extraretinal information to improve the accuracy of reaching movements. Here we demonstrate that target fixation also optimizes rapid visuomotor corrections to reach errors viewed in peripheral vision, with the angle of gaze relative to the hand movement being a critical determinant in the gain of the visuomotor response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anouk J de Brouwer
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University , Kingston, Ontario , Canada
| | - Jason P Gallivan
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University , Kingston, Ontario , Canada.,Department of Psychology, Queen's University , Kingston, Ontario , Canada.,Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen's University , Kingston, Ontario , Canada
| | - J Randall Flanagan
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University , Kingston, Ontario , Canada.,Department of Psychology, Queen's University , Kingston, Ontario , Canada
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Batmaz AU, de Mathelin M, Dresp-Langley B. Effects of 2D and 3D image views on hand movement trajectories in the surgeon’s peri-personal space in a computer controlled simulator environment. COGENT MEDICINE 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/2331205x.2018.1426232] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Anil Ufuk Batmaz
- ICube Lab, CNRS and University of Strasbourg, UMR 7357, Strasbourg, France
| | - Michel de Mathelin
- ICube Lab, CNRS and University of Strasbourg, UMR 7357, Strasbourg, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Oostwoud Wijdenes L, Medendorp WP. State Estimation for Early Feedback Responses in Reaching: Intramodal or Multimodal? Front Integr Neurosci 2017; 11:38. [PMID: 29311860 PMCID: PMC5742230 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2017.00038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2017] [Accepted: 12/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans are highly skilled in controlling their reaching movements, making fast and task-dependent movement corrections to unforeseen perturbations. To guide these corrections, the neural control system requires a continuous, instantaneous estimate of the current state of the arm and body in the world. According to Optimal Feedback Control theory, this estimate is multimodal and constructed based on the integration of forward motor predictions and sensory feedback, such as proprioceptive, visual and vestibular information, modulated by context, and shaped by past experience. But how can a multimodal estimate drive fast movement corrections, given that the involved sensory modalities have different processing delays, different coordinate representations, and different noise levels? We develop the hypothesis that the earliest online movement corrections are based on multiple single modality state estimates rather than one combined multimodal estimate. We review studies that have investigated online multimodal integration for reach control and offer suggestions for experiments to test for the existence of intramodal state estimates. If proven true, the framework of Optimal Feedback Control needs to be extended with a stage of intramodal state estimation, serving to drive short-latency movement corrections.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - W Pieter Medendorp
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Matsui H, Ryu M, Kawabata H. Visual Feedback of Target Position Affects Accuracy of Sequential Movements at Even Spaces. J Mot Behav 2017; 50:689-696. [PMID: 29236615 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2017.1407744] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
The role of visual feedback during movement is attributed to its accuracy, but findings regarding the utilization of this information are inconsistent. We developed a novel dot-placing task to investigate the role of vision in arm movements. Participants conducted pointing-like movements between two target stimuli at even spaces. In Experiment 1, visual feedback of targets and response positions was manipulated. Although visual loss of target stimuli hindered accuracy of movements, the absence of the position of previously placed dots had little effect. In Experiment 2, the effect of movement time on accuracy was assessed, as the relationship between these has been traditionally understood as a speed/accuracy trade-off. Results revealed that duration of movement did not impact movement accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hiroshi Matsui
- a Department of Psychology , Keio University , Tokyo , Japan
| | - Marika Ryu
- a Department of Psychology , Keio University , Tokyo , Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Abstract spatial, but not body-related, visual information guides bimanual coordination. Sci Rep 2017; 7:16732. [PMID: 29196722 PMCID: PMC5711801 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-16860-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2017] [Accepted: 11/19/2017] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Visual spatial information is paramount in guiding bimanual coordination, but anatomical factors, too, modulate performance in bimanual tasks. Vision conveys not only abstract spatial information, but also informs about body-related aspects such as posture. Here, we asked whether, accordingly, visual information induces body-related, or merely abstract, perceptual-spatial constraints in bimanual movement guidance. Human participants made rhythmic, symmetrical and parallel, bimanual index finger movements with the hands held in the same or different orientations. Performance was more accurate for symmetrical than parallel movements in all postures, but additionally when homologous muscles were concurrently active, such as when parallel movements were performed with differently rather than identically oriented hands. Thus, both perceptual and anatomical constraints were evident. We manipulated visual feedback with a mirror between the hands, replacing the image of the right with that of the left hand and creating the visual impression of bimanual symmetry independent of the right hand’s true movement. Symmetrical mirror feedback impaired parallel, but improved symmetrical bimanual performance compared with regular hand view. Critically, these modulations were independent of hand posture and muscle homology. Thus, visual feedback appears to contribute exclusively to spatial, but not to body-related, anatomical movement coding in the guidance of bimanual coordination.
