1
|
Muraki EJ, Born S, Pexman PM. Grasping Variance in Word Norms: Individual Differences in Motor Imagery and Semantic Ratings. J Cogn 2025; 8:12. [PMID: 39803178 PMCID: PMC11720573 DOI: 10.5334/joc.418] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2024] [Accepted: 11/09/2024] [Indexed: 01/16/2025] Open
Abstract
Word norming datasets have become an important resource for psycholinguistic research, and they are based on the underlying assumption that individual differences are inconsequential to the measurement of semantic dimensions. In this pre-registered study we tested this assumption by examining whether individual differences in motor imagery are related to variance in semantic ratings. We collected graspability ratings (i.e., how easily a word's referent can be grasped using one hand) for 350 words and also had each participant complete a series of motor imagery questionnaires. Using linear mixed effect models we tested whether measures of motor imagery ability (e.g., the Florida Praxis Imagery Questionnaire and the Test of Ability in Movement Imagery for Hands) and motor imagery vividness (e.g., the Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire 2) could account for variance (raw and absolute difference scores) in graspability ratings. We observed a significant relationship between motor imagery vividness and absolute rating difference scores, wherein people with more vivid motor imagery provided ratings that were further from the mean word ratings. However there was no relationship between motor imagery and raw rating difference scores. The results suggest that there are measurable systematic differences in how participants make sensorimotor semantic ratings, which has implications for how sensorimotor semantic word norms are used for investigations of lexical semantic processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emiko J. Muraki
- Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
| | - Sydney Born
- Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
| | - Penny M. Pexman
- Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Colliot P, Plancher G, Fournier H, Labaronne M, Chainay H. Effect of negative emotional stimuli on working memory: Impact of voluntary and automatic attention. Psychon Bull Rev 2024:10.3758/s13423-024-02593-2. [PMID: 39375301 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02593-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/15/2024] [Indexed: 10/09/2024]
Abstract
Emotions are known to influence cognitive performance, particularly working memory (WM) in both its aspects, processing, and maintenance. One explanatory mechanism might be that negative stimuli capture attentional resources, leaving fewer resources for attentional maintenance and processing of information in WM. However, this attentional capture was only investigated using WM tasks in which participants were explicitly asked to process negative items. The aim of this paper was to determine whether explicit processing of emotional stimuli is necessary to impair WM performance, or if their mere presence is enough to capture attention. For this purpose, participants performed a complex span task in which they alternated between memorizing a series of neutral words and processing either emotional images or neutral ones. In Experiment 1, participants were instructed to explicitly process emotional images, whereas in Experiment 2, emotional images were presented without any processing being required. In both experiments, we observed a decrease in memory performance when the images were negative compared to neutral. Whether or not voluntary processing is involved, emotional images seem to capture attentional resources, which in turn leads to a decline in memory performance. These results were discussed in relation to attentional theories and the influence of emotion on the specific mechanisms of WM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pascale Colliot
- Laboratoire d'Étude Des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université Lyon 2, Bron, France.
| | - Gaën Plancher
- Laboratoire d'Étude Des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université Lyon 2, Bron, France
- Institut Universitaire de France, IUF, Paris, France
| | - Hippolyte Fournier
- Laboratoire d'Étude Des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université Lyon 2, Bron, France
- LIG, Université Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, Inria, Grenoble INP, Grenoble, France
| | - Maximilien Labaronne
- Laboratoire d'Étude Des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université Lyon 2, Bron, France
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Fribourg, Fribourg, Suisse
| | - Hanna Chainay
- Laboratoire d'Étude Des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université Lyon 2, Bron, France
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Brunel J, Delord S, Mathey S. Inhibition in the emotional Hayling task: can hypnotic suggestion enhance cognitive control on a prepotent negative word? Cogn Emot 2024:1-9. [PMID: 38888553 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2367712] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2023] [Accepted: 06/09/2024] [Indexed: 06/20/2024]
Abstract
Recent studies suggest that instrumental hypnosis is a useful experimental tool to investigate emotional and language processing effects. However, the capacity of hypnotic suggestions to intervene during the response inhibition of emotional words remains elusive. This study investigated whether hypnotic suggestion can improve the inhibition of prepotent negative word responses in an emotional Hayling sentence completion task. High-suggestible participants performed a computerised emotional Hayling task. They were first asked to select the appropriate words ending highly predictable sentences among two propositions (initiation part), and then to select the filler words that did not end the sentences correctly (inhibition part). Half of the expected final words had a negative emotional valence, while the other half was neutral. The task was performed in a control condition (without suggestion) and with a hypnotic suggestion to decrease emotional reactivity. The results revealed that hypnotic suggestion (compared to the control condition) hastened response times on negative final words in the inhibition part, showing that hypnotic suggestion can enhance cognitive control over prepotent negative word responses in a sentence completion task. We suggest that this modulation stems from a reduction in the emotional relevance of the final words caused by the hypnotic suggestion.
