1
|
Dopkins S. Is separation represented in terms of position? Acta Psychol (Amst) 2025; 253:104725. [PMID: 39826325 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2025.104725] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2024] [Revised: 01/10/2025] [Accepted: 01/13/2025] [Indexed: 01/22/2025] Open
Abstract
How do humans make judgments of separation (distance in a frontal plane)? According to the additive account of separation assessment, the separation between two points is inferred from the number of instances of a unit distance lying between the points. According to the subtractive account, the separation between two points is inferred from the difference between their positions in a localization system. In response to recent findings that are consistent with the additive account and inconsistent with the subtractive account, the present study explicitly tested the subtractive account. The study tested whether separations are represented in memory in terms of positions, as would be expected under the subtractive account. In two experiments the study found no support for this possibility. Although participants showed a bias to mis-recall the members of a set of separations in the direction of the average separation in the set, participants did not show a parallel bias in recalling the positions that defined the separations. Although participants were influenced in recalling the members of a set of separations by the range of the scale that was used to specify the separations, participants were not similarly influenced in recalling the positions that defined the separations.
Collapse
|
2
|
Yousif SR, McDougle SD. Oblique warping: A general distortion of spatial perception. Cognition 2024; 247:105762. [PMID: 38552560 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105762] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2023] [Revised: 12/17/2023] [Accepted: 02/25/2024] [Indexed: 04/24/2024]
Abstract
There are many putatively distinct phenomena related to perception in the oblique regions of space. For instance, the classic oblique effect describes a deficit in visual acuity for oriented lines in the obliques, and classic "prototype effects" reflect a bias to misplace objects towards the oblique regions of space. Yet these effects are explained in very different terms: The oblique effect itself is often understood as arising from orientation-selective neurons, whereas prototype effects are described as arising from categorical biases. Here, we explore the possibility that these effects (and others) may stem from a single underlying spatial distortion. We show that there is a general distortion of (angular) space in the oblique regions that influences not only orientation judgments, but also location, extent, and size. We argue that these findings reflect oblique warping, a general distortion of spatial representations in the oblique regions which may be the root cause of many oblique effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sami R Yousif
- University of Pennsylvania, Department of Psychology, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ghiani A, Mann D, Brenner E. Methods matter: Exploring how expectations influence common actions. iScience 2024; 27:109076. [PMID: 38361615 PMCID: PMC10867666 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.109076] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2023] [Revised: 12/21/2023] [Accepted: 01/26/2024] [Indexed: 02/17/2024] Open
Abstract
Behavior in controlled laboratory studies is not always representative of what people do in daily life. This has prompted a recent shift toward conducting studies in natural settings. We wondered whether expectations raised by how the task is presented should also be considered. To find out, we studied gaze when walking down and up a staircase. Gaze was often directed at steps before stepping on them, but most participants did not look at every step. Importantly, participants fixated more steps and looked around less when asked to navigate the staircase than when navigating the same staircase but asked to walk outside. Presumably, expecting the staircase to be important made participants direct their gaze at more steps, despite the identical requirements when on the staircase. This illustrates that behavior can be influenced by expectations, such as expectations resulting from task instructions, even when studies are conducted in natural settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Ghiani
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Sciences and Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - David Mann
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Sciences and Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Eli Brenner
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Sciences and Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Buckley MG, Myles LA, Easton A, McGregor A. The spatial layout of doorways and environmental boundaries shape the content of event memories. Cognition 2022; 225:105091. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105091] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2021] [Revised: 03/03/2022] [Accepted: 03/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
5
|
Yousif SR. Redundancy and Reducibility in the Formats of Spatial Representations. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2022; 17:1778-1793. [PMID: 35867333 DOI: 10.1177/17456916221077115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Mental representations are the essence of cognition. Yet to understand how the mind works, one must understand not just the content of mental representations (i.e., what information is stored) but also the format of those representations (i.e., how that information is stored). But what does it mean for representations to be formatted? How many formats are there? Is it possible that the mind represents some pieces of information in multiple formats at once? To address these questions, I discuss a "case study" of representational format: the representation of spatial location. I review work (a) across species and across development, (b) across spatial scales, and (c) across levels of analysis (e.g., high-level cognitive format vs. low-level neural format). Along the way, I discuss the possibility that the same information may be organized in multiple formats simultaneously (e.g., that locations may be represented in both Cartesian and polar coordinates). Ultimately, I argue that seemingly "redundant" formats may support the flexible spatial behavior observed in humans and that researchers should approach the study of all mental representations with this possibility in mind.
