1
|
Coppieters R, Bouzigues A, Jiskoot L, Montembeault M, Tee BL, Rohrer JD, Bruffaerts R. A systematic review of the quantitative markers of speech and language of the frontotemporal degeneration spectrum and their potential for cross-linguistic implementation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2024; 167:105909. [PMID: 39393594 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105909] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2024] [Revised: 09/12/2024] [Accepted: 09/25/2024] [Indexed: 10/13/2024]
Abstract
Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a neurodegenerative disease spectrum with an urgent need for reliable biomarkers for early diagnosis and monitoring. Speech and language changes occur in the early stages of FTD and offer a potential non-invasive, early, and accessible diagnostic tool. The use of speech and language markers in this disease spectrum is limited by the fact that most studies investigate English-speaking patients. This systematic review examines the literature on psychoacoustic and linguistic features of speech that occur across the FTD spectrum across as many different languages as possible. 76 papers were identified that investigate psychoacoustic and linguistic markers in discursive speech. 75 % of these papers studied English-speaking patients. The most generalizable features found across different languages, are speech rate, articulation rate, pause frequency, total pause duration, noun-verb ratio, and total number of nouns. While there are clear interlinguistic differences across patient groups, the results show promise for implementation of cross-linguistic markers of speech and language across the FTD spectrum particularly for psychoacoustic features.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rosie Coppieters
- Computational Neurology, Experimental Neurobiology Unit (ENU), Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Antwerp, Belgium; VIB Center for Molecular Neurology, VIB, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Arabella Bouzigues
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK; Paris Brain Institute, Sorbonne University, Paris, France
| | - Lize Jiskoot
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK; Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Maxime Montembeault
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco USA
| | - Boon Lead Tee
- Memory and Aging Center, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco USA; Global Brain Health Institute, University of California, San Francisco, USA
| | - Jonathan D Rohrer
- Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK
| | - Rose Bruffaerts
- Computational Neurology, Experimental Neurobiology Unit (ENU), Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Antwerp, Belgium; Department of Neurology, Antwerp University Hospital, Belgium; Biomedical Research Institute, Hasselt University, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Gomes V, Simón T, Lázaro M. "I don't know who you are": anomia for people's names in Alzheimer's disease. NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENT, AND COGNITION. SECTION B, AGING, NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2024; 31:956-986. [PMID: 38351719 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2024.2315773] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/21/2023] [Accepted: 01/05/2024] [Indexed: 07/18/2024]
Abstract
It is well known that difficulty in the retrieval of people's names is an early symptom of Alzheimer's Disease Dementia (ADD), but there is a controversy about the nature of this deficit. In this study, we analyzed whether the nature of the difficulty in retrieving proper names in ADD reflects pre-semantic, semantic, or post-semantic difficulties. To do so, 85 older adults, 35 with ADD and 50 cognitively healthy (CH), completed a task with famous faces involving: recognition, naming, semantic questions, and naming with phonological cues. The ADD group scored lower than the CH group in all tasks. Both groups showed a greater capacity for recognition than naming, but this difference was more pronounced in the ADD group. Additionally, the ADD group showed significantly fewer semantic errors than the CH group. Overall results suggest that the difficulties people with ADD have in naming reflect a degradation at semantic level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vanessa Gomes
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Processes and Speech Therapy, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Teresa Simón
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Processes and Speech Therapy, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Miguel Lázaro
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Cognitive Processes and Speech Therapy, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Gumus M, Koo M, Studzinski CM, Bhan A, Robin J, Black SE. Linguistic changes in neurodegenerative diseases relate to clinical symptoms. Front Neurol 2024; 15:1373341. [PMID: 38590720 PMCID: PMC10999640 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2024.1373341] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2024] [Accepted: 03/07/2024] [Indexed: 04/10/2024] Open
Abstract
Background The detection and characterization of speech changes may help in the identification and monitoring of neurodegenerative diseases. However, there is limited research validating the relationship between speech changes and clinical symptoms across a wide range of neurodegenerative diseases. Method We analyzed speech recordings from 109 patients who were diagnosed with various neurodegenerative diseases, including Alzheimer's disease, Frontotemporal Dementia, and Vascular Cognitive Impairment, in a cognitive neurology memory clinic. Speech recordings of an open-ended picture description task were processed using the Winterlight speech analysis platform which generates >500 speech features, including the acoustics of speech and linguistic properties of spoken language. We investigated the relationship between the speech features and clinical assessments including the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS), Western Aphasia Battery (WAB), and Boston Naming Task (BNT) in a heterogeneous patient population. Result Linguistic features including lexical and syntactic features were significantly correlated with clinical assessments in patients, across diagnoses. Lower MMSE and DRS scores were associated with the use of shorter words and fewer prepositional phrases. Increased impairment on WAB and BNT was correlated with the use of fewer nouns but more pronouns. Patients also differed from healthy adults as their speech duration was significantly shorter with more pauses. Conclusion Linguistic changes such as the use of simpler vocabularies and syntax were detectable in patients with different neurodegenerative diseases and correlated with cognitive decline. Speech has the potential to be a sensitive measure for detecting cognitive impairments across various neurodegenerative diseases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melisa Gumus
- Winterlight Labs, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Morgan Koo
- Hurvitz Brain Sciences Program, Sunnybrook Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
- School of Public Health Sciences, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | | | - Aparna Bhan
- Hurvitz Brain Sciences Program, Sunnybrook Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | | | - Sandra E. Black
- Hurvitz Brain Sciences Program, Sunnybrook Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Repetto C, Rodella C, Conca F, Santi GC, Catricalà E. The Italian Sensorimotor Norms: Perception and action strength measures for 959 words. Behav Res Methods 2023; 55:4035-4047. [PMID: 36307624 PMCID: PMC10700458 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-022-02004-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/06/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Neuroscience research has provided evidence that semantic information is stored in a distributed brain network involved in sensorimotor and linguistic processing. More specifically, according to the embodied cognition accounts, the representation of concepts is deemed as grounded in our bodily states. For these reasons, normative measures of words should provide relevant information about the extent to which each word embeds perceptual and action properties. In the present study, we collected ratings for 959 Italian nouns and verbs from 398 volunteers, recruited via an online platform. The words were mostly taken from the Italian adaptation of the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW). A pool of 145 verbs was added to the original set. All the words were rated on 11 sensorimotor dimensions: six perceptual modalities (vision, audition, taste, smell, touch, and interoception) and five effectors (hand-arm, foot-leg, torso, mouth, head). The new verbs were also rated on the ANEW dimensions. Results showed good reliability and consistency with previous studies. Relations between perceptual and motor dimensions are described and interpreted, along with relations between the sensorimotor and the affective dimensions. The currently developed dataset represents an important novelty, as it includes different word classes, i.e., both nouns and verbs, and integrates ratings of both sensorimotor and affective dimensions, along with other psycholinguistic parameters; all features only partially accomplished in previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia Repetto
- Deptarment of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Largo Gemelli 1, 20123, Milan, Italy.