Collapse
|
4
|
Pillaud N, Ballot C, Robert C, Mathey S, Ric F. Is the approach avoidance compatibility effect moderated by word imageability? Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2024; 77:1281-1294. [PMID: 37649391 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231194499] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/01/2023]
Abstract
The approach/avoidance (AA) compatibility effect refers to the fact that individuals respond faster by an approach movement to positive than to negative stimuli, whereas they respond faster by an avoidance movement to negative than to positive stimuli. Although this effect has been observed in many studies, the underlying mechanisms remain still unclear. On the basis of recent studies suggesting a key role of sensorimotor information in the emergence of the AA compatibility effect, the present study aimed to investigate the specific role of visual information, operationalised through word imageability, in the production of the AA compatibility effect. We orthogonally manipulated the emotional valence (positive/negative) and the imageability (low/high) of words in an incidental online-AA task (i.e., in the absence of valence processing goals) using a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 300 ms. In line with previous studies, Experiment 1 revealed an AA compatibility effect in the absence of valence processing goals. However, this effect was not moderated by word imageability. In Experiment 2, we examined whether the absence of influence of word imageability could be due to the short SOA (300 ms) used in this experiment. We used the same design as in Experiment 1 and manipulated the SOA (400 vs 600 ms). We again observed an AA compatibility effect which was not moderated by word imageability, whatever the SOA used. The results of both experiments suggest the absence of any influence of sensorimotor information in the AA compatibility effect, at least when provided by the to-be-approached/avoided stimulus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas Pillaud
- Laboratoire de Psychologie, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | | | - Christelle Robert
- Laboratoire de Psychologie, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | - Stéphanie Mathey
- Laboratoire de Psychologie, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | - François Ric
- Laboratoire de Psychologie, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Quillivic R, Gayraud F, Auxéméry Y, Vanni L, Peschanski D, Eustache F, Dayan J, Mesmoudi S. Interdisciplinary approach to identify language markers for post-traumatic stress disorder using machine learning and deep learning. Sci Rep 2024; 14:12468. [PMID: 38816468 PMCID: PMC11139884 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-61557-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2023] [Accepted: 05/07/2024] [Indexed: 06/01/2024] Open
Abstract
Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) lacks clear biomarkers in clinical practice. Language as a potential diagnostic biomarker for PTSD is investigated in this study. We analyze an original cohort of 148 individuals exposed to the November 13, 2015, terrorist attacks in Paris. The interviews, conducted 5-11 months after the event, include individuals from similar socioeconomic backgrounds exposed to the same incident, responding to identical questions and using uniform PTSD measures. Using this dataset to collect nuanced insights that might be clinically relevant, we propose a three-step interdisciplinary methodology that integrates expertise from psychiatry, linguistics, and the Natural Language Processing (NLP) community to examine the relationship between language and PTSD. The first step assesses a clinical psychiatrist's ability to diagnose PTSD using interview transcription alone. The second step uses statistical analysis and machine learning models to create language features based on psycholinguistic hypotheses and evaluate their predictive strength. The third step is the application of a hypothesis-free deep learning approach to the classification of PTSD in our cohort. Results show that the clinical psychiatrist achieved a diagnosis of PTSD with an AUC of 0.72. This is comparable to a gold standard questionnaire (Area Under Curve (AUC) ≈ 0.80). The machine learning model achieved a diagnostic AUC of 0.69. The deep learning approach achieved an AUC of 0.64. An examination of model error informs our discussion. Importantly, the study controls for confounding factors, establishes associations between language and DSM-5 subsymptoms, and integrates automated methods with qualitative analysis. This study provides a direct and methodologically robust description of the relationship between PTSD and language. Our work lays the groundwork for advancing early and accurate diagnosis and using linguistic markers to assess the effectiveness of pharmacological treatments and psychotherapies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robin Quillivic
- PSL-EPHE, Paris, France.