Collapse
|
34
|
Macaluso T, Bourdin C, Buloup F, Mille ML, Sainton P, Sarlegna FR, Vercher JL, Bringoux L. Sensorimotor Reorganizations of Arm Kinematics and Postural Strategy for Functional Whole-Body Reaching Movements in Microgravity. Front Physiol 2017; 8:821. [PMID: 29104544 PMCID: PMC5654841 DOI: 10.3389/fphys.2017.00821] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2017] [Accepted: 10/05/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding the impact of weightlessness on human behavior during the forthcoming long-term space missions is of critical importance, especially when considering the efficiency of goal-directed movements in these unusual environments. Several studies provided a large set of evidence that gravity is taken into account during the planning stage of arm reaching movements to optimally anticipate its consequence upon the moving limbs. However, less is known about sensorimotor changes required to face weightless environments when individuals have to perform fast and accurate goal-directed actions with whole-body displacement. We thus aimed at characterizing kinematic features of whole-body reaching movements in microgravity, involving high spatiotemporal constraints of execution, to question whether and how humans are able to maintain the performance of a functional behavior in the standards of normogravity execution. Seven participants were asked to reach as fast and as accurately as possible visual targets while standing during microgravity episodes in parabolic flight. Small and large targets were presented either close or far from the participants (requiring, in the latter case, additional whole-body displacement). Results reported that participants successfully performed the reaching task with general temporal features of movement (e.g., movement speed) close to land observations. However, our analyses also demonstrated substantial kinematic changes related to the temporal structure of focal movement and the postural strategy to successfully perform -constrained- whole-body reaching movements in microgravity. These immediate reorganizations are likely achieved by rapidly taking into account the absence of gravity in motor preparation and execution (presumably from cues about body limbs unweighting). Specifically, when compared to normogravity, the arm deceleration phase substantially increased. Furthermore, greater whole-body forward displacements due to smaller trunk flexions occurred when reaching far targets in microgravity. Remarkably, these changes of focal kinematics and postural strategy appear close to those previously reported when participants performed the same task underwater with neutral buoyancy applied to body limbs. Overall, these novel findings reveal that humans are able to maintain the performance of functional goal-directed whole-body actions in weightlessness by successfully managing spatiotemporal constraints of execution in this unusual environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Frank Buloup
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France
| | - Marie-Laure Mille
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Marseille, France.,UFR STAPS, Université de Toulon, La Garde, France.,Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, United States
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Batmaz AU, de Mathelin M, Dresp-Langley B. Seeing virtual while acting real: Visual display and strategy effects on the time and precision of eye-hand coordination. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0183789. [PMID: 28859092 PMCID: PMC5578485 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183789] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2016] [Accepted: 08/11/2017] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Effects of different visual displays on the time and precision of bare-handed or tool-mediated eye-hand coordination were investigated in a pick-and-place-task with complete novices. All of them scored well above average in spatial perspective taking ability and performed the task with their dominant hand. Two groups of novices, four men and four women in each group, had to place a small object in a precise order on the centre of five targets on a Real-world Action Field (RAF), as swiftly as possible and as precisely as possible, using a tool or not (control). Each individual session consisted of four visual display conditions. The order of conditions was counterbalanced between individuals and sessions. Subjects looked at what their hands were doing 1) directly in front of them (“natural” top-down view) 2) in top-down 2D fisheye view 3) in top-down undistorted 2D view or 4) in 3D stereoscopic top-down view (head-mounted OCULUS DK 2). It was made sure that object movements in all image conditions matched the real-world movements in time and space. One group was looking at the 2D images with the monitor positioned sideways (sub-optimal); the other group was looking at the monitor placed straight ahead of them (near-optimal). All image viewing conditions had significantly detrimental effects on time (seconds) and precision (pixels) of task execution when compared with “natural” direct viewing. More importantly, we find significant trade-offs between time and precision between and within groups, and significant interactions between viewing conditions and manipulation conditions. The results shed new light on controversial findings relative to visual display effects on eye-hand coordination, and lead to conclude that differences in camera systems and adaptive strategies of novices are likely to explain these.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anil U. Batmaz