Collapse
|
6
|
Abstract
A primary function of human vision is to encode and recall spatial information about visual scenes. We developed an experimental paradigm that reveals the structure of human spatial memory priors in unprecedented detail. We ran a series of 85 large-scale online experiments with 9,202 participants that paint an intricate picture of these priors. Our results suggest a way to understand visuospatial representations as reflecting the efficient allocation of coding resources. In a radical departure from traditional theory, we introduce a model that reinterprets spatial memory priors as reflecting an optimal allocation of perceptual resources. We validate the predictions of the model experimentally by showing that perceptual biases are correlated with variations in discrimination accuracy. An essential function of the human visual system is to locate objects in space and navigate the environment. Due to limited resources, the visual system achieves this by combining imperfect sensory information with a belief state about locations in a scene, resulting in systematic distortions and biases. These biases can be captured by a Bayesian model in which internal beliefs are expressed in a prior probability distribution over locations in a scene. We introduce a paradigm that enables us to measure these priors by iterating a simple memory task where the response of one participant becomes the stimulus for the next. This approach reveals an unprecedented richness and level of detail in these priors, suggesting a different way to think about biases in spatial memory. A prior distribution on locations in a visual scene can reflect the selective allocation of coding resources to different visual regions during encoding (“efficient encoding”). This selective allocation predicts that locations in the scene will be encoded with variable precision, in contrast to previous work that has assumed fixed encoding precision regardless of location. We demonstrate that perceptual biases covary with variations in discrimination accuracy, a finding that is aligned with simulations of our efficient encoding model but not the traditional fixed encoding view. This work demonstrates the promise of using nonparametric data-driven approaches that combine crowdsourcing with the careful curation of information transmission within social networks to reveal the hidden structure of shared visual representations.
Collapse
|
7
|
Holden MP, Hampson E. Endogenous variation in estradiol in women affects the weighting of metric and categorical information in spatial location memory. Horm Behav 2021; 128:104909. [PMID: 33279507 DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104909] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2020] [Revised: 11/25/2020] [Accepted: 11/27/2020] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Recent work has suggested that sex differences may exist in the strategies or types of cues that are utilized by men and women to remember discrete spatial locations or routes through a visual environment. The current study investigated the effects of circulating estradiol levels in women on the relative weighting of categorical versus fine-grained 'metric' information in a test of short-term memory for spatial locations, either presented within a simple geometric surround (a circular enclosure) or within more visually complex landscape scenes. Patterns of displacement error in the point location estimates made by men and women were analyzed. Results confirmed a sex difference in the weighting of metric versus categorical cues. Relative to men, women's estimates of locations were more strongly biased toward the center of the surrounding category (i.e., toward the category 'prototype'). Furthermore, objective measures of estradiol via saliva collected at the time of memory testing showed that, among naturally-cycling women, estradiol concentrations correlated in a positive, graded, fashion with the degree of emphasis that women placed on categorical information when estimating point locations. No associations were found for progesterone. These findings are consistent with a wider body of research showing that biological sex and reproductive hormone levels, including 17β-estradiol, can subtly influence performance on certain spatial tasks. This is the first study to show that circulating estradiol levels may influence the relative emphasis placed on categorical versus metric cues when remembering simple point locations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark P Holden
- Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6G 3V1, Canada.