| | - Claudia Rodella
- Deptarment of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Largo Gemelli 1, 20123, Milan, Italy.
| | | | - Gaia Chiara Santi
- ICoN Cognitive Neuroscience center, Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy
| | - Eleonora Catricalà
- IRCCS Mondino Neurological Institute, Pavia, Italy
- ICoN Cognitive Neuroscience center, Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Polito C, Conca F, Santi GC, Esposito V, Caminiti SP, Boccalini C, Berti V, Morinelli C, Mazzeo S, Marcone A, Iannaccone S, Bessi V, Sorbi S, Perani D, Cappa SF, Catricalà E. Comparing two picture naming tasks in primary progressive aphasia: Insights from behavioural and neural results. Cortex 2023; 166:1-18. [PMID: 37295234 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2023.04.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/29/2022] [Revised: 02/24/2023] [Accepted: 04/26/2023] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Picture naming tests are widely used to evaluate language impairments in neurodegenerative diseases, especially in Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA). The available tests differ for many factors affecting the performance, e.g. format of stimuli and their psycholinguistic properties. We aim to identify the most appropriate naming test to be used on PPA according to the clinical and research demands. We investigated the behavioural characteristics, i.e. proportion of correct responses and error type, and their neural correlates in two Italian naming tests, CaGi naming (CaGi) and naming subtest of the Screening for Aphasia in NeuroDegeneration battery (SAND), administered to 52 PPA patients who underwent an FDG-PET scan. We analysed the effectiveness of the tests in distinguishing PPA versus controls and among PPA variants, considering the psycholinguistic variables affecting performance. We explored the brain metabolic correlates of behavioural performance in the tests. SAND, differently from CaGi, has time limits for the response and its items are less frequent and acquired later. SAND and CaGi differed in terms of number of correct responses and error profile, suggesting a higher difficulty to name SAND items compared to CaGi. Semantic errors predominated in CaGi, while anomic and semantic errors were equally frequent in SAND. Both tests distinguished PPA from controls, but SAND outperformed CaGi in discriminating among PPA variants. FDG-PET imaging revealed a shared metabolic involvement of temporal areas associated with lexico-semantic processing, encompassing anterior fusiform, temporal pole, and extending to posterior fusiform in sv-PPA. Concluding, a picture naming test with response time limit and items which are less frequent and acquired later in life, as SAND, may be effective at highlighting subtle distinctions between PPA variants, improving the diagnosis. Conversely, a naming test without time limit for the response, as CaGi, may be useful for a better characterization of the nature of the naming impairment at the behavioural level, eliciting more naming errors than anomia, possibly helping in the development of rehabilitation protocols.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Gaia C Santi
- ICoN Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy
| | | | - Silvia P Caminiti
- Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, 20132, Milan, Italy; In Vivo Human Molecular and Structural Neuroimaging Unit, Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, 20132, Milan, Italy; Nuclear Medicine Unit, San Raffaele Hospital, 20132, Milan, Italy
| | - Cecilia Boccalini
- Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, 20132, Milan, Italy; In Vivo Human Molecular and Structural Neuroimaging Unit, Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, 20132, Milan, Italy
| | - Valentina Berti
- Nuclear Medicine, Department of Biomedical Experimental and Clinical Sciences "Mario Serio", University of Florence, Italy
| | - Carmen Morinelli
- SOD Neurologia 1, Dipartimento Neuromuscolo-Scheletrico e Degli Organi di Senso, Azienda Ospedaliera-Universitaria Careggi, Florence, Italy
| | - Salvatore Mazzeo
- IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Florence, Italy; NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - Alessandra Marcone
- Department of Rehabilitation and Functional Recovery, San Raffaele Hospital, Milan, Italy
| | - Sandro Iannaccone
- Department of Rehabilitation and Functional Recovery, San Raffaele Hospital, Milan, Italy
| | - Valentina Bessi
- IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Florence, Italy; SOD Neurologia 1, Dipartimento Neuromuscolo-Scheletrico e Degli Organi di Senso, Azienda Ospedaliera-Universitaria Careggi, Florence, Italy; NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - Sandro Sorbi
- IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Florence, Italy; SOD Neurologia 1, Dipartimento Neuromuscolo-Scheletrico e Degli Organi di Senso, Azienda Ospedaliera-Universitaria Careggi, Florence, Italy; NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - Daniela Perani
- Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, 20132, Milan, Italy; In Vivo Human Molecular and Structural Neuroimaging Unit, Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, 20132, Milan, Italy; Nuclear Medicine Unit, San Raffaele Hospital, 20132, Milan, Italy
| | - Stefano F Cappa
- IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy; ICoN Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy.