- ISCPIF, Institut des Systèmes Complexes, Paris île-de-France, France.
| | - Frédérique Gayraud
- Laboratoire dynamique du langage, UMR 5596, CNRS, université ́ Lyon-II, Lyon, France
| | - Yann Auxéméry
- Centre Hospitalier de Jury-les-Metz, centre de réhabilitation pour adultes, Metz, France
- UMR 1319 Inspiire, INSERM, Université de Lorraine, 9 avenue de la forêt de Haye, Nancy, France
| | - Laurent Vanni
- CNRS, UMR 7320 : Bases, Corpus, Langage, Nice, France
| | - Denis Peschanski
- Université PARIS 1 Panthéon-Sorbonne, Paris, France
- CNRS, CESSP, UMR 8209, Paris, France
| | - Francis Eustache
- PSL-EPHE, Paris, France
- INSERM, NIMH U1077, Caen, France
- UNICAEN, Caen, France
| | - Jacques Dayan
- PSL-EPHE, Paris, France
- INSERM, NIMH U1077, Caen, France
- UNICAEN, Caen, France
- CHU de Rennes, Rennes, France
| | - Salma Mesmoudi
- PSL-EPHE, Paris, France
- ISCPIF, Institut des Systèmes Complexes, Paris île-de-France, France
- Université PARIS 1 Panthéon-Sorbonne, Paris, France
- CNRS, CESSP, UMR 8209, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Brunel J, Delord S, Mathey S. Hypnotic suggestion modulates visual recognition of negative words depending on word arousal. Conscious Cogn 2023; 115:103569. [PMID: 37660419 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2023.103569] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/18/2023] [Revised: 07/13/2023] [Accepted: 08/25/2023] [Indexed: 09/05/2023]
Abstract
This study examined whether and how emotional hypnotic suggestions modulate the visual recognition of negative words. We investigated the influence of hypnotic suggestions aimed at modifying emotional reactivity on the arousal effect in negative words. High and low suggestible individuals performed a go/no-go lexical decision task in three intra-individual conditions: with a suggestion to increase emotional reactivity, with a suggestion to decrease emotional reactivity and without hypnotic suggestion. Results showed that hypnotic suggestions modulated the arousal facilitation effect differently depending on the level of suggestibility of the participants. In high suggestible individuals, response times for low-arousal negative words varied oppositely according to the suggestion administered, while no modulations were retrieved for high-arousal ones. In contrast, no suggestion effects were found for low suggestible participants. Altogether, these findings suggest a higher influence of hypnotic suggestions on emotional words that require longer processing times in high suggestible individuals.
