1
|
Sawyer H, Bannard C, Pine J. Gradually increasing context-sensitivity shapes the development of children's verb marking: A corpus study. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13543. [PMID: 38961809 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2023] [Revised: 05/22/2024] [Accepted: 06/16/2024] [Indexed: 07/05/2024]
Abstract
There is substantial evidence that children's apparent omission of grammatical morphemes in utterances such as "She play tennis" and "Mummy eating" is in fact errors of commission in which contextually licensed unmarked forms encountered in the input are reproduced in a context-blind fashion. So how do children stop making such errors? In this study, we test the assumption that children's ability to recover from error is related to their developing sensitivity to longer-range dependencies. We use a pre-registered corpus analysis to explore the predictive value of different cues with regards to children's verb-marking errors and observe a developmental pattern consistent with this account. We look at context-independent cues (the identity of the specific verb being used) and at the relative value of context-dependent cues (the identity of the specific subject+verb sequence being used). We find that the only consistent effect across a group of 2- to 3-year-olds and a group of 3- to 4-year-olds is the relative frequency of unmarked forms of specific subject+verb sequences being used. The relative frequency of unmarked forms of the verb alone is predictive only in the younger age group. This is consistent with an account in which children recover from making errors by becoming progressively more sensitive to context, at first the immediately preceding lexical contexts (e.g., the subject that precedes the verb) and eventually more distant grammatical markers (e.g., the fronted auxiliary that precedes the subject in questions). RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We provide a corpus analysis investigating input effects on young children's verb-marking errors (e.g., Mummy go) across development (between 2 and 4 years of age). We find evidence that these apparent errors of omission are in fact input-driven errors of commission that persist into the third year of life. We compare the relative effect on error rates of context-independent (e.g., verb) and context-dependent (e.g., subject+verb sequence) cues across developmental time. Our findings support the proposal that children recover from making verb-marking errors by becoming progressively more sensitive to preceding context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hannah Sawyer
- Department of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
| | - Colin Bannard
- Department of Linguistics and English Language, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Julian Pine
- Department of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Endress AD. Hebbian learning can explain rhythmic neural entrainment to statistical regularities. Dev Sci 2024:e13487. [PMID: 38372153 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13487] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2023] [Revised: 12/26/2023] [Accepted: 01/29/2024] [Indexed: 02/20/2024]
Abstract
In many domains, learners extract recurring units from continuous sequences. For example, in unknown languages, fluent speech is perceived as a continuous signal. Learners need to extract the underlying words from this continuous signal and then memorize them. One prominent candidate mechanism is statistical learning, whereby learners track how predictive syllables (or other items) are of one another. Syllables within the same word predict each other better than syllables straddling word boundaries. But does statistical learning lead to memories of the underlying words-or just to pairwise associations among syllables? Electrophysiological results provide the strongest evidence for the memory view. Electrophysiological responses can be time-locked to statistical word boundaries (e.g., N400s) and show rhythmic activity with a periodicity of word durations. Here, I reproduce such results with a simple Hebbian network. When exposed to statistically structured syllable sequences (and when the underlying words are not excessively long), the network activation is rhythmic with the periodicity of a word duration and activation maxima on word-final syllables. This is because word-final syllables receive more excitation from earlier syllables with which they are associated than less predictable syllables that occur earlier in words. The network is also sensitive to information whose electrophysiological correlates were used to support the encoding of ordinal positions within words. Hebbian learning can thus explain rhythmic neural activity in statistical learning tasks without any memory representations of words. Learners might thus need to rely on cues beyond statistical associations to learn the words of their native language. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Statistical learning may be utilized to identify recurring units in continuous sequences (e.g., words in fluent speech) but may not generate explicit memory for words. Exposure to statistically structured sequences leads to rhythmic activity with a period of the duration of the underlying units (e.g., words). I show that a memory-less Hebbian network model can reproduce this rhythmic neural activity as well as putative encodings of ordinal positions observed in earlier research. Direct tests are needed to establish whether statistical learning leads to declarative memories for words.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ansgar D Endress
- Department of Psychology, City, University of London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Monaghan P, Donnelly S, Alcock K, Bidgood A, Cain K, Durrant S, Frost RLA, Jago LS, Peter MS, Pine JM, Turnbull H, Rowland CF. Learning to generalise but not segment an artificial language at 17 months predicts children's language skills 3 years later. Cogn Psychol 2023; 147:101607. [PMID: 37804784 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101607] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2023] [Revised: 08/30/2023] [Accepted: 09/18/2023] [Indexed: 10/09/2023]
Abstract
We investigated whether learning an artificial language at 17 months was predictive of children's natural language vocabulary and grammar skills at 54 months. Children at 17 months listened to an artificial language containing non-adjacent dependencies, and were then tested on their learning to segment and to generalise the structure of the language. At 54 months, children were then tested on a range of standardised natural language tasks that assessed receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar. A structural equation model demonstrated that learning the artificial language generalisation at 17 months predicted language abilities - a composite of vocabulary and grammar skills - at 54 months, whereas artificial language segmentation at 17 months did not predict language abilities at this age. Artificial language learning tasks - especially those that probe grammar learning - provide a valuable tool for uncovering the mechanisms driving children's early language development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Lana S Jago
- Lancaster University, UK; Liverpool John Moores University, UK
| | | | | | | | - Caroline F Rowland
- Liverpool University, UK; Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, the Netherlands; Radboud University, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Zhu H, Clark A. Distributional Lattices as a Model for Discovering Syntactic Categories in Child-Directed Speech. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2022; 51:917-931. [PMID: 35348946 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-022-09872-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/10/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
Distribution information plays an important role in word categorization. In this paper, we present a novel distributional model, distributional lattices to discover syntactic categories in child directed speech. A distributional lattice is a hierarchy formed by closed sets of words that are distributionally similar. Such a hierarchy is potentially useful for capturing syntactic categories by clustering words with associate patterns they occur in. In order to empirically support the suggestion that the distributional lattice is effective at categorizing words, we present a distributional lattice analysis of the Brent corpus of child-directed speech. The results show that distributional lattices are able to yield extremely accurate syntactic categories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Haiting Zhu
- School of Foreign Studies, Minzu University of China, No. 27 Zhongguancun South Avenue, Beijing, 100081, People's Republic of China.