| | - Eleonora Catricalà
- ICoN Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Characterization of the logopenic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev 2022; 82:101760. [PMID: 36244629 DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2022.101760] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2022] [Accepted: 10/11/2022] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
The linguistic and anatomical variability of the logopenic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia (lv-PPA) as defined by current diagnostic criteria has been the topic of an intense debate. The present review and meta-analysis aims at characterizing the profile of lv-PPA, by a comprehensive analysis of the available literature on the neuropsychological, neuroimaging, electrophysiological, pathological, and genetic features of lv-PPA. We conducted a systematic bibliographic search, leading to the inclusion of 207 papers. Of them, 12 were used for the Anatomical Likelihood Estimation meta-analysis on grey matter revealed by magnetic resonance imaging data. The results suggest that the current guidelines outline a relatively consistent syndrome, characterized by a core set of linguistic and, to a lesser extent, non-linguistic deficits, mirroring the involvement of left temporal and parietal regions typically affected by Alzheimer Disease pathology. Variations of the lv-PPA profile are discussed in terms of heterogeneity of the neuropsychological instruments and the diagnostic criteria adopted.
Collapse
|
7
|
Nickels L, Lampe LF, Mason C, Hameau S. Investigating the influence of semantic factors on word retrieval: Reservations, results and recommendations. Cogn Neuropsychol 2022; 39:113-154. [PMID: 35972430 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2022.2109958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
There is consensus that word retrieval starts with activation of semantic representations. However, in adults without language impairment, relatively little attention has been paid to the effects of the semantic attributes of to-be-retrieved words. This paper, therefore, addresses the question of which item-inherent semantic factors influence word retrieval. Specifically, it reviews the literature on a selection of these factors: imageability, concreteness, number of semantic features, typicality, intercorrelational density, featural distinctiveness, concept distinctiveness, animacy, semantic neighbourhood density, semantic similarity, operativity, valence, and arousal. It highlights several methodological challenges in this field, and has a focus on the insights from studies with people with aphasia where the effects of these variables are more prevalent. The paper concludes that further research simultaneously examining the effects of different semantic factors that are likely to affect lexical co-activation, and the interaction of these variables, would be fruitful, as would suitably scaled computational modelling of these effects in unimpaired language processing and in language impairment. Such research would enable the refinement of theories of semantic processing and word production, and potentially have implications for diagnosis and treatment of semantic and lexical impairments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lyndsey Nickels
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Macquarie Centre for Reading, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Leonie F Lampe
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Catherine Mason
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Macquarie Centre for Reading, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Solène Hameau
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Macquarie Centre for Reading, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Isella V, Rosazza C, Ferri F, Gazzotti M, Impagnatiello V, Mapelli C, Morzenti S, Crivellaro C, Appollonio IM, Ferrarese C. Learning From Mistakes: Cognitive and Metabolic Correlates of Errors on Picture Naming in the Alzheimer’s Disease Spectrum. J Alzheimers Dis 2022; 87:1033-1053. [DOI: 10.3233/jad-220053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
Abstract
Background: Analysis of subtypes of picture naming errors produced by patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have seldom been investigated yet may clarify the cognitive and neural underpinnings of naming in the AD spectrum. Objective: To elucidate the neurocognitive bases of picture naming in AD through a qualitative analysis of errors. Methods: Over 1000 naming errors produced by 70 patients with amnestic, visuospatial, linguistic, or frontal AD were correlated with general cognitive tests and with distribution of hypometabolism on FDG-PET. Results: Principal component analysis identified 1) a Visual processing factor clustering visuospatial tests and unrecognized stimuli, pure visual errors and visual-semantic errors, associated with right parieto-occipital hypometabolism; 2) a Concept-Lemma factor grouping language tests and anomias, circumlocutions, superordinates, and coordinates, correlated with left basal temporal hypometabolism; 3) a Lemma-Phonology factor including the digit span and phonological errors, linked with left temporo-parietal hypometabolism. Regression of brain metabolism on individual errors showed that errors due to impairment of basic and higher-order processing of object visual attributes or of their interaction with semantics, were related with bilateral occipital and left occipito-temporal dysfunction. Omissions and superordinates were linked to degradation of broad and basic concepts in the left basal temporal cortex. Semantic-lexical errors derived from faulty semantically- and phonologically-driven lexical retrieval in the left superior and middle temporal gyri. Generation of nonwords was underpinned by of phonological impairment within the left inferior parietal cortex. Conclusion: Analysis of individual naming errors allowed to outline a comprehensive anatomo-functional model of picture naming in classical and atypical AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valeria Isella
- Department of Neurology, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
- NeuroMI, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| | - Cristina Rosazza
- Dipartimento di Studi Umanistici (DISTUM), Università degli Studi di Urbino Carlo Bo, Urbino, Italy
- Neuroradiology Unit, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milan, Italy
| | - Francesca Ferri
- Department of Neurology, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
- NeuroMI, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| | - Maria Gazzotti
- Department of Neurology, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| | | | - Cristina Mapelli
- Department of Neurology, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
- NeuroMI, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| | - Sabrina Morzenti
- Medical Physics, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, Italy
- NeuroMI, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| | - Cinzia Crivellaro
- Nuclear Medicine, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, Italy
- NeuroMI, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| | - Ildebrando M. Appollonio
- Department of Neurology, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
- NeuroMI, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| | - Carlo Ferrarese
- Department of Neurology, S. Gerardo Hospital, Monza, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
- NeuroMI, University of Milano - Bicocca, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
In search of different categories of abstract concepts: a fMRI adaptation study. Sci Rep 2021; 11:22587. [PMID: 34799624 PMCID: PMC8604982 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-02013-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2021] [Accepted: 10/26/2021] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Concrete conceptual knowledge is supported by a distributed neural network representing different semantic features according to the neuroanatomy of sensory and motor systems. If and how this framework applies to abstract knowledge is currently debated. Here we investigated the specific brain correlates of different abstract categories. After a systematic a priori selection of brain regions involved in semantic cognition, i.e. responsible of, respectively, semantic representations and cognitive control, we used a fMRI-adaptation paradigm with a passive reading task, in order to modulate the neural response to abstract (emotions, cognitions, attitudes, human actions) and concrete (biological entities, artefacts) categories. Different portions of the left anterior temporal lobe responded selectively to abstract and concrete concepts. Emotions and attitudes adapted the left middle temporal gyrus, whereas concrete items adapted the left fusiform gyrus. Our results suggest that, similarly to concrete concepts, some categories of abstract knowledge have specific brain correlates corresponding to the prevalent semantic dimensions involved in their representation.