Collapse
|
7
|
Song D, Wen H, Dong Y. Affective Norms for Chinese Words of Typical Life Scenes Rated by Older Adults (ANCO). JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2023; 52:1115-1140. [PMID: 37022625 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-023-09948-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/19/2023] [Indexed: 06/19/2023]
Abstract
The present study provides an affective norm collected from older adults for 1,050 Chinese words that are closely related to the typical life scenes commonly encountered by older adults. Data were collected for key affective dimensions of valence and arousal using the method of adapted Self-Assessment Manikin (Bradley & Lang, 1994) in a paper-and-pencil procedure. The results showed that the current database (ANCO) was of high reliability and validity. Valence and arousal were in an asymmetrically quadratic relationship in the valence-by-arousal space; i.e., older adults rated negative words as the highest arousing, followed by positive and neutral words. In addition, by comparing affective ratings of the shared words between the present norm collected from older Chinese adults and previous norms collected from young Chinese adults (Wang et al., 2008; Yao et al., 2017; Yu et al., 2016), we found that compared with young adults, older ones perceived negative words as more negative and more arousing, and perceived positive words as more positive and less arousing. ANCO can serve as a valuable source of information for age-related affective research and help explicate the effects of emotion on linguistic and cognitive processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dangui Song
- Language Processing and Development Lab, School of International Studies, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
| | - Hao Wen
- Language Processing and Development Lab, School of International Studies, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
| | - Yanping Dong
- Language Processing and Development Lab, School of International Studies, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Camblats AM, Mathey S, Robert C, Estival S, Chevalère J, Maire J, Tauber M, Laurier V, Tricot J, Mourre F, Postal V. Interference effect of food and emotional stimuli in Stroop-like tasks for children and adults with Prader-Willi Syndrome. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2023:1-16. [PMID: 37113059 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2023.2207777] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/29/2023]
Abstract
Interference effect of food and emotional stimuli in Stroop-like tasks for children and adults with Prader-Willi Syndrome. The aim of this work was to study the way items related to food or emotion are processed by a population known to have difficulties with dietary restriction, namely individuals with Prader-Willi Syndrome (PWS). Given the presence of intellectual disability (ID) in PWS, our experiments were designed to examine whether these difficulties were specific to PWS or linked with their ID. Two modified Stroop tasks (i.e., a food version and an emotional version) were administered to seventy-four children (aged between 6 and 16 years old) divided into three groups (one with PWS, one with ID matched on age and Intellectual Quotient (IQ), and one healthy group matched on age) and to eighty-four adults (aged between 18 and 48 years old) distributed in the same three groups. For both tasks, a picture version was used for the children and a word version for the adults. For the food Stroop task, (Experiment 1), materials were composed of low or high-caloric food items and stimuli not related to food. The results show a food Stroop effect for children and adults with PWS that was absent in the group of healthy participants. Moreover, a food Stroop effect was also significant for adults with ID. For the emotional Stroop task (Experiment 2), materials were composed of negative, positive and neutral stimuli. The emotional Stroop effect was also obtained for children and adults with PWS as well as for the healthy group, but not for the age- and IQ-matched group. For the PWS groups, results show a preservation to process positive pictures for children and difficulties to process negative stimuli for both age-groups. These results suggest that people with PWS have difficulties in disengaging their attention when food stimuli are present in their environment and poorer abilities to process negative ones. These difficulties endure in adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Stéphanie Mathey
- laboratoire de Psychologie EA4139, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | - Christelle Robert
- laboratoire de Psychologie EA4139, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | - Séverine Estival
- laboratoire de Psychologie EA4139, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| | - Johann Chevalère
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale et Cognitive (LAPSCO), UMR 6024, CNRS & Université Clermont-Auvergne, Clermont-Ferrand, France
| | - Jenna Maire
- Unité de recherche CERES, Institut Catholique de Toulouse, Toulouse, France
| | - Maïthé Tauber
- Centre de Référence du Syndrome de Prader-Willi, CHU Toulouse, Toulouse, France
| | - Virginie Laurier
- Centre de Référence des maladies rares PRADORT pour le Syndrome de Prader-Willi et autres et autres Obésités Rares avec TCA, AP-HP Hôpital Marin, Hendaye, France
| | - Julie Tricot
- Centre de Référence des maladies rares PRADORT pour le Syndrome de Prader-Willi et autres et autres Obésités Rares avec TCA, AP-HP Hôpital Marin, Hendaye, France
| | - Fabien Mourre
- Centre de Référence des maladies rares PRADORT pour le Syndrome de Prader-Willi et autres et autres Obésités Rares avec TCA, AP-HP Hôpital Marin, Hendaye, France
| | - Virginie Postal