- ICube Lab Robotics Department, University of Strasbourg, 1 Place de l'Hôpital, Strasbourg, France
| | - Michel de Mathelin
- ICube Lab Robotics Department, University of Strasbourg, 1 Place de l'Hôpital, Strasbourg, France
| | - Birgitta Dresp-Langley
- ICube Lab Cognitive Science Department, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, 1 Place de l'Hôpital, Strasbourg, France
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Franklin S, Wolpert DM, Franklin DW. Rapid visuomotor feedback gains are tuned to the task dynamics. J Neurophysiol 2017; 118:2711-2726. [PMID: 28835530 PMCID: PMC5672538 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00748.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2016] [Revised: 07/24/2017] [Accepted: 08/18/2017] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Here, we test whether rapid visuomotor feedback responses are selectively tuned to the task dynamics. The responses do not exhibit gain scaling, but they do vary with the level and stability of task dynamics. Moreover, these feedback gains are independently tuned to perturbations to the left and right, depending on these dynamics. Our results demonstrate that the sensorimotor control system regulates the feedback gain as part of the adaptation process, tuning them appropriately to the environment. Adaptation to novel dynamics requires learning a motor memory, or a new pattern of predictive feedforward motor commands. Recently, we demonstrated the upregulation of rapid visuomotor feedback gains early in curl force field learning, which decrease once a predictive motor memory is learned. However, even after learning is complete, these feedback gains are higher than those observed in the null field trials. Interestingly, these upregulated feedback gains in the curl field were not observed in a constant force field. Therefore, we suggest that adaptation also involves selectively tuning the feedback sensitivity of the sensorimotor control system to the environment. Here, we test this hypothesis by measuring the rapid visuomotor feedback gains after subjects adapt to a variety of novel dynamics generated by a robotic manipulandum in three experiments. To probe the feedback gains, we measured the magnitude of the motor response to rapid shifts in the visual location of the hand during reaching. While the feedback gain magnitude remained similar over a larger than a fourfold increase in constant background load, the feedback gains scaled with increasing lateral resistance and increasing instability. The third experiment demonstrated that the feedback gains could also be independently tuned to perturbations to the left and right, depending on the lateral resistance, demonstrating the fractionation of feedback gains to environmental dynamics. Our results show that the sensorimotor control system regulates the gain of the feedback system as part of the adaptation process to novel dynamics, appropriately tuning them to the environment. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Here, we test whether rapid visuomotor feedback responses are selectively tuned to the task dynamics. The responses do not exhibit gain scaling, but they do vary with the level and stability of task dynamics. Moreover, these feedback gains are independently tuned to perturbations to the left and right, depending on these dynamics. Our results demonstrate that the sensorimotor control system regulates the feedback gain as part of the adaptation process, tuning them appropriately to the environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sae Franklin
- Computational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom.,Institute for Cognitive Systems, Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany; and
| | - Daniel M Wolpert
- Computational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - David W Franklin
- Computational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom; .,Neuromuscular Diagnostics, Department of Sport and Health Sciences, Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
de Grosbois J, Tremblay L. Distinct and flexible rates of online control. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2017; 82:1054-1072. [PMID: 28733770 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-017-0888-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2016] [Accepted: 07/03/2017] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