Collapse
|
10
|
Catricalà E, Conca F, Borsa VM, Cotelli M, Manenti R, Gobbi E, Binetti G, Cotta Ramusino M, Perini G, Costa A, Rusconi ML, Cappa SF. Different types of abstract concepts: evidence from two neurodegenerative patients. Neurocase 2021; 27:270-280. [PMID: 34058940 DOI: 10.1080/13554794.2021.1931345] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
The observation of neurological patients showing selective impairments for specific conceptual categories contributed in the development of semantic memory theories. Here, we studied two patients (P01, P02), affected, respectively, by the semantic variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia (sv-PPA) and Cortico-Basal Syndrome (CBS). An implicit lexical decision task, including concrete (animals, tools) and abstract (emotions, social, quantity) concepts, was administered to patients and healthy controls.P01 and P02 showed an abolished priming effect for social and quantity-related concepts, respectively. This double dissociation suggests a role of different brain areas in representing specific abstract categories, giving insights for current semantic memory theories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Catricalà
- Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy
| | - F Conca
- Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy.,IRCCS Fondazione Mondino, Pavia, Italy
| | - V M Borsa
- Department of Human and Social Sciences, Università Degli Studi Di Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
| | - M Cotelli
- Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni Di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy
| | - R Manenti
- Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni Di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy
| | - E Gobbi
- Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni Di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy
| | - G Binetti
- MAC Memory Clinic and Molecular Markers Laboratory, IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni Di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy
| | - M Cotta Ramusino
- IRCCS Fondazione Mondino, Pavia, Italy.,Department of Brain and Behavior, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
| | - G Perini
- IRCCS Fondazione Mondino, Pavia, Italy.,Department of Brain and Behavior, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
| | - A Costa
- IRCCS Fondazione Mondino, Pavia, Italy.,Department of Brain and Behavior, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
| | - M L Rusconi
- Department of Human and Social Sciences, Università Degli Studi Di Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
| | - S F Cappa
- Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy.,IRCCS Fondazione Mondino, Pavia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Conca F, Borsa VM, Cappa SF, Catricalà E. The multidimensionality of abstract concepts: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 127:474-491. [PMID: 33979574 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2020] [Revised: 05/01/2021] [Accepted: 05/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The neuroscientific study of conceptual representation has largely focused on categories of concrete entities (biological entities, tools…), while abstract knowledge has been less extensively investigated. The possible presence of a categorical organization of abstract knowledge is a debated issue. An embodied cognition framework predicts an organization of the abstract domain into different dimensions, grounded in the brain regions engaged by the corresponding experience. Here we review the types of experience that have been proposed to characterize different categories of abstract concepts, and the evidence supporting a corresponding organization derived from behavioural, neuroimaging (i.e., fMRI, MRI, PET, SPECT), EEG, and neurostimulation (i.e., TMS) studies in healthy and clinical populations. The available data provide substantial converging evidence in favour of the presence of distinct neural representations of social and emotional knowledge, mental states and magnitude concepts, engaging brain systems involved in the corresponding experiences. This evidence is supporting an extension of embodied models of semantic memory organization to several types of abstract knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- F Conca
- Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy; IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino, Pavia, Italy
| | - V M Borsa
- Università degli Studi di Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
| | - S F Cappa
- Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy; IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino, Pavia, Italy.
| | - E Catricalà
- Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Semantic memory impairment in dementia: A cross-cultural adaptation study. Neurol Sci 2021; 43:265-273. [PMID: 33966130 DOI: 10.1007/s10072-021-05272-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2020] [Accepted: 04/17/2021] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Semantic memory deficits are frequently encountered in dementia and distinct patterns of semantic impairment characterize the subtypes of dementia. Life course and cultural experiences significantly influence semantic memory. Hence, there is a need to assess semantic memory using culturally appropriate tests, to aid accurate diagnosis of dementia and facilitate cross-cultural collaborative research. AIMS In this prospective study, we adapted and validated the Cambridge Semantic Memory (CSM) test battery to the Indian cultural context and studied the patterns of semantic memory impairment across dementia subtypes. METHODS The CSM battery was modified using standard methods and by incorporating culturally appropriate changes and new semantic categories relevant to India. The adapted Indian Semantic Memory (ISM) test battery was administered to a cohort of 121 subjects, consisting of controls and dementia: Alzheimer's disease (AD), progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA), semantic dementia (SD), and behavioral variant fronto-temporal dementia (BvFTD). Profile of semantic memory performance across groups was examined. RESULTS The ISM battery was found to be a valid measure of semantic memory. The novel semantic categories of gods/religious icons, vegetables, and food items added value to the diagnostic process. Distinct semantic memory profiles in SD, PNFA, AD, and BvFTD were demonstrated. CONCLUSIONS The cultural adaptation of a semantic memory battery for the Indian context provided sensitive evidence of semantic memory impairment in dementia and its subtypes. The clinical and research application of the ISM battery will enhance diagnostic evaluation that can aid in early and accurate identification of deficits and devising intervention strategies and enable research across cultures.