| | - Elizabeth Hampson
- Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6G 3V1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Azañón E, Tucciarelli R, Siromahov M, Amoruso E, Longo MR. Mapping visual spatial prototypes: Multiple reference frames shape visual memory. Cognition 2020; 198:104199. [PMID: 32014716 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2019] [Revised: 01/14/2020] [Accepted: 01/19/2020] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Categories provide a fundamental source of information used to structure our perception of the world. For example, when people reproduce the remembered location of a dot in a circle, they implicitly impose vertical and horizontal axes onto the circle, and responses are biased towards the center of each of the resulting quadrants. Such results reveal the existence of spatial prototypes, which function as Bayesian priors and which are integrated with actual memory traces. Spatial prototypes have been extensively investigated and described in previous studies, but it remains unclear what type of information is used to create spatial categories. We developed a new approach that allowed to 'image' patterns of spatial bias in detail, and map the internal representational structure of objects and space. Previous studies, using circular shapes suggested that boundaries are established based on a viewer-based frame of reference, therefore using cues extrinsic to the object. Given that a circle has radial symmetry, the axes imposed cannot come from the shape itself. Here we investigated if the same applies for shapes with clearly-defined symmetry axes and thus intrinsic frames of reference. Using rotated shapes (squares and rectangles), where extrinsic and intrinsic cues are dissociated, we observed flexible usage of multiple reference frames. Furthermore, in certain contexts, participants relied mostly on cues intrinsic to the shape itself. These results show that humans divide visual space as a function of multiple reference frames, in a flexible, and context dependent manner.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Azañón
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, WC1E 7HX London, United Kingdom; Institute of Psychology, Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, 39106 Magdeburg, Germany; Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences, 39106 Magdeburg, Germany; Department of Behavioral Neurology, Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, 39118 Magdeburg, Germany.
| | - Raffaele Tucciarelli
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, WC1E 7HX London, United Kingdom
| | - Metodi Siromahov
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, WC1E 7HX London, United Kingdom; Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, TW20 Egham, United Kingdom
| | - Elena Amoruso
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, WC1E 7HX London, United Kingdom
| | - Matthew R Longo
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, WC1E 7HX London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Zax A, Williams K, Patalano AL, Slusser E, Cordes S, Barth H. What Do Biased Estimates Tell Us about Cognitive Processing? Spatial Judgments as Proportion Estimation. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1653297] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Emily Slusser
- Wesleyan University, USA
- San Jose State University, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Abstract
ABSTRACT
As babies rapidly acquire motor skills that give them increasingly independent and wide-ranging access to the environment over the first two years of human life, they decrease their reliance on habit systems for spatial localization, switching to their emerging inertial navigation system and to allocentric frameworks. Initial place learning is evident towards the end of the period. From 3 to 10 years, children calibrate their ability to encode various sources of spatial information (inertial information, geometric cues, beacons, proximal landmarks and distal landmarks) and begin to combine cues, both within and across systems. Geometric cues are important, but do not constitute an innate and encapsulated module. In addition, from 3 to 10 years, children build the capacity to think about frames of reference different from their current one (i.e. to perform perspective taking). By around 12 years, we see adult-level performance and adult patterns of individual differences on cognitive mapping tasks requiring the integration of vista views of space into environmental space. These lines of development are continuous rather than stage-like. Spatial development builds on important beginnings in the neural systems of newborns, but changes in experience-expectant ways with motor development, action in the world and success–failure feedback. Human systems for integrating and manipulating spatial information also benefit from symbolic capacities and technological inventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nora S. Newcombe
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Working memory and spatial judgments: Cognitive load increases the central tendency bias. Psychon Bull Rev 2017; 23:1825-1831. [PMID: 27084778 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-016-1039-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Previous work demonstrates that memory for simple stimuli can be biased by information about the distribution of which the stimulus is a member. Specifically, people underestimate values greater than the distribution's average and overestimate values smaller than the average. This is referred to as the central tendency bias. This bias has been explained as an optimal use of both noisy sensory information and category information. In largely separate literature, cognitive load (CL) experiments attempt to manipulate the available working memory of participants in order to observe the effect on choice or judgments. In two experiments, we demonstrate that participants under high cognitive load exhibit a stronger central tendency bias than when under a low cognitive load. Although not anticipated at the outset, we also find that judgments exhibit an anchoring bias not described previously.