| | - Alexander Clark
- Department of Philosophy, King's College London, Strand, London, WC2R 2LS, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Cognitive mechanisms of statistical learning and segmentation of continuous sensory input. Mem Cognit 2021; 50:979-996. [PMID: 34964955 PMCID: PMC9209387 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-021-01264-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Two classes of cognitive mechanisms have been proposed to explain segmentation of continuous sensory input into discrete recurrent constituents: clustering and boundary-finding mechanisms. Clustering mechanisms are based on identifying frequently co-occurring elements and merging them together as parts that form a single constituent. Bracketing (or boundary-finding) mechanisms work by identifying rarely co-occurring elements that correspond to the boundaries between discrete constituents. In a series of behavioral experiments, I tested which mechanisms are at play in the visual modality both during segmentation of a continuous syllabic sequence into discrete word-like constituents and during recognition of segmented constituents. Additionally, I explored conscious awareness of the products of statistical learning—whole constituents versus merged clusters of smaller subunits. My results suggest that both online segmentation and offline recognition of extracted constituents rely on detecting frequently co-occurring elements, a process likely based on associative memory. However, people are more aware of having learnt whole tokens than of recurrent composite clusters.
Collapse
|
6
|
Marimon M, Hofmann A, Veríssimo J, Männel C, Friederici AD, Höhle B, Wartenburger I. Children's Learning of Non-adjacent Dependencies Using a Web-Based Computer Game Setting. Front Psychol 2021; 12:734877. [PMID: 34803816 PMCID: PMC8595475 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.734877] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2021] [Accepted: 10/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Infants show impressive speech decoding abilities and detect acoustic regularities that highlight the syntactic relations of a language, often coded via non-adjacent dependencies (NADs, e.g., is singing). It has been claimed that infants learn NADs implicitly and associatively through passive listening and that there is a shift from effortless associative learning to a more controlled learning of NADs after the age of 2 years, potentially driven by the maturation of the prefrontal cortex. To investigate if older children are able to learn NADs, Lammertink et al. (2019) recently developed a word-monitoring serial reaction time (SRT) task and could show that 6-11-year-old children learned the NADs, as their reaction times (RTs) increased then they were presented with violated NADs. In the current study we adapted their experimental paradigm and tested NAD learning in a younger group of 52 children between the age of 4-8 years in a remote, web-based, game-like setting (whack-a-mole). Children were exposed to Italian phrases containing NADs and had to monitor the occurrence of a target syllable, which was the second element of the NAD. After exposure, children did a "Stem Completion" task in which they were presented with the first element of the NAD and had to choose the second element of the NAD to complete the stimuli. Our findings show that, despite large variability in the data, children aged 4-8 years are sensitive to NADs; they show the expected differences in r RTs in the SRT task and could transfer the NAD-rule in the Stem Completion task. We discuss these results with respect to the development of NAD dependency learning in childhood and the practical impact and limitations of collecting these data in a web-based setting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mireia Marimon
- Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Andrea Hofmann
- Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
- Early Childhood Education Research, University of Applied Sciences, Potsdam, Germany
| | - João Veríssimo
- Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
- School of Arts and Humanities, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Claudia Männel
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- Department of Audiology and Phoniatrics, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Angela D Friederici
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Barbara Höhle
- Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Isabell Wartenburger
- Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Brusini P, Seminck O, Amsili P, Christophe A. The Acquisition of Noun and Verb Categories by Bootstrapping From a Few Known Words: A Computational Model. Front Psychol 2021; 12:661479. [PMID: 34489784 PMCID: PMC8416756 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.661479] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2021] [Accepted: 07/14/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
While many studies have shown that toddlers are able to detect syntactic regularities in speech, the learning mechanism allowing them to do this is still largely unclear. In this article, we use computational modeling to assess the plausibility of a context-based learning mechanism for the acquisition of nouns and verbs. We hypothesize that infants can assign basic semantic features, such as “is-an-object” and/or “is-an-action,” to the very first words they learn, then use these words, the semantic seed, to ground proto-categories of nouns and verbs. The contexts in which these words occur, would then be exploited to bootstrap the noun and verb categories: unknown words are attributed to the class that has been observed most frequently in the corresponding context. To test our hypothesis, we designed a series of computational experiments which used French corpora of child-directed speech and different sizes of semantic seed. We partitioned these corpora in training and test sets: the model extracted the two-word contexts of the seed from the training sets, then used them to predict the syntactic category of content words from the test sets. This very simple algorithm demonstrated to be highly efficient in a categorization task: even the smallest semantic seed (only 8 nouns and 1 verb known) yields a very high precision (~90% of new nouns; ~80% of new verbs). Recall, in contrast, was low for small seeds, and increased with the seed size. Interestingly, we observed that the contexts used most often by the model featured function words, which is in line with what we know about infants' language development. Crucially, for the learning method we evaluated here, all initialization hypotheses are plausible and fit the developmental literature (semantic seed and ability to analyse contexts). While this experiment cannot prove that this learning mechanism is indeed used by infants, it demonstrates the feasibility of a realistic learning hypothesis, by using an algorithm that relies on very little computational and memory resources. Altogether, this supports the idea that a probabilistic, context-based mechanism can be very efficient for the acquisition of syntactic categories in infants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Perrine Brusini