Elliott et al. (Hum Mov Sci 10:393-418, 1991) proposed a pseudocontinuous model of online control whereby overlapping corrections lead to the appearance of smooth kinematic profiles in the presence of online feedback. More recently, it was also proposed that online control is not a singular process [see Elliott et al. (Psychol Bull 136(6):1023-1044, 2010)]. However, support for contemporary models of online control were based on methodologies that were not designed to be sensitive to different online control sub-processes. The current study sought to evaluate the possibility of multiple distinct (i.e., visual and non-visual) mechanisms contributing to the control of reaching movements completed in either a full-vision, a no-vision, or a no-vision memory-guided condition. Frequency domain analysis was applied to the acceleration traces of reaching movements. In an attempt to elicit a modulation in the online control mechanisms, these movements were completed at two levels of spatio-temporal constraint, namely with 10 and 30 cm target distances. One finding was that performance in the full-vision relative to both no-vision conditions could be distinguished via two distinct frequency peaks. Increases in the peak magnitude at the lower frequencies were associated with visuomotor mechanisms and increases in the peak magnitude at the higher frequencies were associated with non-visual mechanisms. In addition, performance to the 30-cm target led to a lower peak at a lower frequency relative to the 10 cm target, indicating that the iterative rates of visuomotor control mechanisms are flexible and sensitive to the spatio-temporal constraints of the associated movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John de Grosbois
- Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Toronto, 55 Harbord St., Toronto, ON, M5S 2W6, Canada.,Centre for Motor Control, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Perceptual-Motor Behaviour Laboratory, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Luc Tremblay
- Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Toronto, 55 Harbord St., Toronto, ON, M5S 2W6, Canada. .,Centre for Motor Control, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. .,Perceptual-Motor Behaviour Laboratory, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Demchenko I, Katz R, Pratt H, Zacksenhouse M. Distinct electroencephalographic responses to disturbances and distractors during continuous reaching movements. Brain Res 2016; 1652:178-187. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.09.040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2016] [Revised: 09/23/2016] [Accepted: 09/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
39
|
Batmaz AU, de Mathelin M, Dresp-Langley B. Getting nowhere fast: trade-off between speed and precision in training to execute image-guided hand-tool movements. BMC Psychol 2016; 4:55. [PMID: 27842577 PMCID: PMC5109684 DOI: 10.1186/s40359-016-0161-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2016] [Accepted: 10/27/2016] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Background The speed and precision with which objects are moved by hand or hand-tool interaction under image guidance depend on a specific type of visual and spatial sensorimotor learning. Novices have to learn to optimally control what their hands are doing in a real-world environment while looking at an image representation of the scene on a video monitor. Previous research has shown slower task execution times and lower performance scores under image-guidance compared with situations of direct action viewing. The cognitive processes for overcoming this drawback by training are not yet understood. Methods We investigated the effects of training on the time and precision of direct view versus image guided object positioning on targets of a Real-world Action Field (RAF). Two men and two women had to learn to perform the task as swiftly and as precisely as possible with their dominant hand, using a tool or not and wearing a glove or not. Individuals were trained in sessions of mixed trial blocks with no feed-back. Results As predicted, image-guidance produced significantly slower times and lesser precision in all trainees and sessions compared with direct viewing. With training, all trainees get faster in all conditions, but only one of them gets reliably more precise in the image-guided conditions. Speed-accuracy trade-offs in the individual performance data show that the highest precision scores and steepest learning curve, for time and precision, were produced by the slowest starter. Fast starters produced consistently poorer precision scores in all sessions. The fastest starter showed no sign of stable precision learning, even after extended training. Conclusions Performance evolution towards optimal precision is compromised when novices start by going as fast as they can. The findings have direct implications for individual skill monitoring in training programmes for image-guided technology applications with human operators.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anil Ufuk Batmaz
- Laboratoire ICube UMR 7357 CNRS-University of Strasbourg, 2, rue Boussingault, 67000, Strasbourg, France
| | - Michel de Mathelin
- Laboratoire ICube UMR 7357 CNRS-University of Strasbourg, 2, rue Boussingault, 67000, Strasbourg, France
| | - Birgitta Dresp-Langley
- Laboratoire ICube UMR 7357 CNRS-University of Strasbourg, 2, rue Boussingault, 67000, Strasbourg, France.
