1
|
Zhang X, Zhou P. Effects of semantic and pragmatic factors on preschool children's negation-triggered inferences on plausible alternatives. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 247:106057. [PMID: 39226857 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2024] [Revised: 08/01/2024] [Accepted: 08/05/2024] [Indexed: 09/05/2024]
Abstract
Negation-triggered inferences are universal across human languages. Hearing "This is not X" should logically lead to the inference that all elements other than X constitute possible alternatives. However, not all logically possible alternatives are equally accessible in the real world. To qualify as a plausible alternative, it must share with the negated element as many similarities as possible, and the most plausible one is often from the same taxonomic category as the negated element. The current article reports on two experiments that investigated the development of preschool children's ability to infer plausible alternatives triggered by negation. Experiment 1 showed that in a context where children were required to determine the most plausible alternative to the negated element, the 4- and 5-year-olds, but not the 3-year-olds, exhibited a robust preference for the taxonomic associates. Experiment 2 further demonstrated that the 3-, 4- and 5-year-olds considered all the complement set members as equally possible alternatives in a context where they were not explicitly required to evaluate the plausibility of different candidates. Taken together, our findings reveal interesting developmental continuity in preschool children's ability to make inferences about plausible alternatives triggered by negation. We discuss the potential semantic and pragmatic factors that contribute to children's emerging awareness of typical alternatives triggered by negative expressions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Peng Zhou
- Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310058, China.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Vanek N, Matić Škorić A, Košutar S, Matějka Š, Stone K. Looks at what isn't there: eye movements on a blank screen when processing negation in a first and a second language. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 18:1457038. [PMID: 39450318 PMCID: PMC11499152 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1457038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2024] [Accepted: 09/30/2024] [Indexed: 10/26/2024] Open
Abstract
Is negation more difficult to process than affirmation? If it is, does processing negation in a second language (L2) compound the difficulty compared to the first language (L1)? This article addresses the issues of difficulties in processing different types of negation in the L1 and L2 by looking at the differences in the ways in which comprehenders anticipate upcoming visual information during sentence processing. Using a blank-screen paradigm, we recorded eye fixations of Croatian native speakers and Croatian learners of English while they were anticipating matching or mismatching pictures to sentences with various types of negation in L1 (Croatian) and L2 (English). Using a between-group design, we manipulated sentence polarity (affirmative vs. negative), negation type (sentential vs. negative quantifier) within both L1 Croatian and L2 English so that we could observe potential anticipation effects varying as a function of the two predictors. In line with previous studies, affirmation in the L1 was easier to process than negation, and participants were able to anticipate sentence-picture matches in both the L1 and the L2 group. In contrast with our prediction, anticipatory looks did not significantly vary across negation types in Croatian based on the number of structural cues. In L2 English, learners exhibited prediction ability across negation types. These findings go against the view that comprehension in L2 comes with a reduced ability to generate expectations, and they highlight the robustness of mental simulations in both L1 and L2 negation processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Norbert Vanek
- School of Cultures, Languages and Linguistics, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Ana Matić Škorić
- Department of Speech and Language Pathology, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia
| | - Sara Košutar
- Department of Language and Culture, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
| | - Štěpán Matějka
- Institute of Czech Language and Theory of Communication, Charles University, Prague, Czechia
| | - Kate Stone
- Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Zuanazzi A, Ripollés P, Lin WM, Gwilliams L, King JR, Poeppel D. Negation mitigates rather than inverts the neural representations of adjectives. PLoS Biol 2024; 22:e3002622. [PMID: 38814982 PMCID: PMC11139306 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002622] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2023] [Accepted: 04/11/2024] [Indexed: 06/01/2024] Open
Abstract
Combinatoric linguistic operations underpin human language processes, but how meaning is composed and refined in the mind of the reader is not well understood. We address this puzzle by exploiting the ubiquitous function of negation. We track the online effects of negation ("not") and intensifiers ("really") on the representation of scalar adjectives (e.g., "good") in parametrically designed behavioral and neurophysiological (MEG) experiments. The behavioral data show that participants first interpret negated adjectives as affirmative and later modify their interpretation towards, but never exactly as, the opposite meaning. Decoding analyses of neural activity further reveal significant above chance decoding accuracy for negated adjectives within 600 ms from adjective onset, suggesting that negation does not invert the representation of adjectives (i.e., "not bad" represented as "good"); furthermore, decoding accuracy for negated adjectives is found to be significantly lower than that for affirmative adjectives. Overall, these results suggest that negation mitigates rather than inverts the neural representations of adjectives. This putative suppression mechanism of negation is supported by increased synchronization of beta-band neural activity in sensorimotor areas. The analysis of negation provides a steppingstone to understand how the human brain represents changes of meaning over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arianna Zuanazzi
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Pablo Ripollés