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom.,Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, École Normale Supérieure/PSL University, Paris, France
| | - Olga Seminck
- Laboratoire Langues, Textes, Traitements Informatiques, Cognition (Lattice), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, École Normale Supérieure/PSL University, Université Sorbonne Nouvelle, Paris, France
| | - Pascal Amsili
- Laboratoire Langues, Textes, Traitements Informatiques, Cognition (Lattice), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, École Normale Supérieure/PSL University, Université Sorbonne Nouvelle, Paris, France
| | - Anne Christophe
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, École Normale Supérieure/PSL University, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Toro JM, Crespo-Bojorque P. Arc-shaped pitch contours facilitate item recognition in non-human animals. Cognition 2021; 213:104614. [PMID: 33558018 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104614] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2020] [Revised: 01/11/2021] [Accepted: 01/26/2021] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Acoustic changes linked to natural prosody are a key source of information about the organization of language. Both human infants and adults readily take advantage of such changes to discover and memorize linguistic patterns. Do they so because our brain is efficiently wired to specifically process linguistic stimuli? Or are we co-opting for language acquisition purposes more general principles that might be inherited from our animal ancestors? Here, we address this question by exploring if other species profit from prosody to better process acoustic sequences. More specifically, we test whether arc-shaped pitch contours defining natural prosody might facilitate item recognition and memorization in rats. In two experiments, we presented to the rats nonsense words with flat, natural, inverted and random prosodic contours. We observed that the animals correctly recognized the familiarization words only when arc-shaped pitch contours were implemented over them. Our results suggest that other species might also benefit from prosody for the memorization of items in a sequence. Such capacity seems to be rooted in general principles of how biological sounds are produced and processed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan M Toro
- Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats (ICREA), Pg. Lluis Companys, 23, 08019 Barcelona, Spain; Universitat Pompeu Fabra, C. Ramon Trias Fargas, 25-27, 08005 Barcelona, Spain.
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Bettoni R, Bulf H, Brady S, Johnson SP. Infants' learning of non-adjacent regularities from visual sequences. INFANCY 2021; 26:319-326. [PMID: 33438835 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12384] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2020] [Revised: 11/03/2020] [Accepted: 12/16/2020] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Tracking adjacent (AD) and non-adjacent (NAD) dependencies in a sequence of elements is critical for the development of many complex abilities, such as language acquisition and social interaction. While learning of AD in infancy is a domain-general ability that is functioning across different domains, infants' processing of NAD has been reported only for speech sequences. Here, we tested 9- to 12- and 13- to 15-month-olds' ability to extract AxB grammars in visual sequences of unfamiliar elements. Infants were habituated to a series of 3-visual arrays following an AxB grammar in which the first element (A) predicted the third element (B), while intervening X elements changed continuously. Following habituation, infants were tested with 3-item arrays in which initial and final positions were switched (novel) or kept consistent with the habituation phase (familiar). Older infants successfully recognized the familiar AxB grammar at test, whereas the younger group showed some sensitivity to extract to NAD, albeit in a less robust form. This finding provides the first evidence that the ability to track NAD is a domain-general ability that is present also in the visual domain and that the sensitivity to such dependencies is related to developmental changes, as demonstrated in the auditory domain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roberta Bettoni
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy.,NeuroMi, Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milan, Italy
| | - Hermann Bulf
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy.,NeuroMi, Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milan, Italy
| | - Shannon Brady
- Department of Psychology, UC Riverside, Riverside, CA, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Frost RLA, Dunn K, Christiansen MH, Gómez RL, Monaghan P. Exploring the "anchor word" effect in infants: Segmentation and categorisation of speech with and without high frequency words. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0243436. [PMID: 33332419 PMCID: PMC7746152 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243436] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2020] [Accepted: 11/23/2020] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
