1
|
How variability shapes learning and generalization. Trends Cogn Sci 2022; 26:462-483. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.03.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2020] [Revised: 03/18/2022] [Accepted: 03/21/2022] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
|
2
|
Frye CI, Creel SC. Perceptual flexibility in word learning: Preschoolers learn words with speech sound variability. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2022; 226:105078. [PMID: 35074621 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2022.105078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2020] [Revised: 12/21/2021] [Accepted: 01/03/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
Children's language input is rife with acoustic variability. Much of this variability may facilitate learning by highlighting unvarying, criterial speech attributes. But in many cases, learners experience variation in those criterial attributes themselves, as when hearing speakers with different accents. How flexible are children in the face of this variability? The current study taught 3-5-year-olds new words containing speech-sound variability: a single picture might be labeled both deev and teev. After learning, children's knowledge was tested by presenting two pictures and asking them to point to one. Picture-pointing accuracy and eye movements were tracked. While children pointed less accurately and looked less rapidly to dual-label than single-label words, they robustly exceeded chance. Performance was weaker when children learned two distinct labels, such as vayfe and fosh, for a single object. Findings suggest moderate learning even with speech-sound variability. One implication is that neural representations of speech contain rich gradient information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Conor I Frye
- Department of Cognitive Science, UC San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | - Sarah C Creel
- Department of Cognitive Science, UC San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sučević J, Althaus N, Plunkett K. Discovering category boundaries: The role of comparison in infants' novel category learning. INFANCY 2022; 27:533-554. [PMID: 35099114 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12459] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2020] [Revised: 10/04/2021] [Accepted: 01/05/2022] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
A key question in categorization is how infants extract regularities from the exemplars they encounter. Detecting similarities and dissimilarities across items is vital in order to determine category-relevant features. Previous research found evidence that infants acquire a single category more easily with paired presentations in comparison with single presentations (Oakes & Ribar, 2005, Infancy, 7, 85; Oakes & Kovack-Lesh, 2007, Cogniție, Creier, Comportament / Cognition, Brain, Behavior, XI, 661). Here, we focus on infants' acquisition of a category contrast, that is, when they are exposed to two categories. In an eye-tracking study, we examined 10-month-old infants' ability to learn two novel visual categories when presented with one item at a time and with items in pairs. Infants were familiarized with pairs of items from the same category or with pairs of items from different categories (cross-category pairs). Using a linear model with a priori contrasts, we show that infants' learning is directly related to the opportunity for category comparison: There is no evidence of category learning in the single-item condition, improved performance when familiarized with same-category pairs, and finally robust category learning when familiarized with cross-category pairs. We conclude that comparison which involves items from different categories promotes category formation, by highlighting differences and promoting a discovery of category boundaries.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jelena Sučević
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Nadja Althaus
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
| | - Kim Plunkett
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ruba AL, Meltzoff AN, Repacholi BM. Linguistic and developmental influences on superordinate facial configuration categorization in infancy. INFANCY 2021; 26:857-876. [PMID: 34418252 PMCID: PMC8530983 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12430] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2021] [Revised: 07/09/2021] [Accepted: 07/30/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Humans perceive emotions in terms of categories, such as "happiness," "sadness," and "anger." To learn these complex conceptual emotion categories, humans must first be able to perceive regularities in expressive behaviors (e.g., facial configurations) across individuals. Recent research suggests that infants spontaneously form "basic-level" categories of facial configurations (e.g., happy vs. fear), but not "superordinate" categories of facial configurations (e.g., positive vs. negative). The current studies further explore how infant age and language impact superordinate categorization of facial configurations associated with different negative emotions. Across all experiments, infants were habituated to one person displaying facial configurations associated with anger and disgust. While 10-month-olds formed a category of person identity (Experiment 1), 14-month-olds formed a category that included negative facial configurations displayed by the same person (Experiment 2). However, neither age formed the hypothesized superordinate category of negative valence. When a verbal label ("toma") was added to each of the habituation events (Experiment 3), 10-month-olds formed a category similar to 14-month-olds in Experiment 2. These findings intersect a larger conversation about the nature and development of children's emotion categories and highlight the importance of considering developmental processes, such as language learning and attentional/memory development, in the design and interpretation of infant categorization studies.
