1
|
Kim N, Wellwood A, Yoshida M. Processing wh-filler-gap dependencies. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2024:17470218241231872. [PMID: 38320864 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241231872] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/08/2024]
Abstract
We present experimental evidence showing that different wh-filler-gap dependencies are processed differently, depending on their syntactic licensors. Our studies compared the active storage profiles for why, how, and who (serving as subject or object of the verb). The results of offline and online experiments revealed that these wh-fillers are stored in memory for different durations, and predictably so based on the hypothesised structural distance between each wh-filler and the licensor which determines its grammatical and interpretive functions. Furthermore, the results showed that once the wh-filler is licenced, it is integrated to the current structure, and no longer engenders additional memory costs. Based on these findings, we argue that the mechanism of online sentence processing may employ both storage and integration components in memory.
Collapse
|
2
|
Yoo MH, Tollan R. Transitivity and non-uniform subjecthood in agreement attraction. Mem Cognit 2024; 52:536-553. [PMID: 38114715 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01482-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/06/2023] [Indexed: 12/21/2023]
Abstract
Research on human language converges on a view in which a grammatical "subject" is the most saliently encoded entity in mental representation. However, subjecthood is not a syntactically uniform phenomenon. Notably, many languages encode morphological distinctions between subjects of transitive verbs (i.e., verbs that require an object) and subjects of intransitive verbs. We ask how this typological pattern manifests in a language like English (which does not morphologically signal it) by examining the "distinctiveness" of transitive versus intransitive subjects in memory during online sentence processing. We conducted a self-paced reading experiment that tested for "attraction" effects (Dillon et al., Journal of Memory and Language, 69(2), 85-103, 2013; Wagers et al., Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 206-237, 2009) in the processing of subject-verb number agreement. We find that transitive subjects trigger attraction effects, but that these effects are mitigated for intransitive subject attractors (independently of the number of other noun phrases present in the intervening clause). We interpret this as indicating that transitive subjects are less distinctive and therefore less representationally salient than intransitive subjects: This is because a transitive subject must compete with another clause-mate core argument (i.e., a direct object), which draws on resources from the same pool of memory resources. On the other hand, an intransitive subject minimally only competes with a non-core argument (i.e., an oblique noun phrase); this consumes fewer memory resources, leaving the subject to enjoy greater spoils.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Myung Hye Yoo
- Department of English, Linguistics and Theatre Studies, National University of Singapore, Block AS5, 7 Arts Link, Singapore, 117570, Singapore.
| | - Rebecca Tollan
- Department of Linguistics and Cognitive Science, University of Delaware, Newark, NE, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Barbieri E, Thompson CK, Higgins J, Caplan D, Kiran S, Rapp B, Parrish T. Treatment-induced neural reorganization in aphasia is language-domain specific: Evidence from a large-scale fMRI study. Cortex 2023; 159:75-100. [PMID: 36610109 PMCID: PMC9931666 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2021] [Revised: 08/14/2022] [Accepted: 11/29/2022] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
Studies investigating the effects of language intervention on the re-organization of language networks in chronic aphasia have resulted in mixed findings, likely related to-among other factors-the language function targeted during treatment. The present study investigated the effects of the type of treatment provided on neural reorganization. Seventy individuals with chronic stroke-induced aphasia, recruited from three research laboratories and meeting criteria for agrammatism, anomia or dysgraphia were assigned to either treatment (N = 51) or control (N = 19) groups. Participants in the treatment group received 12-weeks of language intervention targeting sentence comprehension/production, naming, or spelling. At baseline and post-testing, all participants performed an fMRI story comprehension task, with blocks of auditorily-presented stories alternated with blocks of reversed speech. Participants in the treatment, but not control, group significantly improved in the treated language domain. FMRI region-of-interest (ROI) analyses, conducted within regions that were either active (or homologous to active) regions in a group of 22 healthy participants on the story comprehension task, revealed a significant increase in activation from pre-to post-treatment in right-hemisphere homologues of these regions for participants in the sentence and spelling, but not naming, treatment groups, not predicted by left-hemisphere lesion size. For the sentence (but not the spelling) treatment group, activation changes within right-hemisphere homologues of language regions were positively associated with changes in measures of verb and sentence comprehension. These findings support previous research pointing to recruitment of right hemisphere tissue as a viable route for language recovery and suggest that sentence-level treatment may promote greater neuroplasticity on naturalistic, language comprehension tasks, compared to word-level treatment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Barbieri
- Center for the Neurobiology of Language Recovery, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, School of Communication, Northwestern University, 70 Arts Circle Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA.
