1
|
Ramirez-Amaro K, Beetz M, Cheng G. Transferring skills to humanoid robots by extracting semantic representations from observations of human activities. ARTIF INTELL 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.artint.2015.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
2
|
Vu VH, Isableu B, Berret B. Adaptive use of interaction torque during arm reaching movement from the optimal control viewpoint. Sci Rep 2016; 6:38845. [PMID: 27941920 PMCID: PMC5151091 DOI: 10.1038/srep38845] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2016] [Accepted: 11/15/2016] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
The study aimed at investigating the extent to which the brain adaptively exploits or compensates interaction torque (IT) during movement control in various velocity and load conditions. Participants performed arm pointing movements toward a horizontal plane without a prescribed reach endpoint at slow, neutral and rapid speeds and with/without load attached to the forearm. Experimental results indicated that IT overall contributed to net torque (NT) to assist the movement, and that such contribution increased with limb inertia and instructed speed and led to hand trajectory variations. We interpreted these results within the (inverse) optimal control framework, assuming that the empirical arm trajectories derive from the minimization of a certain, possibly composite, cost function. Results indicated that mixing kinematic, energetic and dynamic costs was necessary to replicate the participants' adaptive behavior at both kinematic and dynamic levels. Furthermore, the larger contribution of IT to NT was associated with an overall decrease of the kinematic cost contribution and an increase of its dynamic/energetic counterparts. Altogether, these results suggest that the adaptive use of IT might be tightly linked to the optimization of a composite cost which implicitly favors more the kinematic or kinetic aspects of movement depending on load and speed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Van Hoan Vu
- CIAMS, Univ. Paris-Sud., Université Paris-Saclay, Orsay, France
- CIAMS, Université d’Orléans, 45067, Orléans, France
| | | | - Bastien Berret
- CIAMS, Univ. Paris-Sud., Université Paris-Saclay, Orsay, France
- CIAMS, Université d’Orléans, 45067, Orléans, France
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Vu VH, Isableu B, Berret B. On the nature of motor planning variables during arm pointing movement: Compositeness and speed dependence. Neuroscience 2016; 328:127-46. [PMID: 27132233 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.04.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2016] [Revised: 04/15/2016] [Accepted: 04/17/2016] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to investigate the nature of the variables and rules underlying the planning of unrestrained 3D arm reaching. To identify whether the brain uses kinematic, dynamic and energetic values in an isolated manner or combines them in a flexible way, we examined the effects of speed variations upon the chosen arm trajectories during free arm movements. Within the optimal control framework, we uncovered which (possibly composite) optimality criterion underlays at best the empirical data. Fifteen participants were asked to perform free-endpoint reaching movements from a specific arm configuration at slow, normal and fast speeds. Experimental results revealed that prominent features of observed motor behaviors were significantly speed-dependent, such as the chosen reach endpoint and the final arm posture. Nevertheless, participants exhibited different arm trajectories and various degrees of speed dependence of their reaching behavior. These inter-individual differences were addressed using a numerical inverse optimal control methodology. Simulation results revealed that a weighted combination of kinematic, energetic and dynamic cost functions was required to account for all the critical features of the participants' behavior. Furthermore, no evidence for the existence of a speed-dependent tuning of these weights was found, thereby suggesting subject-specific but speed-invariant weightings of kinematic, energetic and dynamic variables during the motor planning process of free arm movements. This suggested that the inter-individual difference of arm trajectories and speed dependence was not only due to anthropometric singularities but also to critical differences in the composition of the subjective cost function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Van Hoan Vu
- CIAMS, Univ. Paris-Sud., Université Paris-Saclay, 91405 Orsay, France; CIAMS, Université d'Orléans, 45067 Orléans, France.