Collapse
|
13
|
Lampe LF, Hameau S, Fieder N, Nickels L. Effects of semantic variables on word production in aphasia. Cortex 2021; 141:363-402. [PMID: 34130047 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.02.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2020] [Revised: 10/29/2020] [Accepted: 02/09/2021] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Words differ in the complexity of their semantic representations and their relationships to other words and these differences can be operationalised as a variety of semantic variables. The research presented here investigates how word production in aphasia is influenced by six feature-based semantic variables (number of near semantic neighbours, semantic similarity, number of semantic features, typicality, intercorrelational density, and distinctiveness). Previous research has reported inconsistent findings for some of the semantic variables, while others have not been previously studied in aphasia. Spoken picture naming data from a large group of individuals with aphasia with mixed spoken word production impairments (n = 175) and a sub-group who produced few phonological errors (n = 60) was analysed. We examined effects of the semantic variables on overall naming accuracy and on the occurrence of different error types (semantic errors overall, coordinate errors, omissions), while controlling for other psycholinguistic variables using generalised linear mixed effects models and Bayesian correlations. Across analyses, number of semantic features was the most important predictor with a facilitatory main effect on naming accuracy in the sub-group analysis. Number of semantic features, along with typicality and semantic similarity, also predicted error types and in some analyses these effects depended on the integrity of semantic processing. Effects of the semantic variables and their theoretical explanations and implications are discussed in light of previous research and models of word production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leonie F Lampe
- Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, NSW, Australia; International Doctorate for Experimental Approaches to Language and Brain (IDEALAB) University of Groningen, the Netherlands; University of Potsdam, Germany; University of Newcastle, UK; Macquarie University, Australia.
| | - Solène Hameau
- Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, NSW, Australia; Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, NSW, Australia
| | - Nora Fieder
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany
| | - Lyndsey Nickels
- Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Linguistic features and automatic classifiers for identifying mild cognitive impairment and dementia. COMPUT SPEECH LANG 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.csl.2020.101113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
15
|
Ferré P, Jarret J, Brambati S, Bellec P, Joanette Y. Functional Connectivity of Successful Picture-Naming: Age-Specific Organization and the Effect of Engaging in Stimulating Activities. Front Aging Neurosci 2020; 12:535770. [PMID: 33250759 PMCID: PMC7674930 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.535770] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2020] [Accepted: 10/05/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Aging is a lifelong process that starts at birth. Throughout the course of their life, individuals are exposed to various levels of stimulating activities. A higher level of engagement in such activities is suspected to protect against the normal course of cognitive aging or the cognitive manifestations of age-related brain diseases. However, the exact mechanism underlying such protective action remains unclear. The concept of the neurocognitive reserve was introduced to refer to the hypothesis that engagement in stimulating activities shapes brain structure and function, thus indirectly allowing for better preserved cognitive abilities. Although it is known that word production is among the best-preserved cognitive abilities in aging, the underlying neurofunctional mechanisms that allow this relative preservation are still unknown, and it is still unclear how engagement in stimulating activities affects these processes. The objective of this study is to describe the brain functional connectivity patterns associated with picture-naming abilities in younger and older adults with varying levels of engagement in stimulating activities, as a proxy for neurocognitive reserve. A mediation analysis was applied to determine whether the association between reserve proxies and naming accuracy is dependent on task FC. Results show that naming accuracy depends on the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) functional decoupling in both younger and older adults but through different pathways. While high-performing older adults rely on the asynchronization of this area from motor speech regions’ activity, the best-performing younger adults rely on the functional decoupling with language-related regions. Mediation analysis reveals that the PCC decoupling mediates the relationship between the level of engagement in stimulating activities and naming accuracy in younger adults, but not in older adults. These findings suggest that reserve-related mechanisms may be more critical for naming in early adult life, while older adults’ neurofunctional organization may benefit more from a lifetime of acquired knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Perrine Ferré
- Centre de Recherche de l’institut de Gériatrie de l’Université de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, QC, Canada
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
- *Correspondence: Perrine Ferré
| | - Julien Jarret
- Centre de Recherche de l’institut de Gériatrie de l’Université de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, QC, Canada
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Simona Brambati
- Centre de Recherche de l’institut de Gériatrie de l’Université de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, QC, Canada
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Pierre Bellec
- Centre de Recherche de l’institut de Gériatrie de l’Université de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, QC, Canada
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Yves Joanette
- Centre de Recherche de l’institut de Gériatrie de l’Université de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, QC, Canada
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Catricalà E, Polito C, Presotto L, Esposito V, Sala A, Conca F, Gasparri C, Berti V, Filippi M, Pupi A, Sorbi S, Iannaccone S, Magnani G, Cappa SF, Perani D. Neural correlates of naming errors across different neurodegenerative diseases: An FDG-PET study. Neurology 2020; 95:e2816-e2830. [PMID: 33004608 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000010967] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2020] [Accepted: 07/23/2020] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To investigate the types of errors produced in a picture naming task by patients with neurodegenerative dementia due to different etiologies and their neural correlates. METHODS The same standardized picture naming test was administered to a consecutive sample of patients (n = 148) who had been studied with [18F] FDG-PET. The errors were analyzed in 3 categories (visual, semantic, and phonologic). The PET data were analyzed using an optimized single-subject procedure, and the statistical parametric mapping multiple regression design was used to explore the correlation between each type of error and brain hypometabolism in the whole group. Metabolic connectivity analyses were run at the group level on 7 left hemisphere cortical areas corresponding to an a priori defined naming network. RESULTS Semantic errors were predominant in most patients, independent of clinical diagnosis. In the whole group analysis, visual errors correlated with hypometabolism in the right inferior occipital lobe and in the left middle occipital lobe. Semantic errors correlated with hypometabolism in the left fusiform gyrus, the inferior and middle temporal gyri, and the temporal pole. Phonologic errors were associated with hypometabolism in the left superior and middle temporal gyri. Both positive (occipital-posterior fusiform) and negative (anterior fusiform gyrus and the superior anterior temporal lobe) connectivity changes were associated with semantic errors. CONCLUSIONS Naming errors reflect the dysfunction of separate stages of the naming process and are specific markers for different patterns of brain involvement. These correlations are not limited to primary progressive aphasia but extend to other neurodegenerative dementias.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eleonora Catricalà