High frequency words play a key role in language acquisition, with recent work suggesting they may serve both speech segmentation and lexical categorisation. However, it is not yet known whether infants can detect novel high frequency words in continuous speech, nor whether they can use them to help learning for segmentation and categorisation at the same time. For instance, when hearing "you eat the biscuit", can children use the high-frequency words "you" and "the" to segment out "eat" and "biscuit", and determine their respective lexical categories? We tested this in two experiments. In Experiment 1, we familiarised 12-month-old infants with continuous artificial speech comprising repetitions of target words, which were preceded by high-frequency marker words that distinguished the targets into two distributional categories. In Experiment 2, we repeated the task using the same language but with additional phonological cues to word and category structure. In both studies, we measured learning with head-turn preference tests of segmentation and categorisation, and compared performance against a control group that heard the artificial speech without the marker words (i.e., just the targets). There was no evidence that high frequency words helped either speech segmentation or grammatical categorisation. However, segmentation was seen to improve when the distributional information was supplemented with phonological cues (Experiment 2). In both experiments, exploratory analysis indicated that infants' looking behaviour was related to their linguistic maturity (indexed by infants' vocabulary scores) with infants with high versus low vocabulary scores displaying novelty and familiarity preferences, respectively. We propose that high-frequency words must reach a critical threshold of familiarity before they can be of significant benefit to learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Kirsty Dunn
- Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
| | | | - Rebecca L. Gómez
- University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
van der Kant A, Männel C, Paul M, Friederici AD, Höhle B, Wartenburger I. Linguistic and non-linguistic non-adjacent dependency learning in early development. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2020; 45:100819. [PMID: 32828032 PMCID: PMC7451682 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100819] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2019] [Revised: 05/27/2020] [Accepted: 06/29/2020] [Indexed: 01/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Non-adjacent dependencies (NADs) are important building blocks for language and extracting them from the input is a fundamental part of language acquisition. Prior event-related potential (ERP) studies revealed changes in the neural signature of NAD learning between infancy and adulthood, suggesting a developmental shift in the learning route for NADs. The present study aimed to specify which brain regions are involved in this developmental shift and whether this shift extends to NAD learning in the non-linguistic domain. In two experiments, 2- and 3-year-old German-learning children were familiarized with either Italian sentences or tone sequences containing NADs and subsequently tested with NAD violations, while functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) data were recorded. Results showed increased hemodynamic responses related to the detection of linguistic NAD violations in the left temporal, inferior frontal, and parietal regions in 2-year-old children, but not in 3-year-old children. A different developmental trajectory was found for non-linguistic NADs, where 3-year-old, but not 2-year-old children showed evidence for the detection of non-linguistic NAD violations. These results confirm a developmental shift in the NAD learning route and point to distinct mechanisms underlying NAD learning in the linguistic and the non-linguistic domain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anne van der Kant
- Cognitive Sciences, Department Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Germany.
| | - Claudia Männel
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Department of Neuropsychology, Leipzig, Germany; Department of Audiology and Phoniatrics, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany
| | - Mariella Paul
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Department of Neuropsychology, Leipzig, Germany; Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany
| | - Angela D Friederici
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Department of Neuropsychology, Leipzig, Germany; Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany
| | - Barbara Höhle
- Cognitive Sciences, Department Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Germany
| | - Isabell Wartenburger
- Cognitive Sciences, Department Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Germany; Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
|
13
|
Endress AD, Slone LK, Johnson SP. Statistical learning and memory. Cognition 2020; 204:104346. [PMID: 32615468 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104346] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2019] [Revised: 05/12/2020] [Accepted: 05/30/2020] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Learners often need to identify and remember recurring units in continuous sequences, but the underlying mechanisms are debated. A particularly prominent candidate mechanism relies on distributional statistics such as Transitional Probabilities (TPs). However, it is unclear what the outputs of statistical segmentation mechanisms are, and if learners store these outputs as discrete chunks in memory. We critically review the evidence for the possibility that statistically coherent items are stored in memory and outline difficulties in interpreting past research. We use Slone and Johnson's (2018) experiments as a case study to show that it is difficult to delineate the different mechanisms learners might use to solve a learning problem. Slone and Johnson (2018) reported that 8-month-old infants learned coherent chunks of shapes in visual sequences. Here, we describe an alternate interpretation of their findings based on a multiple-cue integration perspective. First, when multiple cues to statistical structure were available, infants' looking behavior seemed to track with the strength of the strongest one - backward TPs, suggesting that infants process multiple cues simultaneously and select the strongest one. Second, like adults, infants are exquisitely sensitive to chunks, but may require multiple cues to extract them. In Slone and Johnson's (2018) experiments, these cues were provided by immediate chunk repetitions during familiarization. Accordingly, infants showed strongest evidence of chunking following familiarization sequences in which immediate repetitions were more frequent. These interpretations provide a strong argument for infants' processing of multiple cues and the potential importance of multiple cues for chunk recognition in infancy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ansgar D Endress
- Department of Psychology, City, University of London, United Kingdom.