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Music and Audio Research Lab (MARL), New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Center for Language, Music and Emotion (ClaME), New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Wy Ming Lin
- Hector Research Institute for Education Sciences and Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Laura Gwilliams
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
| | - Jean-Rémi King
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Ecole Normale Supérieure, PSL University, Paris, France
| | - David Poeppel
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Center for Language, Music and Emotion (ClaME), New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
- Ernst Strüngmann Institute for Neuroscience, Frankfurt, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Vanek N, Matić Škorić A, Košutar S, Matějka Š, Stone K. Mental simulation of the factual and the illusory in negation processing: evidence from anticipatory eye movements on a blank screen. Sci Rep 2024; 14:2844. [PMID: 38310189 PMCID: PMC10838274 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-53353-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2023] [Accepted: 01/31/2024] [Indexed: 02/05/2024] Open
Abstract
How do comprehenders process negative statements such as The fish is not jumping out of the water? Opinions vary. Some argue for two steps, namely that processing starts off with the representation of the positive/illusory [fish jumping out of the water] and then shifts to the (f)actual. To test this idea, we measured fixations on the factual (fish not jumping) versus the illusory (fish jumping) during auditory processing of negation and affirmation. We tested speakers of English (single-cued negation) and Croatian (double-cued negation) and focused on anticipatory fixations in the absence of pictures to indicate the strength of mental simulations. Our findings contribute to negation processing research in two ways. First, dominant anticipatory fixations on the factual suggest a direct rather than a two-step process. Second, time-sensitive insights from two languages call for a finer-grained account of negation processing with negation-specific support of inferences of the factual over the illusory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Norbert Vanek
- Charles University Prague, Prague, Czechia.
- University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand.
| | | | - Sara Košutar
- UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Albu E, Dudschig C, Warren T, Kaup B. Does negation influence the choice of sentence continuations? Evidence from a four-choice cloze task. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2024; 77:90-110. [PMID: 36760063 PMCID: PMC10712206 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231158109] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2022] [Revised: 01/07/2023] [Accepted: 01/29/2023] [Indexed: 02/11/2023]
Abstract
Event plausibility facilitates the processing of affirmative sentences, but little is known about how it affects negative sentences. In six behavioural experiments, we investigated negation's impact on the choice of sentence continuations that differ with respect to event plausibility. In a four-choice cloze task, participants saw affirmative and negative sentence fragments (The child will [not] eat the . . .) in combination with four potential continuations: yoghurt (a plausible word), shellfish (a weak world knowledge violating word), branch (a severe world knowledge violating word), and minivan (a word resulting in a semantic violation). Across all experiments the plausible word was highly preferred in both affirmative and negative sentences. Experiment 2 replicated Experiment 1 while ruling out the possibility that the lack of effect of negation in Experiment 1 stemmed from participants not fully processing the negation. Experiment 3 showed that the observed plausibility effects can be generalised to other aspectual forms (The child has [not] eaten the yoghurt). Experiment 4 ruled out the possibility that the choices were mainly driven by lexical associations and additionally suggested a role for informativity. Experiment 5 replicated Experiment 4 and reinforced the general pattern according to which negative sentences express the denial of plausible positive events. Experiment 6 provided evidence that informativity might be driving patterns of choices in the negative sentences. All in all, these findings suggest that upcoming continuations are chosen to maximise the plausibility of the event in the affirmative sentences and to deny that event in the negative sentences. The observed plausibility effects do not seem to be modulated by the internal representation of events, but they can be modulated by changes to the expected informativity of the sentence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Albu
- Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Carolin Dudschig
- Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Tessa Warren
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
- Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Barbara Kaup
- Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Groß A, Singh A, Banh NC, Richter B, Scharlau I, Rohlfing KJ, Wrede B. Scaffolding the human partner by contrastive guidance in an explanatory human-robot dialogue. Front Robot AI 2023; 10:1236184. [PMID: 37965633 PMCID: PMC10642948 DOI: 10.3389/frobt.2023.1236184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2023] [Accepted: 10/11/2023] [Indexed: 11/16/2023] Open
Abstract
Explanation has been identified as an important capability for AI-based systems, but research on systematic strategies for achieving understanding in interaction with such systems is still sparse. Negation is a linguistic strategy that is often used in explanations. It creates a contrast space between the affirmed and the negated item that enriches explaining processes with additional contextual information. While negation in human speech has been shown to lead to higher processing costs and worse task performance in terms of recall or action execution when used in isolation, it can decrease processing costs when used in context. So far, it has not been considered as a guiding strategy for explanations in human-robot interaction. We conducted an empirical study to investigate the use of negation as a guiding strategy in explanatory human-robot dialogue, in which a virtual robot explains tasks and possible actions to a human explainee to solve them in terms of gestures on a touchscreen. Our results show that negation vs. affirmation 1) increases processing costs measured as reaction time and 2) increases several aspects of task performance. While there was no significant effect of negation on the number of initially correctly executed gestures, we found a significantly lower number of attempts-measured as breaks in the finger movement data before the correct gesture was carried out-when being instructed through a negation. We further found that the gestures significantly resembled the presented prototype gesture more following an instruction with a negation as opposed to an affirmation. Also, the participants rated the benefit of contrastive vs. affirmative explanations significantly higher. Repeating the instructions decreased the effects of negation, yielding similar processing costs and task performance measures for negation and affirmation after several iterations. We discuss our results with respect to possible effects of negation on linguistic processing of explanations and limitations of our study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- André Groß