Collapse
|
5
|
One versus many: Multiple examples in word learning. J Exp Child Psychol 2021; 209:105173. [PMID: 34000590 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105173] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2020] [Revised: 03/19/2021] [Accepted: 03/24/2021] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
A large body of research indicates that children can map words to categories and generalize the label to new instances of the category after hearing a single instance of the category labeled. Additional research demonstrates that word learning is enhanced when children are presented with multiple instances of a category through comparison or contrast. In this study, 3-year-old children participated in a novel noun generalization task in which a label was given for either (a) a single instance of a category, (b) multiple instances of a category, or (c) contrasting a category instance with non-category members. Children were asked to extend the label to a new category at test either immediately (Study 1) or after a 10-s delay (Study 2). The results indicate that when tested immediately, children who heard a single instance labeled outperformed children who were presented with multiple instances. However, when tested after a brief delay, there was no difference among the conditions.
Collapse
|
6
|
Althaus N, Gliozzi V, Mayor J, Plunkett K. Infant categorization as a dynamic process linked to memory. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2020; 7:200328. [PMID: 33204445 PMCID: PMC7657915 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.200328] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2020] [Accepted: 09/29/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Recency effects are well documented in the adult and infant literature: recognition and recall memory are better for recently occurring events. We explore recency effects in infant categorization, which does not merely involve memory for individual items, but the formation of abstract category representations. We present a computational model of infant categorization that simulates category learning in 10-month-olds. The model predicts that recency effects outweigh previously reported order effects for the same stimuli. According to the model, infant behaviour at test should depend mainly on the identity of the most recent training item. We evaluate these predictions in a series of experiments with 10-month-old infants. Our results show that infant behaviour confirms the model's prediction. In particular, at test infants exhibited a preference for a category outlier over the category average only if the final training item had been close to the average, rather than distant from it. Our results are consistent with a view of categorization as a highly dynamic process where the end result of category learning is not the overall average of all stimuli encountered, but rather a fluid representation that moves depending on moment-to-moment novelty. We argue that this is a desirable property of a flexible cognitive system that adapts rapidly to different contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nadja Althaus
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich Research Park, Norwich NR4 7TJ, UK
| | - Valentina Gliozzi
- Center for Logic, Language, and Cognition, Department of Computer Science, University of Torino, C.so Svizzera 185, 10149 Torino, Italy
| | - Julien Mayor
- Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, PO Box 1094, Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway
| | - Kim Plunkett
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
The development of categorisation and conceptual thinking in early childhood: methods and limitations. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2020; 33:17. [PMID: 32700155 PMCID: PMC7377002 DOI: 10.1186/s41155-020-00154-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2019] [Accepted: 07/05/2020] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
We present a systematic and qualitative review of academic literature on early conceptual development (0–24 months of age), with an emphasis on methodological aspects. The final sample of our review included 281 studies reported in 115 articles. The main aims of the article were four: first, to organise studies into sets according to methodological similarities and differences; second, to elaborate on the methodological procedures that characterise each set; third, to circumscribe the empirical indicators that different sets of studies consider as proof of the existence of concepts in early childhood; last, to identify methodological limitations and to propose possible ways to overcome them. We grouped the studies into five sets: preference and habituation experiments, category extension tasks, object sorting tasks, sequential touching tasks and object examination tasks. In the “Results” section, we review the core features of each set of studies. In the “Discussion” and “Conclusions” sections, we describe, for one thing, the most relevant methodological shortcomings. We end by arguing that a situated, semiotic and pragmatic perspective that emphasises the importance of ecological validity could open up new avenues of research to better understand the development of concepts in early childhood.