| | - Brice Isableu
- CIAMS, Univ. Paris-Sud., Université Paris-Saclay, 91405 Orsay, France; CIAMS, Université d'Orléans, 45067 Orléans, France
| | - Bastien Berret
- CIAMS, Univ. Paris-Sud., Université Paris-Saclay, 91405 Orsay, France; CIAMS, Université d'Orléans, 45067 Orléans, France
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ewart S, Hynes SM, Darling WG, Capaday C. A Donders' Like Law for Arm Movements: The Signal not the Noise. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:136. [PMID: 27065836 PMCID: PMC4811900 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2015] [Accepted: 03/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Experiments were done to determine whether the starting position of the arm influences its final configuration (posture) when pointing to, or grasping, targets located within the common workspace of the arm. Subjects were asked to point to, or grasp, each of six targets from five, or seven, widely spaced starting positions. We found that the variability (standard deviation) of the arm’s configuration, measured as the angle of inclination of the plane delimited by the arm and forearm, averaged about 4° for comfortable speed pointing movements and was only slightly higher for fast pointing movements. Comfortable speed reaches to grasp the targets were associated with slightly lower variability (3.5°) in final arm configuration. The average variability of repeated movements to a given target from a single start position (3.5°) was comparable to that of movements from different start positions to the same target (4.2°). A small difference in final arm inclination angle, averaged across all subjects and targets, of 3° was found between two pairs of starting positions. This small and possibly idiosyncratic effect is within the “noise” of final arm orientation variability for repeated movements (i.e., 3.5°). Thus, the variability of final posture is not for the most part due to different start positions, it is inherent to movement per se. Our results reconcile conflicting previous studies and are consistent with past works suggesting that a Donders’ like law is indeed largely upheld for unconstrained visually guided arm movements. In summary, considering movements within a typical work space, when the hand is moved voluntarily to a given spatial location the posture of the arm is nearly the same regardless of its starting position. Importantly, variability is inherent to the rule.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven Ewart
- Department of Health and Human Physiology, Motor Control Laboratories, University of Iowa Iowa City, IA, USA
| | - Stephanie M Hynes
- Department of Health and Human Physiology, Motor Control Laboratories, University of Iowa Iowa City, IA, USA
| | - Warren G Darling
- Department of Health and Human Physiology, Motor Control Laboratories, University of Iowa Iowa City, IA, USA
| | - Charles Capaday
- Institute of Neurorehabilitation Engineering, Bernstein Focus Neurotechnology Göttingen, Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Universitätsmedizin Göttingen, Georg-August-Universität Göttingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kashima T, Hori K. Control of biomimetic robots based on analysis of human arm trajectories in 3D movements. ARTIFICIAL LIFE AND ROBOTICS 2015. [DOI: 10.1007/s10015-015-0244-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
6
|
Neilson PD, Neilson MD, Bye RT. A Riemannian geometry theory of human movement: The geodesic synergy hypothesis. Hum Mov Sci 2015; 44:42-72. [PMID: 26302481 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2014] [Revised: 08/09/2015] [Accepted: 08/11/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Mass-inertia loads on muscles change with posture and with changing mechanical interactions between the body and the environment. The nervous system must anticipate changing mass-inertia loads, especially during fast multi-joint coordinated movements. Riemannian geometry provides a mathematical framework for movement planning that takes these inertial interactions into account. To demonstrate this we introduce the controlled (vs. biomechanical) degrees of freedom of the body as the coordinate system for a configuration space with movements represented as trajectories. This space is not Euclidean. It is endowed at each point with a metric equal to the mass-inertia matrix of the body in that configuration. This warps the space to become Riemannian with curvature at each point determined by the differentials of the mass-inertia at that point. This curvature takes nonlinear mass-inertia interactions into account with lengths, velocities, accelerations and directions of movement trajectories all differing from those in Euclidean space. For newcomers to Riemannian geometry we develop the intuitive groundwork for a Riemannian field theory of human movement encompassing the entire body moving in gravity and in mechanical interaction with the environment. In particular we present a geodesic synergy hypothesis concerning planning of multi-joint coordinated movements to achieve goals with minimal muscular effort.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter D Neilson
- School of Electrical Engineering and Telecommunications, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.