| | - Cynthia K Thompson
- Center for the Neurobiology of Language Recovery, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, School of Communication, Northwestern University, 70 Arts Circle Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Neurology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, 303 East Chicago Avenue, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - James Higgins
- Center for the Neurobiology of Language Recovery, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Radiology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, 737 N Michigan Avenue, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - David Caplan
- Center for the Neurobiology of Language Recovery, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 15 Parkman Street, Boston, MA 02114, USA
| | - Swathi Kiran
- Center for the Neurobiology of Language Recovery, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Speech, Language, And Hearing, College of Health & Rehabilitation, Boston University, 635 Commonwealth Avenue, Boston, MA 02215, USA
| | - Brenda Rapp
- Center for the Neurobiology of Language Recovery, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Cognitive Science, Krieger School of Arts & Sciences, Johns Hopkins Univeristy, 3400 N Charles Street, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA
| | - Todd Parrish
- Center for the Neurobiology of Language Recovery, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; Department of Radiology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, 737 N Michigan Avenue, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Lago S, Acuña Fariña C, Meseguer E. The Reading Signatures of Agreement Attraction. Open Mind (Camb) 2021; 5:132-153. [PMID: 35024528 PMCID: PMC8746120 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2021] [Accepted: 08/05/2021] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
Abstract
The comprehension of subject-verb agreement shows "attraction effects," which reveal that number computations can be derailed by nouns that are grammatically unlicensed to control agreement with a verb. However, previous results are mixed regarding whether attraction affects the processing of grammatical and ungrammatical sentences alike. In a large-sample eye-tracking replication of Lago et al. (2015), we support this "grammaticality asymmetry" by showing that the reading profiles associated with attraction depend on sentence grammaticality. In ungrammatical sentences, attraction affected both fixation durations and regressive eye-movements at the critical disagreeing verb. Meanwhile, both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences showed effects of the attractor noun number prior to the verb, in the first- and second-pass reading of the subject phrase. This contrast suggests that attraction effects in comprehension have at least two different sources: the first reflects verb-triggered processes that operate mainly in ungrammatical sentences. The second source reflects difficulties in the encoding of the subject phrase, which disturb comprehension in both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences.
Collapse
|
5
|
Delgado J, Raposo A, Santos AL. Assessing Intervention Effects in Sentence Processing: Object Relatives vs. Subject Control. Front Psychol 2021; 12:610909. [PMID: 33603700 PMCID: PMC7884622 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.610909] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2020] [Accepted: 01/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Object relative clauses are harder to process than subject relative clauses. Under Grillo’s (2009) Generalized Minimality framework, complexity effects of object relatives are construed as intervention effects, which result from an interaction between locality constraints on movement (Relativized Minimality) and the sentence processing system. Specifically, intervention of the subject DP in the movement dependency is expected to generate a minimality violation whenever processing limitations render the moved object underspecified, resulting in compromised comprehension. In the present study, assuming Generalized Minimality, we compared the processing of object relatives with the processing of subject control in ditransitives, which, like object relatives, instantiates a syntactic dependency across an intervening DP. This comparison is justified by the current debate on whether Control should be analyzed as movement: if control involves movement of the controller DP, as proposed by Hornstein (1999), a parallel between the processing of object relatives and subject control in ditransitives may be anticipated on the basis of intervention. In addition, we explored whether general cognitive factors contribute to complexity effects ascribed to movement across a DP. Sixty-nine adult speakers of European Portuguese read sentences and answered comprehension probes in a self-paced reading task with moving-window display, comprising four experimental conditions: Subject Relatives; Object Relatives; Subject Control; Object Control. Furthermore, participants performed four supplementary tasks, serving as measures of resistance to interference, lexical knowledge, working memory capacity and lexical access ability. The results from the reading task showed that whereas object relatives were harder to process than subject relatives, subject control was not harder to process than object control, arguing against recent movement accounts of control. Furthermore, we found that whereas object relative complexity effects assessed by response times to comprehension probes interacted with Reading Span, object relative complexity effects assessed by comprehension accuracy and reading times did not interact with any of the supplementary tasks. We discuss these results in light of Generalized Minimality and the hypothesis of modularity in syntactic processing (Caplan and Waters, 1999).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- João Delgado