- laboratoire de Psychologie EA4139, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Camblats AM, Gobin P, Mathey S. The Influence of Negative Orthographic Neighborhood in the Lexical Decision Task: Valence and Arousal Contributions. LANGUAGE AND SPEECH 2022; 65:740-754. [PMID: 34894850 DOI: 10.1177/00238309211061090] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
This study investigated whether the visual recognition of neutral words might be influenced by the emotional dimensions (i.e., valence and arousal) of orthographically similar lexical representations, and whether this might also depend on emotional-related traits of participants (i.e., alexithymia). To this end, 108 participants performed a lexical decision task with 80 neutral words with a higher frequency orthographic neighbor that varied in valence (from neutral to negative) and arousal (from low to high). The main finding was the expected interaction effect between the valence and arousal of the neighbor on the lexical decision times of neutral stimulus words. Longer reaction times were found when the valence score of the neighbor decreased from neutral to negative for words with a low-arousal orthographic neighbor while this emotional neighbor effect was reversed for words with a high-arousal negative neighbor. This combined influence of the valence and arousal of the neighbor was interpreted in terms of increased lexical competition processes and direct influence of the affective system on the participant's response. Moreover, this interaction effect was smaller when the level of alexithymia of the participants increased, suggesting that people with a higher level of alexithymia are less sensitive to the emotional content of the neighbor. The results are discussed within an interactive activation model of visual word recognition incorporating an affective system with valence and arousal dimensions, with regard to the role of the alexithymia level of participants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Pamela Gobin
- Laboratoire Cognition, Santé, Socialisation, C2S, EA 6291, Université de Reims, France; Département de Psychiatrie, Hôpital universitaire de Reims, France; Pôle Universitaire de Psychiatrie Adulte, CHU de Reims, EPSMM, France
| | - Stéphanie Mathey
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA4139, Université de Bordeaux, France
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Planchuelo C, Baciero A, Hinojosa JA, Perea M, Duñabeitia JA. Social context effects on emotional language: The influence of the COVID-19 pandemic on the emotional evaluation of words. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2022; 229:103686. [PMID: 35878447 PMCID: PMC9304427 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2021] [Revised: 07/15/2022] [Accepted: 07/17/2022] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic has altered our routines, our conversations, the specific social contexts in which we hear or use certain words, and potentially, the representation of the words related to the disease and its consequences. Here we investigated whether the effects of the pandemic have changed the representation of the affective features of COVID-19-related words. To this aim, we collected new ratings of valence (from unpleasant to pleasant) and arousal (from calm to activated) dimensions for COVID-19-related words (e.g., hospital) and COVID-19-unrelated words (e.g., whale). Subsequently, we compared these scores with those from databases that reported ratings for the same pool of words before the pandemic. Our results showed significant changes in arousal for COVID-19-related words but not unrelated words, thus revealing that the pandemic social context modified their affective representation. These findings support the flexibility of emotional representations and the malleability and dynamicity of the mental lexicon as a function of contextual factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clara Planchuelo
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| | - Ana Baciero
- Faculty of Science and Technology, Bournemouth University, Dorset, United Kingdom
| | - José Antonio Hinojosa
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain; Instituto Pluridisciplinar, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain; Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Procesos Cognitivos y Logopedia, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Manuel Perea
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain; Departamento de Metodología and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| | - Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain; Department of Languages and Culture, The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Ballot C, Robert C, Mathey S. Word imageability influences the emotionality effect in episodic memory. Cogn Process 2022; 23:655-660. [PMID: 35857171 PMCID: PMC9553820 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-022-01102-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2022] [Accepted: 06/16/2022] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
This study examines how and to what extent imageability influences the effect of word emotionality in episodic memory. A total of 52 young adults successively performed a free recall task and a recognition task in which word emotionality and imageability were orthogonally manipulated across six conditions of French words: low-imageability positive words (e.g., éloge [praise]), low-imageability negative words (e.g., viral [viral]), low-imageability neutral words (e.g., global [global]), high-imageability positive words (e.g., ourson [teddy]), high-imageability negative words (e.g., tornade [tornado]), and low-imageability neutral words (e.g., noyau [core]). The results from both the recall and the recognition memory tasks show that word imageability enhances memory performance. Importantly, word imageability interacted with word emotionality in both tasks. Specifically, we found that the advantage of emotional over neutral words in episodic memory performance emerged for high-imageability words only, as did the advantage of positive over negative words. These results highlight the role of imageability in the mechanisms underlying emotional word episodic memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claire Ballot