| | - Megan D Neilson
- School of Electrical Engineering and Telecommunications, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
| | - Robin T Bye
- Faculty of Engineering and Natural Sciences, Aalesund University College, Ålesund, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Weir MK, Wale AP. Revealing non-analytic kinematic shifts in smooth goal-directed behaviour. BIOLOGICAL CYBERNETICS 2011; 105:89-119. [PMID: 21809130 DOI: 10.1007/s00422-011-0449-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2010] [Accepted: 05/30/2011] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
How do biological agents plan and organise a smooth accurate path to shift from one smooth mode of behaviour to another as part of graceful movement that is both plastic and controlled? This paper addresses the question in conducting a novel shape analysis of approach and adjustment phases in rapid voluntary target aiming and 2-D reaching hand actions. A number of mode changing experiments are reported that investigate these actions under a range of goals and conditions. After a typically roughly aimed approach, regular projective adjustment is observed that has height and velocity kinematic profiles that are scaled copies of one another. This empirical property is encapsulated as a novel self-similar shift function. The mathematics shows that the biological shifts consist of continual deviation from their full Taylor series everywhere throughout their interval, which is a deep form of plasticity not described before. The experimental results find the same approach and adjustment strategy to occur with behavioural trajectories over the full and varied range of tested goals and conditions. The trajectory shapes have a large degree of predictability through using the shift function to handle extensive variation in the trajectories' adjustment across individual behaviours and subjects. We provide connections between the behavioural features and results and various neural studies to show how the methodology may be exploited. The conclusion is that a roughly aimed approach followed by a specific highly plastic shift adjustment can provide a regular basis for fast and accurate goal-directed motion in a simple and generalisable way.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M K Weir
- School of Computer Science, St. Andrews University, St. Andrews, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Kistemaker DA, Wong JD, Gribble PL. The central nervous system does not minimize energy cost in arm movements. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104:2985-94. [PMID: 20884757 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00483.2010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
It has been widely suggested that the many degrees of freedom of the musculoskeletal system may be exploited by the CNS to minimize energy cost. We tested this idea by having subjects making point-to-point movements while grasping a robotic manipulandum. The robot created a force field chosen such that the minimal energy hand path for reaching movements differed substantially from those observed in a null field. The results show that after extended exposure to the force field, subjects continued to move exactly as they did in the null field and thus used substantially more energy than needed. Even after practicing to move along the minimal energy path, subjects did not adapt their freely chosen hand paths to reduce energy expenditure. The results of this study indicate that for point-to-point arm movements minimization of energy cost is not a dominant factor that influences how the CNS arrives at kinematics and associated muscle activation patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dinant A Kistemaker
- University of Western Ontario, Social Science Centre, London, ON, Canada N6G 3A9.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Morasso P, Casadio M, Mohan V, Zenzeri J. A neural mechanism of synergy formation for whole body reaching. BIOLOGICAL CYBERNETICS 2010; 102:45-55. [PMID: 19937068 DOI: 10.1007/s00422-009-0349-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2009] [Accepted: 11/02/2009] [Indexed: 05/28/2023]
Abstract
The present study proposes a computational model for the formation of whole body reaching synergy, i.e., coordinated movements of lower and upper limbs, characterized by a focal component (the hand must reach a target) and a postural component (the center of mass must remain inside the support base). The model is based on an extension of the equilibrium point hypothesis that has been called Passive Motion Paradigm (PMP), modified in order to achieve terminal attractor features and allow the integration of multiple constraints. The model is a network with terminal attractor dynamics. By simulating it in various conditions it was possible to show that it exhibits many of the spatio-temporal features found in experimental data. In particular, the motion of the center of mass appears to be synchronized with the motion of the hand and with proportional amplitude. Moreover, the joint rotation patterns can be accounted for by a single functional degree of freedom, as shown by principal component analysis. It is also suggested that recent findings in motor imagery support the idea that the PMP network may represent the motor cognitive part of synergy formation, uncontaminated by the effect of execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pietro Morasso