Collapse
|
8
|
Quinn PC, Lee K, Pascalis O. Beyond perceptual development: Infant responding to social categories. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2020; 58:35-61. [PMID: 32169198 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2020.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
A body of research is reviewed that has investigated how infants respond to social category information in faces based on differential experience. Whereas some aspects of behavioral performance (visual preference, discrimination, and scanning) are consistent with traditional models of perceptual development (induction, maintenance, and attunement), other aspects (category formation, association with valence, and selective learning) suggest the need for an account that links perceptual with social-emotional processing. We also consider how responding to social categories in infancy may anticipate subsequent responding to these categories in childhood and adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul C Quinn
- University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States.
| | - Kang Lee
- University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Taxitari L, Twomey KE, Westermann G, Mani N. The Limits of Infants' Early Word Learning. LANGUAGE LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT 2019; 16:1-21. [PMID: 32256251 PMCID: PMC7077354 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2019.1670184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
In this series of experiments, we tested the limits of young infants' word learning and generalization abilities in light of recent findings reporting sophisticated word learning abilities in the first year of life. Ten-month-old infants were trained with two word-object pairs and tested with either the same or different members of the corresponding categories. In Experiment 1, infants showed successful learning of the word-object associations, when trained and tested with a single exemplar from each category. In Experiment 2, infants were presented with multiple within-category items during training but failed to learn the word-object associations. In Experiment 3, infants were presented with a single exemplar from each category during training, and failed to generalize words to a new category exemplar. However, when infants were trained with items from perceptually and conceptually distinct categories in Experiment 4, they showed weak evidence for generalization of words to novel members of the corresponding categories. It is suggested that word learning in the first year begins as the formation of simple associations between words and objects that become enriched as experience with objects, words and categories accumulates across development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Loukia Taxitari
- Department of Secondary General Education, Ministry of Education and Culture, Nicosia, Cyprus
| | - Katherine E. Twomey
- Division of Human Communication, Development and Hearing, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Gert Westermann
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
| | - Nivedita Mani
- Georg-Elias-Müller Institute for Psychology, University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
- Leibniz ScienceCampus Primate Cognition, Göttingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Twomey KE, Westermann G. Curiosity-based learning in infants: a neurocomputational approach. Dev Sci 2018; 21:e12629. [PMID: 29071759 PMCID: PMC6032944 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12629] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2016] [Accepted: 09/05/2017] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Infants are curious learners who drive their own cognitive development by imposing structure on their learning environment as they explore. Understanding the mechanisms by which infants structure their own learning is therefore critical to our understanding of development. Here we propose an explicit mechanism for intrinsically motivated information selection that maximizes learning. We first present a neurocomputational model of infant visual category learning, capturing existing empirical data on the role of environmental complexity on learning. Next we "set the model free", allowing it to select its own stimuli based on a formalization of curiosity and three alternative selection mechanisms. We demonstrate that maximal learning emerges when the model is able to maximize stimulus novelty relative to its internal states, depending on the interaction across learning between the structure of the environment and the plasticity in the learner itself. We discuss the implications of this new curiosity mechanism for both existing computational models of reinforcement learning and for our understanding of this fundamental mechanism in early development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine E. Twomey
- Division of Human CommunicationDevelopment and HearingSchool of Health SciencesUniversity of ManchesterManchesterUK
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Vukatana E, Graham SA, Curtin S, Zepeda MS. One is Not Enough: Multiple Exemplars Facilitate Infants' Generalizations of Novel Properties. INFANCY 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/infa.12092] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
12
|
The development of voicing categories: a quantitative review of over 40 years of infant speech perception research. Psychon Bull Rev 2015; 21:884-906. [PMID: 24550074 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0569-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Most research on infant speech categories has relied on measures of discrimination. Such work often employs categorical perception as a linking hypothesis to enable inferences about categorization on the basis of discrimination measures. However, a large number of studies with adults challenge the utility of categorical perception in describing adult speech perception, and this in turn calls into question how to interpret measures of infant speech discrimination. We propose here a parallel channels model of discrimination (built on Pisoni and Tash Perception & Psychophysics, 15(2), 285-290, 1974), which posits that both a noncategorical or veridical encoding of speech cues and category representations can simultaneously contribute to discrimination. This can thus produce categorical perception effects without positing any warping of the acoustic signal, but it also reframes how we think about infant discrimination and development. We test this model by conducting a quantitative review of 20 studies examining infants' discrimination of voice onset time contrasts. This review suggests that within-category discrimination is surprisingly prevalent even in classic studies and that, averaging across studies, discrimination is related to continuous acoustic distance. It also identifies several methodological factors that may mask our ability to see this. Finally, it suggests that infant discrimination may improve over development, contrary to commonly held notion of perceptual narrowing. These results are discussed in terms of theories of speech development that may require such continuous sensitivity.