- University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France. .,Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva, Boulevard du Pont-d'Arve 40, 1211, Geneva 4, Switzerland.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Age-related evaluations of imageability and subjective frequency for 1286 neutral and emotional French words: ratings by young, middle-aged, and older adults. Behav Res Methods 2021; 54:196-215. [PMID: 34131872 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-021-01621-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/14/2021] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
This study aimed at providing imageability and subjective frequency ratings collected from four adult age groups ranging from 18 to 85 years old (18-25; 26-39; 40-59; and 60 and over) for 1286 neutral and emotional French words available in the EMA database (Gobin et al., 2017). Overall, the older adults rated words as more (subjectively) frequent and more imageable than the younger adults. Furthermore, we examined the relationships between subjective frequency and imageability, as well as those with emotional variables (i.e., valence, arousal) already available for these words, for each age group. For all age groups, more subjective frequent words were more imageable. Emotional words were more imageable and more frequent. Arousal scores were lower for low- and high-imageability words, and higher for more subjective frequent words. The strength of these links between subjective frequency, imageability, and emotional ratings was found to decrease as a function of age. Finally, by using the lexical decision reaction times and accuracy rates of young adults from Megalex (Ferrand et al., 2018), imageability and subjective frequency across age were found to provide an additional contribution to visual word recognition performance as compared to objective lexical variables (i.e., number of letters, syllables, objective frequency, orthographic neighborhood). More importantly, subjective frequency and imageability ratings from the youngest group predicted reaction times and accuracy better than ratings from the oldest group. By providing new age-adapted word characteristics, this norm should be of great use to researchers in the field of cognitive aging who use word materials.
Collapse
|
13
|
Negative emotions influence EEG correlates of inference formation during analogical reasoning. Int J Psychophysiol 2021; 162:49-59. [PMID: 33549608 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2020] [Revised: 12/02/2020] [Accepted: 02/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Previous research indicates that negative emotions influence cognitive resource utilization during analogical reasoning. However, no research has yet demonstrated an influence of negative emotions on inference formation during analogical reasoning. For this reason, we used evoked response potentials to investigate how negatively valenced content affects inference formation during analogical reasoning. Participants generated inferences about the missing term of 256 four-term analogies consisting of a first pair (A is to B), a second incomplete pair (as C is to?), and a probe term (D). We manipulated the affective valence of the terms (negative/neutral) forming the first two pairs and the soundness of the analogies. In Experiment 1, the terms were words and the relations were semantic in nature. We recorded the N400 event-related component time-locked to the probe term. The effect of analogy soundness on N400 amplitude was weaker when both pairs of terms were negative than when one or both pairs were neutral. In Experiment 2, we used analogies with negatively or neutrally conditioned symbols as terms, and visuospatial transformations as relations. We recorded the P3b event-related component time-locked to the final term of the analogy. The effect of analogy soundness on P3b amplitude was weaker when the first pair of terms was negatively conditioned than when they were neutrally conditioned. Results of both experiments suggested that negatively valenced content impairs the formation of inferences during analogical reasoning, as indicated by reduced effects of analogy soundness on N400 and P3b in the presence of negatively valenced content.
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
The present study develops key research for French word norms that combines the predominant theories of dimensional and discrete (or categorical) emotions. As a result, we provide the database FANCat, affective norms for a set of 1031 French words on ten discrete emotion categories: fear, anger, disgust, sadness, anxiety, awe, excitement, contentment, amusement, and serenity. FANCat complements a previous word set, FAN, which provides only the dimensional norms, valence, and arousal (Monnier & Syssau, 2014). Herein, we introduce five discrete positive emotions in efforts to differentiate positive emotions at higher resolution and specificity. Although ten emotional categories were considered in FANCat norms, results showed a high degree of inter-rater reliability and a good external validity. Then, distributional analyses of words into the ten emotion categories revealed that positive words evoked mainly the emotions awe, contentment, and amusement, and principally evoked either one positive emotion only ("pure" words) or two (mixed words). This study contributes to a deeper understanding of the relationship between language, and negative and positive emotions. It is also currently the only norms database in French that analyses ten discrete emotions as well as including valence and arousal. FANCat is available at https://www.researchgate.net/publication/338622765_FANCat_database .