- Medical Assistance Systems, Medical School OWL, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
- Center for Cognitive Interaction Technology,CITEC, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Amit Singh
- Psycholinguistics, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Ngoc Chi Banh
- Cognitive Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Birte Richter
- Medical Assistance Systems, Medical School OWL, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
- Center for Cognitive Interaction Technology,CITEC, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Ingrid Scharlau
- Cognitive Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Katharina J. Rohlfing
- Psycholinguistics, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Britta Wrede
- Medical Assistance Systems, Medical School OWL, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
- Center for Cognitive Interaction Technology,CITEC, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Fyndanis V, Miceli G, Capasso R, Killmer H, Malefaki S, Grohmann KK. Production of Sentential Negation in German and Italian Non-fluent Aphasia. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2023; 52:497-524. [PMID: 35751768 PMCID: PMC10163124 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-022-09894-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/01/2022] [Indexed: 05/07/2023]
Abstract
The ability of persons with non-fluent aphasia (PWAs) to produce sentential negation has been investigated in several languages, but only in small samples. Accounts of (morpho)syntactic impairment in PWAs have emphasized various factors, such as whether the negative marker blocks or interferes with verb movement, the position of the Negation Phrase in the syntactic hierarchy or the interpretability of negation. This study investigates the ability of German- and Italian-speaking PWAs to construct negative sentences, as well as the role of verbal working memory (WM) capacity and education in task performance and production of sentential negation. German and Italian differ in the syntactic properties of the negative markers that are relevant here (nicht and non, respectively). A sentence anagram task tapping into the construction of negative and affirmative declarative sentences was administered to 9 German- and 7 Italian-speaking PWAs, and to 14 German- and 11 Italian-speaking age- and education-matched healthy volunteers. We fitted generalized linear mixed-effects models to the datasets. There was no significant difference between negative and affirmative sentences in either group of PWAs. There was a main effect of verbal WM capacity on task performance, but no interaction between verbal WM capacity and production of negative vs. affirmative sentences. Education did not affect task performance. The results are discussed in light of different linguistically-informed accounts of (morpho)syntactic impairment in non-fluent aphasia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valantis Fyndanis
- Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Cyprus University of Technology, Limassol, Cyprus.
- MultiLing/Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.
- Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany.
| | - Gabriele Miceli
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Trento/Rovereto, Italy
| | | | - Helene Killmer
- MultiLing/Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
- Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Sonia Malefaki
- Department of Mechanical Engineering and Aeronautics, University of Patras, Rio Patras, Greece
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Qin M, Xiao D, Yuan L, Karabulatova I. Emotive Tonality of the "Youth-Old Age" Dichotomy in Russian and Chinese Media Discourses: The Stage of Psychosemantic Expertise. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2023; 52:525-553. [PMID: 35788864 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-022-09899-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/09/2022] [Indexed: 05/07/2023]
Abstract
The issue of attitude to age has the character of social stigma at different stages of human civilization, therefore, the identification of the general and particular in the assessments of the "youth-old age" dichotomy in different structural languages seems relevant. The results are based on the materials of an in-depth interview among 510 respondents belonging to different age groups: young, middle, and elderly; and 1257 respondents' responses. The in-depth interview covered 128 characteristics of interpersonal interaction. This made it possible to compile descriptor tables. The 54 characteristics identified were 27 bipolar scales. The objects of evaluation are characterized by 8 pole positions. A comparison was also made with data extracted from 368 articles in Russian and Chinese media. This work contributes to the understanding of the psycholinguistic processes of emotional perception of age in Russian and Chinese linguistic cultures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meng Qin
- Department of Foreign Languages, Faculty of Philology, Peoples' Friendship, University of Russia, Moscow, Russian Federation
| | - Dongxia Xiao
- Department of Foreign Languages, Faculty of Philology, Peoples' Friendship, University of Russia, Moscow, Russian Federation
| | - Lin Yuan
- Department of Foreign Languages, Faculty of Philology, Peoples' Friendship, University of Russia, Moscow, Russian Federation
| | - Irina Karabulatova
- Laboratory of Machine Learning and Semantic Analysis, Institute of Artificial Intelligence, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russian Federation.