Collapse
|
13
|
Horst JS, Simmering VR. Category learning in a dynamic world. Front Psychol 2015; 6:46. [PMID: 25688224 PMCID: PMC4311603 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2014] [Accepted: 01/09/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica S. Horst
- Word Lab, School of Psychology, University of SussexBrighton, UK
| | - Vanessa R. Simmering
- Space Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin—MadisonMadison, WI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Array heterogeneity prevents catastrophic forgetting in infants. Cognition 2014; 136:365-80. [PMID: 25543889 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2013] [Revised: 11/25/2014] [Accepted: 11/28/2014] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Working memory is limited in adults and infants. But unlike adults, infants whose working memory capacity is exceeded often fail in a particularly striking way: they do not represent any of the presented objects, rather than simply remembering as many objects as they can and ignoring anything further (Feigenson & Carey, 2003, 2005). Here we explored the nature of this "catastrophic forgetting," asking whether stimuli themselves modulate the way in which infants' memory fails. We showed 13-month old infants object arrays that either were within or that exceeded working memory capacity--but, unlike previous experiments, presented objects with contrasting features. Although previous studies have repeatedly documented infants' failure to represent four identical hidden objects, in Experiments 1 and 2 we found that infants who saw four contrasting objects hidden, and then retrieved just two of the four, successfully continued searching for the missing objects. Perceptual contrast between objects sufficed to drive this success; infants succeeded regardless of whether the different objects were contrastively labeled, and regardless of whether the objects were semantically familiar or completely novel. In Experiment 3 we explored the nature of this surprising success, asking whether array heterogeneity actually expanded infants' working memory capacity or rather prevented catastrophic forgetting. We found that infants successfully continued searching after seeing four contrasting objects hidden and retrieving two of them, but not after retrieving three of them. This suggests that, like adults, infants were able to remember up to, but not beyond, the limits of their working memory capacity when representing heterogeneous arrays.