| | - Lauren K Slone
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, United States; Department of Psychology, Hope College, Holland, United States
| | - Scott P Johnson
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Wilson B, Spierings M, Ravignani A, Mueller JL, Mintz TH, Wijnen F, van der Kant A, Smith K, Rey A. Non-adjacent Dependency Learning in Humans and Other Animals. Top Cogn Sci 2020; 12:843-858. [PMID: 32729673 PMCID: PMC7496455 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12381] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2018] [Revised: 05/22/2018] [Accepted: 05/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Learning and processing natural language requires the ability to track syntactic relationships between words and phrases in a sentence, which are often separated by intervening material. These nonadjacent dependencies can be studied using artificial grammar learning paradigms and structured sequence processing tasks. These approaches have been used to demonstrate that human adults, infants and some nonhuman animals are able to detect and learn dependencies between nonadjacent elements within a sequence. However, learning nonadjacent dependencies appears to be more cognitively demanding than detecting dependencies between adjacent elements, and only occurs in certain circumstances. In this review, we discuss different types of nonadjacent dependencies in language and in artificial grammar learning experiments, and how these differences might impact learning. We summarize different types of perceptual cues that facilitate learning, by highlighting the relationship between dependent elements bringing them closer together either physically, attentionally, or perceptually. Finally, we review artificial grammar learning experiments in human adults, infants, and nonhuman animals, and discuss how similarities and differences observed across these groups can provide insights into how language is learned across development and how these language-related abilities might have evolved.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Andrea Ravignani
- Research DepartmentSealcentre Pieterburen
- Artificial Intelligence LabVrije Universiteit Brussel
| | | | - Toben H. Mintz
- Departments of Psychology and LinguisticsUniversity of Southern California
| | - Frank Wijnen
- Utrecht Institute of Linguistics OTSUtrecht University
| | | | - Kenny Smith
- Centre for Language EvolutionUniversity of Edinburgh
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Frost RLA, Jessop A, Durrant S, Peter MS, Bidgood A, Pine JM, Rowland CF, Monaghan P. Non-adjacent dependency learning in infancy, and its link to language development. Cogn Psychol 2020; 120:101291. [PMID: 32197131 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2020.101291] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2019] [Revised: 02/20/2020] [Accepted: 03/05/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
To acquire language, infants must learn how to identify words and linguistic structure in speech. Statistical learning has been suggested to assist both of these tasks. However, infants' capacity to use statistics to discover words and structure together remains unclear. Further, it is not yet known how infants' statistical learning ability relates to their language development. We trained 17-month-old infants on an artificial language comprising non-adjacent dependencies, and examined their looking times on tasks assessing sensitivity to words and structure using an eye-tracked head-turn-preference paradigm. We measured infants' vocabulary size using a Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) concurrently and at 19, 21, 24, 25, 27, and 30 months to relate performance to language development. Infants could segment the words from speech, demonstrated by a significant difference in looking times to words versus part-words. Infants' segmentation performance was significantly related to their vocabulary size (receptive and expressive) both currently, and over time (receptive until 24 months, expressive until 30 months), but was not related to the rate of vocabulary growth. The data also suggest infants may have developed sensitivity to generalised structure, indicating similar statistical learning mechanisms may contribute to the discovery of words and structure in speech, but this was not related to vocabulary size.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Andrew Jessop
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands
| | | | | | | | | | - Caroline F Rowland
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands; University of Liverpool, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Hall J, Owen Van Horne AJ, McGregor KK, Farmer TA. Individual and Developmental Differences in Distributional Learning. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2019; 49:694-709. [PMID: 30120447 DOI: 10.1044/2018_lshss-stlt1-17-0134] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2017] [Accepted: 03/11/2018] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose This study examined whether children and adults with developmental language disorder (DLD) could use distributional information in an artificial language to learn about grammatical category membership similarly to their typically developing (TD) peers and whether developmental differences existed within and between DLD and TD groups. Method Sixteen children ages 7-9 with DLD, 26 age-matched TD children, 17 college students with DLD, and 17 TD college students participated in this task. We used an artificial grammar learning paradigm in which participants had to use knowledge of category membership to determine the acceptability of test items that they had not heard during a training phase. Results Individuals with DLD performed similarly to TD peers in distinguishing grammatical from ungrammatical combinations, with no differences between age groups. The order in which items were heard at test differentially affected child versus adult participants and showed a relation with attention and phonological working memory as well. Conclusion Differences in ratings between grammatical and ungrammatical items in this task suggest that individuals with DLD can form grammatical categories from novel input and more broadly use distributional information. Differences in order effects suggest a developmental timeline for sensitivity to updating distributional information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Karla K McGregor
- The University of Iowa, Iowa City.,Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, Nebraska
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Mueller JL, Cate CT, Toro JM. A Comparative Perspective on the Role of Acoustic Cues in Detecting Language Structure. Top Cogn Sci 2018; 12:859-874. [PMID: 30033636 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12373] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2018] [Revised: 05/20/2018] [Accepted: 06/20/2018] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Most human language learners acquire language primarily via the auditory modality. This is one reason why auditory artificial grammars play a prominent role in the investigation of the development and evolutionary roots of human syntax. The present position paper brings together findings from human and non-human research on the impact of auditory cues on learning about linguistic structures with a special focus on how different types of cues and biases in auditory cognition may contribute to success and failure in artificial grammar learning (AGL). The basis of our argument is the link between auditory cues and syntactic structure across languages and development. Cross-species comparison suggests that many aspects of auditory cognition that are relevant for language are not human specific and are present even in rather distantly related species. Furthermore, auditory cues and biases impact on learning, which we will discuss in the example of auditory perception and AGL studies. This observation, together with the significant role of auditory cues in language processing, supports the idea that auditory cues served as a bootstrap to syntax during language evolution. Yet this also means that potentially human-specific syntactic abilities are not due to basic auditory differences between humans and non-human animals but are based upon more advanced cognitive processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Carel Ten Cate