Collapse
|
15
|
Concreteness norms for 1,659 French words: Relationships with other psycholinguistic variables and word recognition times. Behav Res Methods 2019; 50:2366-2387. [PMID: 29435912 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-018-1014-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Words that correspond to a potential sensory experience-concrete words-have long been found to possess a processing advantage over abstract words in various lexical tasks. We collected norms of concreteness for a set of 1,659 French words, together with other psycholinguistic norms that were not available for these words-context availability, emotional valence, and arousal-but which are important if we are to achieve a better understanding of the meaning of concreteness effects. We then investigated the relationships of concreteness with these newly collected variables, together with other psycholinguistic variables that were already available for this set of words (e.g., imageability, age of acquisition, and sensory experience ratings). Finally, thanks to the variety of psychological norms available for this set of words, we decided to test further the embodied account of concreteness effects in visual-word recognition, championed by Kousta, Vigliocco, Vinson, Andrews, and Del Campo (Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 14-34, 2011). Similarly, we investigated the influences of concreteness in three word recognition tasks-lexical decision, progressive demasking, and word naming-using a multiple regression approach, based on the reaction times available in Chronolex (Ferrand, Brysbaert, Keuleers, New, Bonin, Méot, Pallier, Frontiers in Psychology, 2; 306, 2011). The norms can be downloaded as supplementary material provided with this article.
Collapse
|
16
|
Grosselin A, Sevos J, Gay A, Pellet J, Massoubre C. Des mots à la bouche : évaluations subjectives des valences émotionnelle et hédonique de la valeur nutritive et de la familiarité de 201 noms d’aliments dans une population française. ANNEE PSYCHOLOGIQUE 2018. [DOI: 10.3917/anpsy1.182.0099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
|
17
|
Dupart M, Auzou N, Mathey S. Emotional valence impacts lexical activation and inhibition differently in aging: an emotional Hayling task investigation. Exp Aging Res 2018; 44:206-220. [PMID: 29589788 DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2018.1449587] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Aging is characterized by cognitive changes such as a potential inhibition deficit. However, growing evidence shows that positive valence stimuli enhance performances in older adults to a greater degree than in younger adults. The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of the emotional valence of words on lexical activation and inhibition in aging by using a new Emotional Hayling Task. METHODS Thirty-eight younger adults (mean age = 20.11 years) and 38 older adults (mean age = 66.47 years) performed a computerized Emotional Hayling task. Participants had to choose the correct (initiation part) or incorrect (inhibition part) final words of highly predictable incomplete sentences. Final words had a negative or positive emotional valence and were paired for reaction time comparison with neutral words. RESULTS Response times were faster in younger adults than in older adults in both the initiation and the inhibition parts. In addition, response times indicated that older adults initiated more slowly negative than neutral words while no differences emerged in inhibition. No differences were obtained between negative and neutral words in younger adults. Response times showed faster initiation and inhibition for positive than for neutral words in both age groups. CONCLUSION These data are consistent with previous findings suggesting a disengagement from the processing of negative versus neutral words in older adults when compared with younger adults. A possible explanation is that activation of negative words in the mental lexicon is weaker in older than in younger adults. Conversely, the positive valence of words seems to enhance both activation and inhibition processes in both young and older adults. These findings suggest that positive stimuli can improve performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marcellin Dupart
- a Laboratoire de Psychologie, Laboratoire de Psychologie , Université de Bordeaux, Laboratoire de Psychologie , France
| | - Nicolas Auzou
- a Laboratoire de Psychologie, Laboratoire de Psychologie , Université de Bordeaux, Laboratoire de Psychologie , France.,b Centre Expert Parkinson, Institut des Maladies Neurodégénératives cliniques, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Bordeau , Bordeaux , France
| | - Stéphanie Mathey
- a Laboratoire de Psychologie, Laboratoire de Psychologie , Université de Bordeaux, Laboratoire de Psychologie , France
| |
Collapse
|