- Department of Machine Learning and Digital Humanities, Moscow Institute of Physics and Technology (National University), Moscow, Russian Federation.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Pescuma VN, Serova D, Lukassek J, Sauermann A, Schäfer R, Adli A, Bildhauer F, Egg M, Hülk K, Ito A, Jannedy S, Kordoni V, Kuehnast M, Kutscher S, Lange R, Lehmann N, Liu M, Lütke B, Maquate K, Mooshammer C, Mortezapour V, Müller S, Norde M, Pankratz E, Patarroyo AG, Pleşca AM, Ronderos CR, Rotter S, Sauerland U, Schnelle G, Schulte B, Schüppenhauer G, Sell BM, Solt S, Terada M, Tsiapou D, Verhoeven E, Weirich M, Wiese H, Zaruba K, Zeige LE, Lüdeling A, Knoeferle P. Situating language register across the ages, languages, modalities, and cultural aspects: Evidence from complementary methods. Front Psychol 2023; 13:964658. [PMID: 36687875 PMCID: PMC9846624 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.964658] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2022] [Accepted: 09/27/2022] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
In the present review paper by members of the collaborative research center "Register: Language Users' Knowledge of Situational-Functional Variation" (CRC 1412), we assess the pervasiveness of register phenomena across different time periods, languages, modalities, and cultures. We define "register" as recurring variation in language use depending on the function of language and on the social situation. Informed by rich data, we aim to better understand and model the knowledge involved in situation- and function-based use of language register. In order to achieve this goal, we are using complementary methods and measures. In the review, we start by clarifying the concept of "register", by reviewing the state of the art, and by setting out our methods and modeling goals. Against this background, we discuss three key challenges, two at the methodological level and one at the theoretical level: (1) To better uncover registers in text and spoken corpora, we propose changes to established analytical approaches. (2) To tease apart between-subject variability from the linguistic variability at issue (intra-individual situation-based register variability), we use within-subject designs and the modeling of individuals' social, language, and educational background. (3) We highlight a gap in cognitive modeling, viz. modeling the mental representations of register (processing), and present our first attempts at filling this gap. We argue that the targeted use of multiple complementary methods and measures supports investigating the pervasiveness of register phenomena and yields comprehensive insights into the cross-methodological robustness of register-related language variability. These comprehensive insights in turn provide a solid foundation for associated cognitive modeling.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valentina N. Pescuma
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Dina Serova
- Institute of Archaeology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Julia Lukassek
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Antje Sauermann
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Roland Schäfer
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Aria Adli
- Institute of Romance Studies, Universität zu Köln, Cologne, Germany
| | - Felix Bildhauer
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Markus Egg
- Department of English and American Studies, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Kristina Hülk
- Institute of Archaeology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Aine Ito
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Department of English, Linguistics and Theatre Studies, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| | | | - Valia Kordoni
- Department of English and American Studies, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Milena Kuehnast
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Silvia Kutscher
- Institute of Archaeology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Robert Lange
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Nico Lehmann
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Mingya Liu
- Department of English and American Studies, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Beate Lütke
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Katja Maquate
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Christine Mooshammer
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | | | - Stefan Müller
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Muriel Norde
- Department for Northern European Studies, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Elizabeth Pankratz
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Centre for Language Evolution, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Angela G. Patarroyo
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Ana-Maria Pleşca
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Camilo R. Ronderos
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Philosophy, Classics, History of Art and Ideas, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Stephanie Rotter
- Department of English and American Studies, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | | | - Gohar Schnelle
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Britta Schulte
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Gediminas Schüppenhauer
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Leibniz-Centre General Linguistics, Berlin, Germany
| | - Bianca Maria Sell
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | | | - Megumi Terada
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Dimitra Tsiapou
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Elisabeth Verhoeven
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Melanie Weirich
- Leibniz-Centre General Linguistics, Berlin, Germany
- Institute for Germanic Linguistics, Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena, Jena, Germany
| | - Heike Wiese
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Kathy Zaruba
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Lars Erik Zeige
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Anke Lüdeling
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Pia Knoeferle
- Department of German Studies and Linguistics, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Einstein Center for Neurosciences Berlin-Charité, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|