- Institute of Biology, Leiden University.,Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition
| | - Juan M Toro
- ICREA (Institució Catalana de Recerca I Estudis Avançats).,Center for Brain and Cognition, University Pompeu Fabra
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Mueller JL, Friederici AD, Männel C. Developmental changes in automatic rule-learning mechanisms across early childhood. Dev Sci 2018; 22:e12700. [PMID: 29949219 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12700] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2017] [Accepted: 05/15/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Infants' ability to learn complex linguistic regularities from early on has been revealed by electrophysiological studies indicating that 3-month-olds, but not adults, can automatically detect non-adjacent dependencies between syllables. While different ERP responses in adults and infants suggest that both linguistic rule learning and its link to basic auditory processing undergo developmental changes, systematic investigations of the developmental trajectories are scarce. In the present study, we assessed 2- and 4-year-olds' ERP indicators of pitch discrimination and linguistic rule learning in a syllable-based oddball design. To test for the relation between auditory discrimination and rule learning, ERP responses to pitch changes were used as predictor for potential linguistic rule-learning effects. Results revealed that 2-year-olds, but not 4-year-olds, showed ERP markers of rule learning. Although, 2-year-olds' rule learning was not dependent on differences in pitch perception, 4-year-old children demonstrated a dependency, such that those children who showed more pronounced responses to pitch changes still showed an effect of rule learning. These results narrow down the developmental decline of the ability for automatic linguistic rule learning to the age between 2 and 4 years, and, moreover, point towards a strong modification of this change by auditory processes. At an age when the ability of automatic linguistic rule learning phases out, rule learning can still be observed in children with enhanced auditory responses. The observed interrelations are plausible causes for age-of-acquisition effects and inter-individual differences in language learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jutta L Mueller
- Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrück, Osnabrück, Germany.,Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Angela D Friederici
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Claudia Männel
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.,Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig and Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, Medical Faculty of the University of Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Malassis R, Rey A, Fagot J. Non-adjacent Dependencies Processing in Human and Non-human Primates. Cogn Sci 2018; 42:1677-1699. [PMID: 29781135 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12617] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2017] [Revised: 03/13/2018] [Accepted: 03/14/2018] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
Human and non-human primates share the ability to extract adjacent dependencies and, under certain conditions, non-adjacent dependencies (i.e., predictive relationships between elements that are separated by one or several intervening elements in a sequence). In this study, we explore the online extraction dynamics of non-adjacent dependencies in humans and baboons using a serial reaction time task. Participants had to produce three-target sequences containing deterministic relationships between the first and last target locations. In Experiment 1, participants from the two species could extract these non-adjacent dependencies, but humans required less exposure than baboons. In Experiment 2, the data show for the first time in a non-human primate species the successful generalization of sequential non-adjacent dependencies over novel intervening items. These findings provide new evidence to further constrain current theories about the nature and the evolutionary origins of the learning mechanisms allowing the extraction of non-adjacent dependencies.
Collapse
|
20
|
Nowak I, Baggio G. Developmental Constraints on Learning Artificial Grammars with Fixed, Flexible and Free Word Order. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1816. [PMID: 29089910 PMCID: PMC5651074 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01816] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2017] [Accepted: 09/29/2017] [Indexed: 01/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Human learning, although highly flexible and efficient, is constrained in ways that facilitate or impede the acquisition of certain systems of information. Some such constraints, active during infancy and childhood, have been proposed to account for the apparent ease with which typically developing children acquire language. In a series of experiments, we investigated the role of developmental constraints on learning artificial grammars with a distinction between shorter and relatively frequent words ('function words,' F-words) and longer and less frequent words ('content words,' C-words). We constructed 4 finite-state grammars, in which the order of F-words, relative to C-words, was either fixed (F-words always occupied the same positions in a string), flexible (every F-word always followed a C-word), or free. We exposed adults (N = 84) and kindergarten children (N = 100) to strings from each of these artificial grammars, and we assessed their ability to recognize strings with the same structure, but a different vocabulary. Adults were better at recognizing strings when regularities were available (i.e., fixed and flexible order grammars), while children were better at recognizing strings from the grammars consistent with the attested distribution of function and content words in natural languages (i.e., flexible and free order grammars). These results provide evidence for a link between developmental constraints on learning and linguistic typology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iga Nowak
- Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
- International School for Advanced Studies, Trieste, Italy
| | - Giosuè Baggio
- International School for Advanced Studies, Trieste, Italy
- Language Acquisition and Language Processing Lab, Department of Language and Literature, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Grimm R, Cassani G, Gillis S, Daelemans W. Facilitatory Effects of Multi-Word Units in Lexical Processing and Word Learning: A Computational Investigation. Front Psychol 2017; 8:555. [PMID: 28450842 PMCID: PMC5390038 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00555] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2017] [Accepted: 03/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have suggested that children and adults form cognitive representations of co-occurring word sequences. We propose (1) that the formation of such multi-word unit (MWU) representations precedes and facilitates the formation of single-word representations in children and thus benefits word learning, and (2) that MWU representations facilitate adult word recognition and thus benefit lexical processing. Using a modified version of an existing computational model (McCauley and Christiansen, 2014), we extract MWUs from a corpus of child-directed speech (CDS) and a corpus of conversations among adults. We then correlate the number of MWUs within which each word appears with (1) age of first production and (2) adult reaction times on a word recognition task. In doing so, we take care to control for the effect of word frequency, as frequent words will naturally tend to occur in many MWUs. We also compare results to a baseline model which randomly groups words into sequences-and find that MWUs have a unique facilitatory effect on both response variables, suggesting that they benefit word learning in children and word recognition in adults. The effect is strongest on age of first production, implying that MWUs are comparatively more important for word learning than for adult lexical processing. We discuss possible underlying mechanisms and formulate testable predictions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert Grimm