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Cristina Polito
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Luca Presotto
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Valentina Esposito
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Arianna Sala
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Francesca Conca
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Celeste Gasparri
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Valentina Berti
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Massimo Filippi
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Alberto Pupi
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Sandro Sorbi
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Sandro Iannaccone
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Giuseppe Magnani
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| | - Stefano F Cappa
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy.
| | - Daniela Perani
- From the Faculty of Psychology (E.C., F.C., C.G., S.F.C.), Institute for Advanced Studies, IUSS, Pavia; Nuclear Medicine Unit (C.P., V.B., A.P.), Department of Experimental and Clinical Biomedical Sciences, and NEUROFARBA, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (S.S.), University of Florence; Nuclear Medicine Unit (L.P., A.S., D.P.), IRCCS San Raffaele Hospital, Milan; Faculty of Psychology (V.E., A.S., D.P.), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan; Department of Neurology and INSPE (M.F., G.M.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan; Clinical Neuroscience Department (S.I.), San Raffaele Turro Hospital, Milan; IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi (S.S.), Florence, and IRCCS Fondazione Istituto Neurologico Casimiro Mondino (S.F.C.), Pavia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Vivas L, Montefinese M, Bolognesi M, Vivas J. Core features: measures and characterization for different languages. Cogn Process 2020; 21:651-667. [PMID: 32333125 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-020-00969-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2019] [Accepted: 03/17/2020] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
According to the feature-based view of semantic representation, concepts can be represented as distributed networks of semantic features, which contribute with different weights to determine the overall meaning of a concept. The study of semantic features, typically collected in property generation tasks, is enriched with measures indicating the informativeness and distinctiveness of a given feature for the related concepts. However, while these measures have been provided in several languages (e.g. Italian, Spanish and English), they have hardly been applied comparatively across languages. The purpose of this paper is to investigate language-related differences and similarities emerging from the semantic representation of aggregated core features. Features with higher salience for a set of concrete concepts are identified and described in terms of their feature type. Then, comparisons are made between domains (natural vs. artefacts) and languages (Italian, Spanish and English) and descriptive statistics are provided. These results show that the characterization of concrete concepts is overall fairly stable across languages, although interesting cross-linguistic differences emerged. We will discuss the implications of our findings in relation to the theoretical paradigm of semantic feature norms, as well as in relation to speakers' mutual understanding in multilingual settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leticia Vivas
- UNMDP, CONICET, Psychology Faculty, Basic and Applied Psychology and Technology Institute (IPSIBAT), Funes 3250, CP 7600, Mar del Plata, Argentina.
| | - Maria Montefinese
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 12, 35131, Padua, Italy.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London, WC1H 0AP, UK
| | - Marianna Bolognesi
- Faculty of Languages, Literatures and Modern Cultures, University of Bologna, Via Cartoleira 5, Bologna, Italy
| | - Jorge Vivas
- UNMDP, CONICET, Psychology Faculty, Basic and Applied Psychology and Technology Institute (IPSIBAT), Funes 3250, CP 7600, Mar del Plata, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Catricalà E, Conca F, Fertonani A, Miniussi C, Cappa SF. State-dependent TMS reveals the differential contribution of ATL and IPS to the representation of abstract concepts related to social and quantity knowledge. Cortex 2020; 123:30-41. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.09.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2019] [Revised: 06/17/2019] [Accepted: 09/15/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
19
|
Buchanan EM, De Deyne S, Montefinese M. A practical primer on processing semantic property norm data. Cogn Process 2019; 21:587-599. [PMID: 31768704 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-019-00939-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2019] [Accepted: 10/24/2019] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Semantic property listing tasks require participants to generate short propositions (e.g., [Formula: see text], [Formula: see text]) for a specific concept (e.g., DOG). This task is the cornerstone of the creation of semantic property norms which are essential for modeling, stimuli creation, and understanding similarity between concepts. Despite the wide applicability of semantic property norms for a large variety of concepts across different groups of people, the methodological aspects of the property listing task have received less attention, even though the procedure and processing of the data can substantially affect the nature and quality of the measures derived from them. The goal of this paper is to provide a practical primer on how to collect and process semantic property norms. We will discuss the key methods to elicit semantic properties and compare different methods to derive meaningful representations from them. This will cover the role of instructions and test context, property preprocessing (e.g., lemmatization), property weighting, and relationship encoding using ontologies. With these choices in mind, we propose and demonstrate a processing pipeline that transparently documents these steps, resulting in improved comparability across different studies. The impact of these choices will be demonstrated using intrinsic (e.g., reliability, number of properties) and extrinsic measures (e.g., categorization, semantic similarity, lexical processing). This practical primer will offer potential solutions to several long-standing problems and allow researchers to develop new property listing norms overcoming the constraints of previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin M Buchanan
- Harrisburg University of Science and Technology, 326 Market St., Harrisburg, PA, 17101, USA.