- Department of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Italian Institute of Technology, Genoa, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Khatib O, Demircan E, De Sapio V, Sentis L, Besier T, Delp S. Robotics-based synthesis of human motion. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2009; 103:211-9. [PMID: 19665552 DOI: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2009.08.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The synthesis of human motion is a complex procedure that involves accurate reconstruction of movement sequences, modeling of musculoskeletal kinematics, dynamics and actuation, and characterization of reliable performance criteria. Many of these processes have much in common with the problems found in robotics research. Task-based methods used in robotics may be leveraged to provide novel musculoskeletal modeling methods and physiologically accurate performance predictions. In this paper, we present (i) a new method for the real-time reconstruction of human motion trajectories using direct marker tracking, (ii) a task-driven muscular effort minimization criterion and (iii) new human performance metrics for dynamic characterization of athletic skills. Dynamic motion reconstruction is achieved through the control of a simulated human model to follow the captured marker trajectories in real-time. The operational space control and real-time simulation provide human dynamics at any configuration of the performance. A new criteria of muscular effort minimization has been introduced to analyze human static postures. Extensive motion capture experiments were conducted to validate the new minimization criterion. Finally, new human performance metrics were introduced to study in details an athletic skill. These metrics include the effort expenditure and the feasible set of operational space accelerations during the performance of the skill. The dynamic characterization takes into account skeletal kinematics as well as muscle routing kinematics and force generating capacities. The developments draw upon an advanced musculoskeletal modeling platform and a task-oriented framework for the effective integration of biomechanics and robotics methods.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- O Khatib
- Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Trajectory of the index finger during grasping. Exp Brain Res 2009; 196:497-509. [PMID: 19521692 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-1878-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2008] [Accepted: 05/21/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The trajectory of the index finger during grasping movements was compared to the trajectories predicted by three optimization-based models. The three models consisted of minimizing the integral of the weighted squared joint derivatives along the path (inertia-like cost), minimizing torque change, and minimizing angular jerk. Of the three models, it was observed that the path of the fingertip and the joint trajectories, were best described by the minimum angular jerk model. This model, which does not take into account the dynamics of the finger, performed equally well when the inertia of the finger was altered by adding a 20 g weight to the medial phalange. Thus, for the finger, it appears that trajectories are planned based primarily on kinematic considerations at a joint level.
Collapse
|
12
|
Review of models for the generation of multi-joint movements in 3-D. ADVANCES IN EXPERIMENTAL MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY 2009; 629:523-50. [PMID: 19227519 DOI: 10.1007/978-0-387-77064-2_28] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
Most studies in motor control have focused on movements in two dimensions and only very few studies have systematically investigated movements in three dimensions. As a consequence, the large majority of modeling studies for motor control have tested the predictions of these models using movement data in 2D. As we will explain, movements in 3D cannot be understood from movements in 2D by adding just another dimension. The third dimension adds new and unexpected complexities. In this chapter we will explore the frames of reference, which are used in mapping sensory information about movement targets into motor commands and muscle activation patterns. Moreover, we will make a quantitative comparison between the predictions of various models in the literature with the outcome of 3D movement experiments. Quite surprisingly, none of the existing models is able to explain the data in different movement paradigms.