- Department of Linguistics, Computational Linguistics and Psycholinguistics Research Center, University of AntwerpAntwerp, Belgium
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Thiessen ED. What's statistical about learning? Insights from modelling statistical learning as a set of memory processes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2017; 372:20160056. [PMID: 27872374 PMCID: PMC5124081 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Statistical learning has been studied in a variety of different tasks, including word segmentation, object identification, category learning, artificial grammar learning and serial reaction time tasks (e.g. Saffran et al. 1996 Science 274: , 1926-1928; Orban et al. 2008 Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: , 2745-2750; Thiessen & Yee 2010 Child Development 81: , 1287-1303; Saffran 2002 Journal of Memory and Language 47: , 172-196; Misyak & Christiansen 2012 Language Learning 62: , 302-331). The difference among these tasks raises questions about whether they all depend on the same kinds of underlying processes and computations, or whether they are tapping into different underlying mechanisms. Prior theoretical approaches to statistical learning have often tried to explain or model learning in a single task. However, in many cases these approaches appear inadequate to explain performance in multiple tasks. For example, explaining word segmentation via the computation of sequential statistics (such as transitional probability) provides little insight into the nature of sensitivity to regularities among simultaneously presented features. In this article, we will present a formal computational approach that we believe is a good candidate to provide a unifying framework to explore and explain learning in a wide variety of statistical learning tasks. This framework suggests that statistical learning arises from a set of processes that are inherent in memory systems, including activation, interference, integration of information and forgetting (e.g. Perruchet & Vinter 1998 Journal of Memory and Language 39: , 246-263; Thiessen et al. 2013 Psychological Bulletin 139: , 792-814). From this perspective, statistical learning does not involve explicit computation of statistics, but rather the extraction of elements of the input into memory traces, and subsequent integration across those memory traces that emphasize consistent information (Thiessen and Pavlik 2013 Cognitive Science 37: , 310-343).This article is part of the themed issue 'New frontiers for statistical learning in the cognitive sciences'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erik D Thiessen
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, 5000 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Endress AD, Langus A. Transitional probabilities count more than frequency, but might not be used for memorization. Cogn Psychol 2016; 92:37-64. [PMID: 27907807 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2016] [Revised: 11/08/2016] [Accepted: 11/09/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Learners often need to extract recurring items from continuous sequences, in both vision and audition. The best-known example is probably found in word-learning, where listeners have to determine where words start and end in fluent speech. This could be achieved through universal and experience-independent statistical mechanisms, for example by relying on Transitional Probabilities (TPs). Further, these mechanisms might allow learners to store items in memory. However, previous investigations have yielded conflicting evidence as to whether a sensitivity to TPs is diagnostic of the memorization of recurring items. Here, we address this issue in the visual modality. Participants were familiarized with a continuous sequence of visual items (i.e., arbitrary or everyday symbols), and then had to choose between (i) high-TP items that appeared in the sequence, (ii) high-TP items that did not appear in the sequence, and (iii) low-TP items that appeared in the sequence. Items matched in TPs but differing in (chunk) frequency were much harder to discriminate than items differing in TPs (with no significant sensitivity to chunk frequency), and learners preferred unattested high-TP items over attested low-TP items. Contrary to previous claims, these results cannot be explained on the basis of the similarity of the test items. Learners thus weigh within-item TPs higher than the frequency of the chunks, even when the TP differences are relatively subtle. We argue that these results are problematic for distributional clustering mechanisms that analyze continuous sequences, and provide supporting computational results. We suggest that the role of TPs might not be to memorize items per se, but rather to prepare learners to memorize recurring items once they are presented in subsequent learning situations with richer cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Alan Langus
- Cognitive Neuroscience Sector, International School for Advanced Studies, Trieste, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Abstract
Language learners encounter numerous opportunities to learn regularities, but need to decide which of these regularities to learn, because some are not productive in their native language. Here, we present an account of rule learning based on perceptual and memory primitives (Endress, Dehaene-Lambertz, & Mehler, Cognition, 105(3), 577–614, 2007; Endress, Nespor, & Mehler, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 348–353, 2009), suggesting that learners preferentially learn regularities that are more salient to them, and that the pattern of salience reflects the frequency of language features across languages. We contrast this view with previous artificial grammar learning research, which suggests that infants “choose” the regularities they learn based on rational, Bayesian criteria (Frank & Tenenbaum, Cognition, 120(3), 360–371, 2013; Gerken, Cognition, 98(3)B67–B74, 2006, Cognition, 115(2), 362–366, 2010). In our experiments, adult participants listened to syllable strings starting with a syllable reduplication and always ending with the same “affix” syllable, or to syllable strings starting with this “affix” syllable and ending with the “reduplication”. Both affixation and reduplication are frequently used for morphological marking across languages. We find three crucial results. First, participants learned both regularities simultaneously. Second, affixation regularities seemed easier to learn than reduplication regularities. Third, regularities in sequence offsets were easier to learn than regularities at sequence onsets. We show that these results are inconsistent with previous Bayesian rule learning models, but mesh well with the perceptual or memory primitives view. Further, we show that the pattern of salience revealed in our experiments reflects the distribution of regularities across languages. Ease of acquisition might thus be one determinant of the frequency of regularities across languages.