| | | | - Maria Montefinese
- University of Padova, Padua, Italy.,University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Pineault J, Jolicœur P, Grimault S, Lacombe J, Brambati SM, Bier N, Chayer C, Joubert S. A MEG study of the neural substrates of semantic processing in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Neurocase 2019; 25:118-129. [PMID: 31256711 DOI: 10.1080/13554794.2019.1631853] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Despite a well-documented pattern of semantic impairment, the patterns of brain activation during semantic processing in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia (svPPA) still remain poorly understood. In the current study, one svPPA patient (EC) and six elderly controls carried out a general-level semantic categorization task while their brain activity was recorded using magnetoencephalography (MEG). Despite similar behavioral performance, EC showed hyperactivation of the left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) and right anterior temporal lobe (ATL) relative to controls. This suggests that periatrophic regions within the ATL region may support preserved semantic abilities in svPPA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica Pineault
- a Département de psychologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,b Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| | - Pierre Jolicœur
- a Département de psychologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,c Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| | - Stephan Grimault
- a Département de psychologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,c Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| | - Jacinthe Lacombe
- a Département de psychologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,b Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| | - Simona Maria Brambati
- a Département de psychologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,b Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| | - Nathalie Bier
- b Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,d Faculté de médecine , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| | - Céline Chayer
- d Faculté de médecine , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,e Service de neurologie , Hôpital Maisonneuve-Rosemont , Montréal , Canada
| | - Sven Joubert
- a Département de psychologie , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada.,b Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal , Université de Montréal , Montréal , Canada
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Normative ratings for perceptual and motor attributes of 750 object concepts in Spanish. Behav Res Methods 2019; 50:1632-1644. [PMID: 29052168 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-017-0970-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
Subjective ratings of perceptual and motor attributes were obtained for a set of 750 concrete concepts in Spanish by requiring scale-based judgments from a sample of university students (N = 539). Following on the work of Amsel, Urbach, and Kutas (2012), the seven attributes were color, motion, sound, smell, taste, graspability, and pain. Normative data based on the obtained ratings are provided as a tool for future investigations. Additionally, the relationships of these attributes to other lexical dimensions (e.g., familiarity, frequency, concreteness) and the factorial organization of concepts around the main components were analyzed. The pattern of results is consistent with prior findings that highlight the relevance of dimensions related to survival as being crucially involved in conceptual processing.
Collapse
|
22
|
Ursino M, Cuppini C, Cappa SF, Catricalà E. A feature-based neurocomputational model of semantic memory. Cogn Neurodyn 2018; 12:525-547. [PMID: 30483362 PMCID: PMC6233327 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-018-9494-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2018] [Revised: 06/19/2018] [Accepted: 06/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
According with a featural organization of semantic memory, this work is aimed at investigating, through an attractor network, the role of different kinds of features in the representation of concepts, both in normal and neurodegenerative conditions. We implemented new synaptic learning rules in order to take into account the role of partially shared features and of distinctive features with different saliency. The model includes semantic and lexical layers, coding, respectively for object features and word-forms. Connections among nodes are strongly asymmetrical. To account for the feature saliency, asymmetrical synapses were created using Hebbian rules of potentiation and depotentiation, setting different pre-synaptic and post-synaptic thresholds. A variable post-synaptic threshold, which automatically changed to reflect the feature frequency in different concepts (i.e., how many concepts share a feature), was used to account for partially shared features. The trained network solved naming tasks and word recognition tasks very well, exploiting the different role of salient versus marginal features in concept identification. In the case of damage, superordinate concepts were preserved better than the subordinate ones. Interestingly, the degradation of salient features, but not of marginal ones, prevented object identification. The model suggests that Hebbian rules, with adjustable post-synaptic thresholds, can provide a reliable semantic representation of objects exploiting the statistics of input features.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mauro Ursino
- Department of Electrical, Electronic and Information Engineering, University of Bologna, Viale Risorgimento 2, 40136 Bologna, Italy
| | - Cristiano Cuppini
- Department of Electrical, Electronic and Information Engineering, University of Bologna, Viale Risorgimento 2, 40136 Bologna, Italy
| | - Stefano F. Cappa
- NEtS Center, Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS, Pavia, Italy
- IRCCS S. Giovanni di Dio, Brescia, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Distinctive semantic features in the healthy adult brain. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2018; 19:296-308. [PMID: 30426310 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-018-00668-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The role of semantic features, which are distinctive (e.g., a zebra's stripes) or shared (e.g. has four legs) for accessing a concept, has been studied in detail in early neurodegenerative disease such as semantic dementia (SD). However, potential neural underpinnings of such processing have not been studied in healthy adults. The current study examines neural activation patterns using fMRI while participants completed a feature verification task, in which they identified shared or distinctive semantic features for a set of natural kinds and man-made artifacts. The results showed that the anterior temporal lobe bilaterally is an important area for processing distinctive features, and that this effect is stronger within natural kinds than man-made artifacts. These findings provide converging evidence from healthy adults that is consistent with SD research, and support a model of semantic memory in which patterns of specificity of semantic information can partially explain differences in neural activation between categories.