Collapse
|
12
|
Spatial category bias in a three-dimensional virtual environment. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
13
|
Regier T, Xu Y. The Sapir‐Whorf hypothesis and inference under uncertainty. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2017; 8. [DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/19/2016] [Revised: 01/09/2017] [Accepted: 01/17/2017] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Terry Regier
- Department of Linguistics, Cognitive Science ProgramUniversity of CaliforniaBerkeleyCAUSA
| | - Yang Xu
- Department of Linguistics, Cognitive Science ProgramUniversity of CaliforniaBerkeleyCAUSA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis and Probabilistic Inference: Evidence from the Domain of Color. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0158725. [PMID: 27434643 PMCID: PMC4951127 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158725] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2015] [Accepted: 06/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis holds that our thoughts are shaped by our native language, and that speakers of different languages therefore think differently. This hypothesis is controversial in part because it appears to deny the possibility of a universal groundwork for human cognition, and in part because some findings taken to support it have not reliably replicated. We argue that considering this hypothesis through the lens of probabilistic inference has the potential to resolve both issues, at least with respect to certain prominent findings in the domain of color cognition. We explore a probabilistic model that is grounded in a presumed universal perceptual color space and in language-specific categories over that space. The model predicts that categories will most clearly affect color memory when perceptual information is uncertain. In line with earlier studies, we show that this model accounts for language-consistent biases in color reconstruction from memory in English speakers, modulated by uncertainty. We also show, to our knowledge for the first time, that such a model accounts for influential existing data on cross-language differences in color discrimination from memory, both within and across categories. We suggest that these ideas may help to clarify the debate over the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis.
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Das Konzept der psychometrischen Intelligenz ist in der Öffentlichkeit sowie auch in Teilen der wissenschaftlichen Psychologie umstritten. Warum Intelligenz so große Abwehrreaktionen auslöst und wie die Begründungen für diese zum Stand der Intelligenzforschung stehen, wird in diesem Artikel behandelt. Dabei behandeln wir drei weit verbreitete Vorurteile: 1) Das Definitionsproblem: Es gibt viele Intelligenzen, und Psychologen können sich sowieso nicht auf eine einheitliche Definition einigen; 2) Die Abseitigkeitsannahme: Die Leistung in Intelligenztests hat nichts mit Kompetenzen im wahren Leben zu tun; 3) Die Ursachenverwirrung: Wenn Intelligenz ein in den Genen verankertes Merkmal ist, bleiben Umwelteinflüsse unwirksam. Auf Basis der aktuellen Intelligenzforschung entkräften wir alle drei Einwände: 1) Intelligenz wird heute vom Großteil der Intelligenzforscher präzise und einheitlich definiert; das Fehlen einer einheitlichen Definition ist kein aktuelles Problem der Intelligenzforschung mehr. 2) Eine Reihe von jüngeren Meta-Analysen hat überzeugend die hohen und stabilen Validitäten von Intelligenztests demonstriert: Intelligenz ist einer der besten Prädiktoren von Lern- und Berufserfolg. 3) Die moderne Verhaltensgenetik hat verdeutlicht, dass Intelligenz in den Genen verankert ist, diese aber nur in einer geistig anregenden Umwelt ihr Potenzial entfalten können. Mit diesem Artikel möchten wir Psychologen Argumente an die Hand geben, mit denen sie eine skeptische Öffentlichkeit von der Wichtigkeit der psychometrischen Intelligenzforschung überzeugen können.