Collapse
|
13
|
Rosenbaum DA, Cohen RG, Dawson AM, Jax SA, Meulenbroek RG, van der Wel R, Vaughan J. The posture-based motion planning framework: new findings related to object manipulation, moving around obstacles, moving in three spatial dimensions, and haptic tracking. ADVANCES IN EXPERIMENTAL MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY 2009; 629:485-97. [PMID: 19227517 DOI: 10.1007/978-0-387-77064-2_26] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/04/2022]
Abstract
We describe the results of recent studies inspired by the posture-based motion planning theory (Rosenbaum et al., 2001). The research concerns analyses of human object manipulation, obstacle avoidance, three-dimensional movement generation, and haptic tracking, the findings of which are discussed in relation to whether they support or fail to support the premises of the theory. Each of the aforementioned topics potentially challenges the theory's claim that, in motion, goal postures are planned before the selection of movements towards those postures. However, even the quasi-continuous phenomena under study show features that comply with prospective, end-state-based motion planning. We conclude that progress in motor control should not be frustrated by the view that no model is, or will ever be, optimal. Instead, it should find promise in the steady growth of insights afforded by challenges to existing theories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David A Rosenbaum
- Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Berret B, Darlot C, Jean F, Pozzo T, Papaxanthis C, Gauthier JP. The inactivation principle: mathematical solutions minimizing the absolute work and biological implications for the planning of arm movements. PLoS Comput Biol 2008; 4:e1000194. [PMID: 18949023 PMCID: PMC2561290 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000194] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2008] [Accepted: 08/30/2008] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
An important question in the literature focusing on motor control is to determine
which laws drive biological limb movements. This question has prompted numerous
investigations analyzing arm movements in both humans and monkeys. Many theories
assume that among all possible movements the one actually performed satisfies an
optimality criterion. In the framework of optimal control theory, a first
approach is to choose a cost function and test whether the proposed model fits
with experimental data. A second approach (generally considered as the more
difficult) is to infer the cost function from behavioral data. The cost proposed
here includes a term called the absolute work of forces, reflecting the
mechanical energy expenditure. Contrary to most investigations studying
optimality principles of arm movements, this model has the particularity of
using a cost function that is not smooth. First, a mathematical theory related
to both direct and inverse optimal control approaches is presented. The first
theoretical result is the Inactivation Principle, according to which minimizing
a term similar to the absolute work implies simultaneous inactivation of
agonistic and antagonistic muscles acting on a single joint, near the time of
peak velocity. The second theoretical result is that, conversely, the presence
of non-smoothness in the cost function is a necessary condition for the
existence of such inactivation. Second, during an experimental study,
participants were asked to perform fast vertical arm movements with one, two,
and three degrees of freedom. Observed trajectories, velocity profiles, and
final postures were accurately simulated by the model. In accordance,
electromyographic signals showed brief simultaneous inactivation of opposing
muscles during movements. Thus, assuming that human movements are optimal with
respect to a certain integral cost, the minimization of an absolute-work-like
cost is supported by experimental observations. Such types of optimality
criteria may be applied to a large range of biological movements. When performing reaching and grasping movements, the brain has to choose one
trajectory among an infinite set of possibilities. Nevertheless, because human
and animal movements provide highly stereotyped features, motor strategies used
by the brain were assumed to be optimal according to certain optimality
criteria. In this study, we propose a theoretical model for motor planning of
arm movements that minimizes a compromise between the absolute work exerted by
the muscles and the integral of the squared acceleration. We demonstrate that
under these assumptions agonistic and antagonistic muscles are inactivated
during overlapping periods of time for quick enough movements. Moreover, it is
shown that only this type of criterion can predict these inactivation periods.