Collapse
|
24
|
Catricalà E, Gobbi E, Battista P, Miozzo A, Polito C, Boschi V, Esposito V, Cuoco S, Barone P, Sorbi S, Cappa SF, Garrard P. SAND: a Screening for Aphasia in NeuroDegeneration. Development and normative data. Neurol Sci 2017; 38:1469-1483. [PMID: 28578483 DOI: 10.1007/s10072-017-3001-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2017] [Accepted: 05/17/2017] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
Language assessment has a critical role in the clinical diagnosis of neurodegenerative diseases, in particular, in the case of Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA). The current diagnostic criteria (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011) identify three main variants on the basis of clinical features and patterns of brain atrophy. Widely accepted tools to diagnose, clinically classify, and follow up the heterogeneous language profiles of PPA are still lacking. In this study, we develop a screening battery, composed of nine tests (picture naming, word and sentence comprehension, word and sentence repetition, reading, semantic association, writing and picture description), following the recommendations of current diagnostic guidelines and taking into account recent research on the topic. All tasks were developed with consideration of the psycholinguistic factors that can affect performance, with the aim of achieving sensitivity to the language deficit to which each task was relevant, and to allow identification of the selective characteristic impairments of each PPA variant. Normative data on 134 Italian subjects pooled across homogeneous subgroups for age, sex, and education are reported. Although further work is still needed, this battery represents a first step towards a concise multilingual standard language examination, a fast and simple tool to help clinicians and researchers in the diagnosis of PPA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Elena Gobbi
- Università Vita-Salute San Raffaele, Milano, Italia
| | - Petronilla Battista
- NEtS, Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS-Pavia, Pavia, Italia.,Unità di Malattie Neurodegenerative, Dipartimento di medicina di base, neuroscienze e organi del senso, Università di Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italia.,NEUROFARBA- Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Psicologia, Area del Farmaco e Salute del Bambino, Università di Firenze, Firenze, Italia
| | - Antonio Miozzo
- Unità di Neurologia, Dipartimento di scienze cliniche e sperimentali, Università di Brescia, Brescia, Italia
| | - Cristina Polito
- SBSC - Dipartimento di Scienze Biomediche Sperimentali e Cliniche, Università di Firenze, Firenze, Italia
| | - Veronica Boschi
- NEtS, Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS-Pavia, Pavia, Italia
| | | | - Sofia Cuoco
- Facoltà di Medicina e Chirurgia, Università degli Studi di Salerno, Fisciano, Italia
| | - Paolo Barone
- Facoltà di Medicina e Chirurgia, Università degli Studi di Salerno, Fisciano, Italia
| | - Sandro Sorbi
- NEUROFARBA- Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Psicologia, Area del Farmaco e Salute del Bambino, Università di Firenze, Firenze, Italia
| | - Stefano F Cappa
- NEtS, Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS-Pavia, Pavia, Italia.,IRCCS S. Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italia
| | - Peter Garrard
- Neuroscience Research Centre, St George's-University of London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Boschi V, Catricalà E, Consonni M, Chesi C, Moro A, Cappa SF. Connected Speech in Neurodegenerative Language Disorders: A Review. Front Psychol 2017; 8:269. [PMID: 28321196 PMCID: PMC5337522 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00269] [Citation(s) in RCA: 148] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2016] [Accepted: 02/10/2017] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Language assessment has a crucial role in the clinical diagnosis of several neurodegenerative diseases. The analysis of extended speech production is a precious source of information encompassing the phonetic, phonological, lexico-semantic, morpho-syntactic, and pragmatic levels of language organization. The knowledge about the distinctive linguistic variables identifying language deficits associated to different neurodegenerative diseases has progressively improved in the last years. However, the heterogeneity of such variables and of the way they are measured and classified limits any generalization and makes the comparison among studies difficult. Here we present an exhaustive review of the studies focusing on the linguistic variables derived from the analysis of connected speech samples, with the aim of characterizing the language disorders of the most prevalent neurodegenerative diseases, including primary progressive aphasia, Alzheimer's disease, movement disorders, and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. A total of 61 studies have been included, considering only those reporting group analysis and comparisons with a group of healthy persons. This review first analyzes the differences in the tasks used to elicit connected speech, namely picture description, story narration, and interview, considering the possible different contributions to the assessment of different linguistic domains. This is followed by an analysis of the terminologies and of the methods of measurements of the variables, indicating the need for harmonization and standardization. The final section reviews the linguistic domains affected by each different neurodegenerative disease, indicating the variables most consistently impaired at each level and suggesting the key variables helping in the differential diagnosis among diseases. While a large amount of valuable information is already available, the review highlights the need of further work, including the development of automated methods, to take advantage of the richness of connected speech analysis for both research and clinical purposes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Veronica Boschi
- NETS, Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax, Institute for Advanced Study-Pavia Pavia, Italy
| | - Eleonora Catricalà
- NETS, Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax, Institute for Advanced Study-Pavia Pavia, Italy
| | - Monica Consonni
- Third Neurology Unit and Motor Neuron Diseases Center, IRCCS Foundation "Carlo Besta" Neurological Institute Milan, Italy
| | - Cristiano Chesi
- NETS, Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax, Institute for Advanced Study-Pavia Pavia, Italy
| | - Andrea Moro
- NETS, Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax, Institute for Advanced Study-Pavia Pavia, Italy
| | - Stefano F Cappa
- NETS, Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax, Institute for Advanced Study-PaviaPavia, Italy; IRCCS S. Giovanni di Dio FatebenefratelliBrescia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|