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Mackrous I, Proteau L. Visual Online Control of Goal-Directed Aiming Movements in Children. Front Psychol 2016; 7:989. [PMID: 27458399 PMCID: PMC4932315 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00989] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2016] [Accepted: 06/15/2016] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
The present study investigated whether the initial impulse of goal-directed movements was visually monitored by 5- to 12-years-old children (n = 36) in a manner similar to adults (n = 12). The participants moved a cursor toward a fixed target. In some trials, the cursor was unpredictably translated by 20 mm following movement initiation. The results showed that even the youngest children visually monitor the initial impulse of goal-directed movements. This monitoring and the error correction process that it triggers seem automatic because it occurs even when the cursor jump is not consciously detected. Finally, it appears that this process does not fully mature before late childhood, which suggests that a putative dedicated channel for processing visual hand information develops during childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Isabelle Mackrous
- Département de Kinésiologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal QC, Canada
| | - Luc Proteau
- Département de Kinésiologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Rousseau C, Papaxanthis C, Gaveau J, Pozzo T, White O. Initial information prior to movement onset influences kinematics of upward arm pointing movements. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116:1673-1683. [PMID: 27486106 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00616.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2015] [Accepted: 07/11/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
To elaborate a motor plan and perform online control in the gravity field, the brain relies on priors and multisensory integration of information. In particular, afferent and efferent inputs related to the initial state are thought to convey sensorimotor information to plan the upcoming action. Yet it is still unclear to what extent these cues impact motor planning. Here we examined the role of initial information on the planning and execution of arm movements. Participants performed upward arm movements around the shoulder at three speeds and in two arm conditions. In the first condition, the arm was outstretched horizontally and required a significant muscular command to compensate for the gravitational shoulder torque before movement onset. In contrast, in the second condition the arm was passively maintained in the same position with a cushioned support and did not require any muscle contraction before movement execution. We quantified differences in motor performance by comparing shoulder velocity profiles. Previous studies showed that asymmetric velocity profiles reflect an optimal integration of the effects of gravity on upward movements. Consistent with this, we found decreased acceleration durations in both arm conditions. However, early differences in kinematic asymmetries and EMG patterns between the two conditions signaled a change of the motor plan. This different behavior carried on through trials when the arm was at rest before movement onset and may reveal a distinct motor strategy chosen in the context of uncertainty. Altogether, we suggest that the information available online must be complemented by accurate initial information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Célia Rousseau
- Université de Bourgogne Franche-Comté (UBFC), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; Institut National de Santé et de Recherche Médicale (INSERM U1093), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; and
| | - Charalambos Papaxanthis
- Université de Bourgogne Franche-Comté (UBFC), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; Institut National de Santé et de Recherche Médicale (INSERM U1093), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; and
| | - Jérémie Gaveau