Finally, experimental evidence is in agreement with the predictions of the
model. Indeed, we report the existence of simultaneous inactivation of opposing
muscles during fast vertical arm movements. Therefore, this study suggests that
biological movements partly optimize the energy expenditure, integrating both
inertial and gravitational forces during the motor planning process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bastien Berret
- Université de Bourgogne, INSERM U887 Motricité-Plasticité, Dijon, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Schneider A, Cruse H, Schmitz J. Winching up heavy loads with a compliant arm: a new local joint controller. BIOLOGICAL CYBERNETICS 2008; 98:413-426. [PMID: 18414891 DOI: 10.1007/s00422-008-0230-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2006] [Accepted: 03/12/2008] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
A closed kinematic chain, like an arm that operates a crank, has a constrained movement space. A meaningful movement of the chain's endpoint is only possible along the free movement directions which are given implicitly by the contour of the object that confines the movement of the chain. Many technical solutions for such a movement task, in particular those used in robotics, use central controllers and force-torque sensors in the arm's wrist or a leg's ankle to construct a coordinate system (task frame formalism) at the local point of contact the axes of which coincide with the free and constrained movement directions. Motivated by examples from biology, we introduce a new control system that solves a constrained movement task. The control system is inspired by the control architecture that is found in stick insects like Carausius morosus. It consists of decentral joint controllers that work on elastic joints (compliant manipulator). The decentral controllers are based on local positive velocity feedback (LPVF). It has been shown earlier that LPVF enables contour following of a limb in a compliant motion task without a central controller. In this paper we extend LPVF in such a way that it is even able to follow a contour if a considerable counter force drags the limb away along the contour in a direction opposite to the desired. The controller extension is based on the measurement of the local mechanical power generated in the elastic joint and is called power-controlled relaxation LPVF. The new control approach has the following advantages. First, it still uses local joint controllers without knowledge of the kinematics. Second, it does not need a force or torque measurement at the end of the limb. In this paper we test power-controlled relaxation LPVF on a crank turning task in which a weight has to be winched up by a two-joint compliant manipulator.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Axel Schneider
- Mechatronics of Biomimetic Actuators, Faculty of Technology, University of Bielefeld, P.O. Box 10 01 31, 33501 Bielefeld, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
A computational model for redundant human three-dimensional pointing movements: integration of independent spatial and temporal motor plans simplifies movement dynamics. J Neurosci 2008; 27:13045-64. [PMID: 18045899 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.4334-06.2007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Few computational models have addressed the spatiotemporal features of unconstrained three-dimensional (3D) arm motion. Empirical observations made on hand paths, speed profiles, and arm postures during point-to-point movements led to the assumption that hand path and arm posture are independent of movement speed, suggesting that the geometric and temporal properties of movements are decoupled. In this study, we present a computational model of 3D movements for an arm with four degrees of freedom based on the assumption that optimization principles are separately applied at the geometric and temporal levels of control. Geometric properties (path and posture) are defined in terms of geodesic paths with respect to the kinetic energy metric in the Riemannian configuration space. Accordingly, a geodesic path can be generated with less muscular effort than on any other, nongeodesic path, because the sum of all configuration-speed-dependent torques vanishes. The temporal properties of the movement (speed) are determined in task space by minimizing the squared jerk along the selected end-effector path. The integration of both planning levels into a single spatiotemporal representation simplifies the control of arm dynamics along geodesic paths and results in movements with near minimal torque change and minimal peak value of kinetic energy. Thus, the application of Riemannian geometry allows for a reconciliation of computational models previously proposed for the description of arm movements. We suggest that geodesics are an emergent property of the motor system through the exploration of dynamical space. Our data validated the predictions for joint trajectories, hand paths, final postures, speed profiles, and driving torques.