- Research Center for Psychological Science, Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal.,Centro de Linguística da Universidade de Lisboa, Departmento de Linguística Geral e Românica, Faculdade de Letras da Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Ana Raposo
- Research Center for Psychological Science, Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Ana Lúcia Santos
- Centro de Linguística da Universidade de Lisboa, Departmento de Linguística Geral e Românica, Faculdade de Letras da Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Chow WY, Zhou Y. Eye-tracking evidence for active gap-filling regardless of dependency length. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2018; 72:1297-1307. [PMID: 30238828 DOI: 10.1177/1747021818804988] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Previous work on real-time sentence processing has established that comprehenders build and interpret filler-gap dependencies without waiting for unambiguous evidence about the actual location of the gap ("active gap-filling") as long as such dependencies are grammatically licensed. However, this generalisation was called into question by recent findings in a self-paced reading experiment by Wagers and Phillips, which may be taken to show that comprehenders do not interpret the filler at the posited gap when the dependency spans a longer distance. In the present study, we aimed to replicate these findings in an eye-tracking experiment with better controlled materials and increased statistical power. Crucially, we found clear evidence for active gap-filling across all levels of dependency length. This diverges from Wagers and Phillips's findings but is in line with the long-standing generalisation that comprehenders build and interpret filler-gap dependencies predictively as long as they are grammatically licensed. We found that the effect became smaller in the long dependency conditions in the post-critical region, which suggests the weaker effect in the long dependency conditions may have been undetected in Wagers and Phillips's study due to insufficient statistical power and/or the use of a self-paced reading paradigm.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wing-Yee Chow
- 1 Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, UK
| | - Yangzi Zhou
- 1 Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, UK.,2 Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Fyndanis V, Arcara G, Christidou P, Caplan D. Morphosyntactic Production and Verbal Working Memory: Evidence From Greek Aphasia and Healthy Aging. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2018; 61:1171-1187. [PMID: 29710332 DOI: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2017] [Accepted: 12/22/2017] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The present work investigated whether verbal working memory (WM) affects morphosyntactic production in configurations that do not involve or favor similarity-based interference and whether WM interacts with verb-related morphosyntactic categories and/or cue-target distance (locality). It also explored whether the findings related to the questions above lend support to a recent account of agrammatic morphosyntactic production: Interpretable Features' Impairment Hypothesis (Fyndanis, Varlokosta, & Tsapkini, 2012). METHOD A sentence completion task testing production of subject-verb agreement, tense/time reference, and aspect in local and nonlocal conditions and two verbal WM tasks were administered to 8 Greek-speaking persons with agrammatic aphasia (PWA) and 103 healthy participants. RESULTS The 3 morphosyntactic categories dissociated in both groups (agreement > tense > aspect). A significant interaction emerged in both groups between the 3 morphosyntactic categories and WM. There was no main effect of locality in either of the 2 groups. At the individual level, all 8 PWA exhibited dissociations between agreement, tense, and aspect, and effects of locality were contradictory. CONCLUSIONS Results suggest that individuals with WM limitations (both PWA and healthy older speakers) show dissociations between the production of verb-related morphosyntactic categories. WM affects performance shaping the pattern of morphosyntactic production (in Greek: subject-verb agreement > tense > aspect). The absence of an effect of locality suggests that executive capacities tapped by WM tasks are involved in morphosyntactic processing of demanding categories even when the cue is adjacent to the target. Results are consistent with the Interpretable Features' Impairment Hypothesis (Fyndanis et al., 2012). SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.6024428.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valantis Fyndanis
- MultiLing/Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies, University of Oslo, Norway
- Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Germany
| | | | | | - David Caplan
- Neuropsychology Laboratory, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston
| |
Collapse
|