- Université de Bourgogne Franche-Comté (UBFC), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; Institut National de Santé et de Recherche Médicale (INSERM U1093), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; and
| | - Thierry Pozzo
- Université de Bourgogne Franche-Comté (UBFC), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; Institut National de Santé et de Recherche Médicale (INSERM U1093), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; and Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France
| | - Olivier White
- Université de Bourgogne Franche-Comté (UBFC), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; Institut National de Santé et de Recherche Médicale (INSERM U1093), Cognition Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice (CAPS) UMR1093, Dijon, France; and
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Macaluso T, Bourdin C, Buloup F, Mille ML, Sainton P, Sarlegna FR, Taillebot V, Vercher JL, Weiss P, Bringoux L. Kinematic features of whole-body reaching movements underwater: Neutral buoyancy effects. Neuroscience 2016; 327:125-35. [PMID: 27095713 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.04.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2015] [Revised: 04/08/2016] [Accepted: 04/09/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Astronauts' training is conventionally performed in a pool to reproduce weightlessness by exploiting buoyancy which is supposed to reduce the impact of gravity on the body. However, this training method has not been scientifically validated yet, and requires first to study the effects of underwater exposure on motor behavior. We examined the influence of neutral buoyancy on kinematic features of whole-body reaching underwater and compared them with those produced on land. Eight professional divers were asked to perform arm reaching movements toward visual targets while standing. Targets were presented either close or far from the subjects (requiring in the latter case an additional whole-body displacement). Reaching movements were performed on land or underwater in two different contexts of buoyancy. The divers either wore a diving suit only with neutral buoyancy applied to their center of mass or were additionally equipped with a submersible simulated space suit with neutral buoyancy applied to their body limbs. Results showed that underwater exposure impacted basic movement features, especially movement speed which was reduced. However, movement kinematics also differed according to the way buoyancy was exerted on the whole-body. When neutral buoyancy was applied to the center of mass only, some focal and postural components of whole-body reaching remained close to land observations, notably when considering the relative deceleration duration of arm elevation and concomitant forward trunk bending when reaching the far target. On the contrary, when neutral buoyancy was exerted on body segments, movement kinematics were close to those reported in weightlessness, as reflected by the arm deceleration phase and the whole-body forward displacement when reaching the far target. These results suggest that astronauts could benefit from the application of neutral buoyancy across the whole-body segments to optimize underwater training and acquire specific motor skills which will be used in space.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- T Macaluso
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France
| | - C Bourdin
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France
| | - F Buloup
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France
| | - M-L Mille
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France; Université de Toulon, 83957 La Garde, France; Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University Medical School, Chicago, IL 60611, United States
| | - P Sainton
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France
| | - F R Sarlegna
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France
| | - V Taillebot
- COMEX S.A., 36 Bvd des Océans, 13009 Marseille, France
| | - J-L Vercher
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France
| | - P Weiss
- COMEX S.A., 36 Bvd des Océans, 13009 Marseille, France
| | - L Bringoux
- Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, 13288 Marseille Cedex 09, France.