Collapse
|
17
|
Hirashima M, Kudo K, Watarai K, Ohtsuki T. Control of 3D Limb Dynamics in Unconstrained Overarm Throws of Different Speeds Performed by Skilled Baseball Players. J Neurophysiol 2007; 97:680-91. [PMID: 17079349 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00348.2006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
This study investigated how the human CNS organizes complex three-dimensional (3D) ball-throwing movements that require both speed and accuracy. Skilled baseball players threw a baseball to a target at three different speeds. Kinematic analysis revealed that the fingertip speed at ball release was mainly produced by trunk leftward rotation, shoulder internal rotation, elbow extension, and wrist flexion in all speed conditions. The study participants adjusted the angular velocities of these four motions to throw the balls at three different speeds. We also analyzed the dynamics of the 3D multijoint movements using a recently developed method called “nonorthogonal torque decomposition” that can clarify how angular acceleration about a joint coordinate axis (e.g., shoulder internal rotation) is generated by the muscle, gravity, and interaction torques. We found that the study participants utilized the interaction torque to generate larger angular velocities of the shoulder internal rotation, elbow extension, and wrist flexion. To increase the interaction torque acting at these joints, the ball throwers increased muscle torque at the shoulder and trunk but not at the elbow and wrist. These results indicates that skilled ball throwers adopted a hierarchical control in which the proximal muscle torques created a dynamic foundation for the entire limb motion and beneficial interaction torques for distal joint rotations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Masaya Hirashima
- Department of Life Sciences (Sports Sciences Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, University of Tokyo, 3-8-1 Komaba, Meguro-ku, Tokyo 153-8902, Japan.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Guigon E, Baraduc P, Desmurget M. Computational motor control: redundancy and invariance. J Neurophysiol 2006; 97:331-47. [PMID: 17005621 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00290.2006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 121] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The nervous system controls the behavior of complex kinematically redundant biomechanical systems. How it computes appropriate commands to generate movements is unknown. Here we propose a model based on the assumption that the nervous system: 1) processes static (e.g., gravitational) and dynamic (e.g., inertial) forces separately; 2) calculates appropriate dynamic controls to master the dynamic forces and progress toward the goal according to principles of optimal feedback control; 3) uses the size of the dynamic commands (effort) as an optimality criterion; and 4) can specify movement duration from a given level of effort. The model was used to control kinematic chains with 2, 4, and 7 degrees of freedom [planar shoulder/elbow, three-dimensional (3D) shoulder/elbow, 3D shoulder/elbow/wrist] actuated by pairs of antagonist muscles. The muscles were modeled as second-order nonlinear filters and received the dynamics commands as inputs. Simulations showed that the model can quantitatively reproduce characteristic features of pointing and grasping movements in 3D space, i.e., trajectory, velocity profile, and final posture. Furthermore, it accounted for amplitude/duration scaling and kinematic invariance for distance and load. These results suggest that motor control could be explained in terms of a limited set of computational principles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emmanuel Guigon
- INSERM U742, Action Neuroimagerie Modelisation, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Boîte 23, 9 quai Saint-Bernard, 75005 Paris, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Liebermann DG, Biess A, Gielen CCAM, Flash T. Intrinsic joint kinematic planning. II: Hand-path predictions based on a Listing’s plane constraint. Exp Brain Res 2005; 171:155-73. [PMID: 16341525 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-005-0268-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2004] [Accepted: 09/05/2005] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
This study was aimed at examining the assumption that three-dimensional (3D) hand movements follow specific paths that are dictated by the operation of a Listing's law constraint at the intrinsic joint level of the arm. A kinematic model was used to simulate hand paths during 3D point-to-point movements. The model was based on the assumption that the shoulder obeys a 2D Listing's constraint and that rotations are about fixed single-axes. The elbow rotations were assumed to relate linearly to those of the shoulder. Both joints were assumed to rotate without reversals, and to start and end rotating simultaneously with zero initial and final velocities. Model predictions were compared to experimental observations made on four right-handed individuals that moved toward virtual objects in "extended arm", "radial", and "frontal plane" movement types. The results showed that the model was partially successful in accounting for the observed behavior. Best hand-path predictions were obtained for extended arm movements followed by radial ones. Frontal plane movements resulted in the largest discrepancies between the predicted and the observed paths. During such movements, the upper arm rotation vectors did not obey Listing's law and this may explain the observed discrepancies. For other movement types, small deviations from the predicted paths were observed which could be explained by the fact that single-axis rotations were not followed even though the rotation vectors remained within Listing's plane. Dynamic factors associated with movement execution, which were not taken into account in our purely kinematic approach, could also explain some of these small discrepancies. In conclusion, a kinematic model based on Listing's law can describe an intrinsic joint strategy for the control of arm orientation during pointing and reaching movements, but only in conditions in which the movements closely obey the Listing's plane assumption.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D G Liebermann
- Department of Physical Therapy, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel-Aviv University, 69978, Ramat Aviv, Israel.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|