Collapse
|
25
|
Endress AD, Bonatti LL. Words, rules, and mechanisms of language acquisition. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2015; 7:19-35. [PMID: 26683248 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1376] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2015] [Revised: 09/21/2015] [Accepted: 11/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
We review recent artificial language learning studies, especially those following Endress and Bonatti (Endress AD, Bonatti LL. Rapid learning of syllable classes from a perceptually continuous speech stream. Cognition 2007, 105:247-299), suggesting that humans can deploy a variety of learning mechanisms to acquire artificial languages. Several experiments provide evidence for multiple learning mechanisms that can be deployed in fluent speech: one mechanism encodes the positions of syllables within words and can be used to extract generalization, while the other registers co-occurrence statistics of syllables and can be used to break a continuum into its components. We review dissociations between these mechanisms and their potential role in language acquisition. We then turn to recent criticisms of the multiple mechanisms hypothesis and show that they are inconsistent with the available data. Our results suggest that artificial and natural language learning is best understood by dissecting the underlying specialized learning abilities, and that these data provide a rare opportunity to link important language phenomena to basic psychological mechanisms. For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website.
Collapse
|
26
|
Frequency-based organization of speech sequences in a nonhuman animal. Cognition 2015; 146:1-7. [PMID: 26398859 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2014] [Revised: 07/27/2015] [Accepted: 09/07/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
A recurrent question regarding language acquisition is the extent to which the mechanisms human infants use to discover patterns over the linguistic signal are highly specialized and uniquely human, or are the result of more general mechanisms present in other species. Research with very young infants suggests that they are able to use the relative frequency of elements in a linguistic sequence to infer word order. Here we ask if this ability could emerge from grouping biases present in nonhuman mammals. We show that animals discover differences in the frequency of elements in a sequence and can learn the relative order of frequent and infrequent elements. Nevertheless, in animals, relative frequency does not appear to be overridden by other cues that have been shown to be important to human infants, such as prosody. Our results demonstrate that the basic mechanism that allows listeners to extract ordering relations based on frequency is shared across species.
Collapse
|
27
|
Marchetto E, Bonatti LL. Finding words and word structure in artificial speech: the development of infants' sensitivity to morphosyntactic regularities. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2015; 42:873-902. [PMID: 25300736 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000914000452] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
To achieve language proficiency, infants must find the building blocks of speech and master the rules governing their legal combinations. However, these problems are linked: words are also built according to rules. Here, we explored early morphosyntactic sensitivity by testing when and how infants could find either words or within-word structure in artificial speech snippets embodying properties of morphological constructions. We show that 12-month-olds use statistical relationships between syllables to extract words from continuous streams, but find word-internal regularities only if the streams are segmented. Seven-month-olds fail both tasks. Thus, 12-month-olds infants possess the resources to analyze the internal composition of words if the speech contains segmentation information. However, 7-month-old infants may not possess them, although they can track several statistical relations. This developmental difference suggests that morphosyntactic sensitivity may require computational resources extending beyond the detection of simple statistics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erika Marchetto
- SISSA/ISAS,Trieste,Italy Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique (LSCP),Ecole Normale Supérieure,Paris,France
| | - Luca L Bonatti
- ICREA and Universitat Pompeu Fabra,Centre for Brain and Cognition,Barcelona,Spain
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
Over the past 20 years, the field of cognitive neuroscience has relied heavily on hemodynamic measures of blood oxygenation in local regions of the brain to make inferences about underlying cognitive processes. These same functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) techniques have recently been adapted for use with human infants. We review the advantages and disadvantages of these two neuroimaging methods for studies of infant cognition, with a particular emphasis on their technical limitations and the linking hypotheses that are used to draw conclusions from correlational data. In addition to summarizing key findings in several domains of infant cognition, we highlight the prospects of improving the quality of fNIRS data from infants to address in a more sophisticated way how cognitive development is mediated by changes in underlying neural mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Richard N Aslin
- Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York 14627; ,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|