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Abstract
Goal-directed movements are subject to intrinsic planning and execution variability, which requires that the central nervous system closely monitor our movements to ensure endpoint accuracy. In the present study, we sought to determine how closely the visual system monitored goal-directed aiming movements. We used a cursor-jump paradigm in which a cursor was unexpectedly translated soon after movement initiation. Some of the trials included a second cursor jump, and the cursor remained visible for different durations. The results indicate that seeing the cursor for only 16 ms after the second cursor jump was sufficient to influence the movement endpoint, which suggests that the visual system continuously monitored goal-directed movements. The results also suggest that the perceived position/trajectory of the effector was likely to have been averaged over a period of approximately 70 ms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julien Brière
- a Département de kinésiologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| | - Luc Proteau
- a Département de kinésiologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Franklin DW, Reichenbach A, Franklin S, Diedrichsen J. Temporal Evolution of Spatial Computations for Visuomotor Control. J Neurosci 2016; 36:2329-41. [PMID: 26911681 PMCID: PMC4764656 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0052-15.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2015] [Revised: 09/11/2015] [Accepted: 10/29/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Goal-directed reaching movements are guided by visual feedback from both target and hand. The classical view is that the brain extracts information about target and hand positions from a visual scene, calculates a difference vector between them, and uses this estimate to control the movement. Here we show that during fast feedback control, this computation is not immediate, but evolves dynamically over time. Immediately after a change in the visual scene, the motor system generates independent responses to the errors in hand and target location. Only about 200 ms later, the changes in target and hand positions are combined appropriately in the response, slowly converging to the true difference vector. Therefore, our results provide evidence for the temporal evolution of spatial computations in the human visuomotor system, in which the accurate difference vector computation is first estimated by a fast approximation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David W Franklin
- Computational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1PZ, United Kingdom,
| | - Alexandra Reichenbach
- Motor Control Group, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom, and
| | - Sae Franklin
- Computational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1PZ, United Kingdom
| | - Jörn Diedrichsen
- Motor Control Group, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom, and Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Rapid Visuomotor Corrective Responses during Transport of Hand-Held Objects Incorporate Novel Object Dynamics. J Neurosci 2015. [PMID: 26203151 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1376-15.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Numerous studies have shown that people are adept at learning novel object dynamics, linking applied force and motion, when performing reaching movements with hand-held objects. Here we investigated whether the control of rapid corrective arm responses, elicited in response to visual perturbations, has access to such newly acquired knowledge of object dynamics. Participants first learned to make reaching movements while grasping an object subjected to complex load forces that depended on the distance and angle of the hand from the start position. During a subsequent test phase, we examined grip and load force coordination during corrective arm movements elicited (within ∼150 ms) in response to viewed sudden lateral shifts (1.5 cm) in target or object position. We hypothesized that, if knowledge of object dynamics is incorporated in the control of the corrective responses, grip force changes would anticipate the unusual load force changes associated with the corrective arm movements so as to support grasp stability. Indeed, we found that the participants generated grip force adjustments tightly coupled, both spatially and temporally, to the load force changes associated with the arm movement corrections. We submit that recently learned novel object dynamics are effectively integrated into sensorimotor control policies that support rapid visually driven arm corrective actions during transport of hand held objects. Significance statement: Previous studies have demonstrated that the motor system can learn, and make use of, internal models of object dynamics to generate feedforward motor commands. However, it is not known whether such internal models are incorporated into rapid, automatic arm movement corrections that compensate for errors that arise during movement. Here we demonstrate, for the first time, that internal models of novel object dynamics are integrated into rapid corrective arm movements made in response to visuomotor perturbations that, importantly, do not directly perturb the object.
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
The target article (Smeets, Oostwoud Wijdenes, & Brenner, 2016) proposes that short latency responses to changes in target location during reaching reflect an unconscious, continuous, and incremental minimization of the distance between the hand and the target, which does not require detection of the change in target location. We, instead, propose that short-latency visuomotor responses invoke reflex- or startle-like mechanisms, an idea supported by evidence that such responses are both automatic and resistant to cognitive influences. In addition, the target article fails to address the biological underpinnings for the range of response latencies reported across the literature, including the circuits that might underlie the proposed sensorimotor loops. When considering the range of latencies reported in the literature, we propose that mechanisms grounded in neurophysiology should be more informative than the simple information processing perspective adopted by the target article.