Collapse
|
16
|
Newcombe NS, Levine SC, Mix KS. Thinking about quantity: the intertwined development of spatial and numerical cognition. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2015; 6:491-505. [PMID: 26415916 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1369] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2014] [Revised: 05/19/2015] [Accepted: 08/04/2015] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
There are many continuous quantitative dimensions in the physical world. Philosophical, psychological, and neural work has focused mostly on space and number. However, there are other important continuous dimensions (e.g., time and mass). Moreover, space can be broken down into more specific dimensions (e.g., length, area, and density) and number can be conceptualized discretely or continuously (i.e., natural vs real numbers). Variation on these quantitative dimensions is typically correlated, e.g., larger objects often weigh more than smaller ones. Number is a distinctive continuous dimension because the natural numbers (i.e., positive integers) are used to quantify collections of discrete objects. This aspect of number is emphasized by teaching of the count word sequence and arithmetic during the early school years. We review research on spatial and numerical estimation, and argue that a generalized magnitude system is the starting point for development in both domains. Development occurs along several lines: (1) changes in capacity, durability, and precision, (2) differentiation of the generalized magnitude system into separable dimensions, (3) formation of a discrete number system, i.e., the positive integers, (4) mapping the positive integers onto the continuous number line, and (5) acquiring abstract knowledge of the relations between pairs of systems. We discuss implications of this approach for teaching various topics in mathematics, including scaling, measurement, proportional reasoning, and fractions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nora S Newcombe
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Susan C Levine
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Kelly S Mix
- Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education, Michigan State University, Lansing, MI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Holden MP, Newcombe NS, Resnick I, Shipley TF. Seeing Like a Geologist: Bayesian Use of Expert Categories in Location Memory. Cogn Sci 2015; 40:440-54. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12229] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2013] [Revised: 03/09/2014] [Accepted: 05/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Mark P. Holden
- Department of Psychology Temple University
- Department of Psychology University of Western Ontario
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Barth H, Lesser E, Taggart J, Slusser E. Spatial estimation: a non-Bayesian alternative. Dev Sci 2014; 18:853-62. [PMID: 25440776 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2014] [Accepted: 09/10/2014] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
A large collection of estimation phenomena (e.g. biases arising when adults or children estimate remembered locations of objects in bounded spaces; Huttenlocher, Newcombe & Sandberg, 1994) are commonly explained in terms of complex Bayesian models. We provide evidence that some of these phenomena may be modeled instead by a simpler non-Bayesian alternative. Undergraduates and 9- to 10-year-olds completed a speeded linear position estimation task. Bias in both groups' estimates could be explained in terms of a simple psychophysical model of proportion estimation. Moreover, some individual data were not compatible with the requirements of the more complex Bayesian model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilary Barth
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, USA
| | - Ellen Lesser
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, USA
| | | | - Emily Slusser
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, USA.,Department of Child and Adolescent Development, San Jose State University, USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Baron IS, Hopp C, Weiss BA. Developmental normative data for the Baron-Hopkins Board test of spatial location memory. Child Neuropsychol 2014; 21:732-50. [DOI: 10.1080/09297049.2014.957264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
20
|
Holden MP, Hampson E. Categorical Bias in Line Angle Judgments: Sex Differences and the Use of Multiple Categories. SPATIAL COGNITION AND COMPUTATION 2014. [DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2014.915844] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
21
|
Holden MP, Duff-Canning SJ, Hampson E. Sex differences in the weighting of metric and categorical information in spatial location memory. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2014; 79:1-18. [PMID: 24435543 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-013-0539-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2013] [Accepted: 12/30/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
According to the Category Adjustment model, remembering a spatial location involves the Bayesian combination of fine-grained and categorical information about that location, with each cue weighted by its relative certainty. However, individuals may differ in terms of their certainty about each cue, resulting in estimates that rely more or less on metric or categorical representations. To date, though, very little research has examined individual differences in the relative weighting of these cues in spatial location memory. Here, we address this gap in the literature. Participants were asked to recall point locations in uniform geometric shapes and in photographs of complex, natural scenes. Error patterns were analyzed for evidence of a sex difference in the relative use of metric and categorical information. As predicted, women placed relatively more emphasis on categorical cues, while men relied more heavily on metric information. Location reproduction tasks showed a similar effect, implying that the sex difference arises early in spatial processing, possibly during encoding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark P Holden
- Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, N6A 5C2, Canada,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|