Collapse
|
47
|
Feedback control during voluntary motor actions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2015; 33:85-94. [DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2014] [Revised: 03/10/2015] [Accepted: 03/11/2015] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
48
|
Cluff T, Crevecoeur F, Scott SH. A perspective on multisensory integration and rapid perturbation responses. Vision Res 2015; 110:215-22. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.06.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2014] [Revised: 06/01/2014] [Accepted: 06/23/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
49
|
Fukui T, Inui T. Use of early phase online vision for grip configuration is modulated according to movement duration in prehension. Exp Brain Res 2015; 233:2257-68. [PMID: 25929554 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4295-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2014] [Accepted: 04/18/2015] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Our previous study (Hum Mov Sci 25:349-371, 2006) investigated whether and how online vision in the early phase of movement influences the control of reach-to-grasp movements (movement duration: approximately 1000 ms). We used liquid-crystal shutter goggles to manipulate the duration of available online vision during the movement and specified that online vision during the early phase influences grasping movements. The current study examined the effect of online early phase vision on the grip configuration according to the movement duration and compared it between two different movement durations (approximately 500 and 1000 ms). We found that non-availability of early phase online vision affected the grip configuration (i.e., inducing a larger peak grip aperture) even in the shorter movement duration. The influential period for online vision for grasping control shifts to an earlier time when movement time is shorter (i.e., from approximately 214 to 106 ms after movement onset), indicating a flexible mechanism for grip configuration according to the movement duration and the available online vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Takao Fukui
- Department of Intelligence Science and Technology, Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Yoshida-Honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto, 606-8501, Japan,
| | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Zhang L, Yang J, Inai Y, Huang Q, Wu J. Effects of aging on pointing movements under restricted visual feedback conditions. Hum Mov Sci 2014; 40:1-13. [PMID: 25506638 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.11.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2014] [Revised: 11/11/2014] [Accepted: 11/13/2014] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The goal of this study was to investigate the effects of aging on pointing movements under restricted visual feedback of hand movement and target location. Fifteen young subjects and fifteen elderly subjects performed pointing movements under four restricted visual feedback conditions that included full visual feedback of hand movement and target location (FV), no visual feedback of hand movement and target location condition (NV), no visual feedback of hand movement (NM) and no visual feedback of target location (NT). This study suggested that Fitts' law applied for pointing movements of the elderly adults under different visual restriction conditions. Moreover, significant main effect of aging on movement times has been found in all four tasks. The peripheral and central changes may be the key factors for these different characteristics. Furthermore, no significant main effects of age on the mean accuracy rate under condition of restricted visual feedback were found. The present study suggested that the elderly subjects made a very similar use of the available sensory information as young subjects under restricted visual feedback conditions. In addition, during the pointing movement, information about the hand's movement was more useful than information about the target location for young and elderly subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liancun Zhang
- Intelligent Robotics Institute, School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, 5 Nandajie, Zhongguancun, Haidian, Beijing 100081, China; Key Laboratory of Biomimetic Robots and Systems, Ministry of Education, State Key Laboratory of Intelligent Control and Decision of Complex Systems, Beijing Institute of Technology, 5 Nandajie, Zhongguancun, Haidian, Beijing 100081, China; Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, 3-1-1 Tsushima-Naka, Kita-Ku, Okayama 700-8530, Japan
| | - Jiajia Yang
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, 3-1-1 Tsushima-Naka, Kita-Ku, Okayama 700-8530, Japan
| | - Yoshinobu Inai
- Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, 3-1-1 Tsushima-Naka, Kita-Ku, Okayama 700-8530, Japan
| | - Qiang Huang
- Intelligent Robotics Institute, School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, 5 Nandajie, Zhongguancun, Haidian, Beijing 100081, China; Key Laboratory of Biomimetic Robots and Systems, Ministry of Education, State Key Laboratory of Intelligent Control and Decision of Complex Systems, Beijing Institute of Technology, 5 Nandajie, Zhongguancun, Haidian, Beijing 100081, China.
| | - Jinglong Wu
- Intelligent Robotics Institute, School of Mechatronical Engineering, Beijing Institute of Technology, 5 Nandajie, Zhongguancun, Haidian, Beijing 100081, China; Key Laboratory of Biomimetic Robots and Systems, Ministry of Education, State Key Laboratory of Intelligent Control and Decision of Complex Systems, Beijing Institute of Technology, 5 Nandajie, Zhongguancun, Haidian, Beijing 100081, China; Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, 3-1-1 Tsushima-Naka, Kita-Ku, Okayama 700-8530, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|