Collapse
|
15
|
Althaus N, Mareschal D. Labels direct infants' attention to commonalities during novel category learning. PLoS One 2014; 9:e99670. [PMID: 25014254 PMCID: PMC4094422 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099670] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2013] [Accepted: 05/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent studies have provided evidence that labeling can influence the outcome of infants' visual categorization. However, what exactly happens during learning remains unclear. Using eye-tracking, we examined infants' attention to object parts during learning. Our analysis of looking behaviors during learning provide insights going beyond merely observing the learning outcome. Both labeling and non-labeling phrases facilitated category formation in 12-month-olds but not 8-month-olds (Experiment 1). Non-linguistic sounds did not produce this effect (Experiment 2). Detailed analyses of infants' looking patterns during learning revealed that only infants who heard labels exhibited a rapid focus on the object part successive exemplars had in common. Although other linguistic stimuli may also be beneficial for learning, it is therefore concluded that labels have a unique impact on categorization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nadja Althaus
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Denis Mareschal
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Twomey KE, Lush L, Pearce R, Horst JS. Visual variability affects early verb learning. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2014; 32:359-66. [DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2013] [Revised: 03/10/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Lauren Lush
- School of Psychology; University of Sussex; UK
| | - Ruth Pearce
- School of Psychology; University of Sussex; UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Twomey KE, Ranson SL, Horst JS. That's More Like It: Multiple Exemplars Facilitate Word Learning. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2013. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.1824] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
18
|
Perone S, Spencer JP. Autonomy in action: linking the act of looking to memory formation in infancy via dynamic neural fields. Cogn Sci 2013; 37:1-60. [PMID: 23136815 PMCID: PMC3815444 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Looking is a fundamental exploratory behavior by which infants acquire knowledge about the world. In theories of infant habituation, however, looking as an exploratory behavior has been deemphasized relative to the reliable nature with which looking indexes active cognitive processing. We present a new theory that connects looking to the dynamics of memory formation and formally implement this theory in a Dynamic Neural Field model that learns autonomously as it actively looks and looks away from a stimulus. We situate this model in a habituation task and illustrate the mechanisms by which looking, encoding, working memory formation, and long-term memory formation give rise to habituation across multiple stimulus and task contexts. We also illustrate how the act of looking and the temporal dynamics of learning affect each other. Finally, we test a new hypothesis about the sources of developmental differences in looking.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sammy Perone
- Department of Psychology and Delta Center, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Meltzoff AN, Waismeyer A, Gopnik A. Learning about causes from people: observational causal learning in 24-month-old infants. Dev Psychol 2012; 48:1215-28. [PMID: 22369335 PMCID: PMC3649070 DOI: 10.1037/a0027440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
How do infants and young children learn about the causal structure of the world around them? In 4 experiments we investigate whether young children initially give special weight to the outcomes of goal-directed interventions they see others perform and use this to distinguish correlations from genuine causal relations--observational causal learning. In a new 2-choice procedure, 2- to 4-year-old children saw 2 identical objects (potential causes). Activation of 1 but not the other triggered a spatially remote effect. Children systematically intervened on the causal object and predictively looked to the effect. Results fell to chance when the cause and effect were temporally reversed, so that the events were merely associated but not causally related. The youngest children (24- to 36-month-olds) were more likely to make causal inferences when covariations were the outcome of human interventions than when they were not. Observational causal learning may be a fundamental learning mechanism that enables infants to abstract the causal structure of the world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew N Meltzoff
- Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Box 357920, Seattle, WA 98195, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
A crucial part of language development is learning how various social and contextual language-external factors constrain an utterance's meaning. This learning process is poorly understood. Five experiments addressed one hundred thirty-one 3- to 5-year-old children's use of one such socially relevant information source: talker characteristics. Participants learned 2 characters' favorite colors; then, those characters asked participants to select colored shapes, as eye movements were tracked. Results suggest that by preschool, children use voice characteristics predictively to constrain a talker's domain of reference, visually fixating the talker's preferred color shapes. Indicating flexibility, children used talker information when the talker made a request for herself but not when she made a request for the other character. Children's ease at using voice characteristics and possible developmental changes are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah C Creel
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla,CA 92093-0515., USA.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Comparison Versus Contrast: Task Specifics Affect Category Acquisition. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2012. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.1764] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
22
|
Using a single feature to discriminate and form categories: the interaction between color, form and exemplar number. Infant Behav Dev 2012; 35:348-59. [PMID: 22721735 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2011] [Revised: 03/18/2012] [Accepted: 04/04/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
There is relatively little work that has focused on how infants use a single feature to discriminate objects or flexibly group objects together. Existing research suggests that the ease with which infants learn form and color discriminations is not equal. However, which of these dimensions is easier when discriminating between objects is still unclear. The studies in this paper tested how infants used these two dimensions under varying levels of diversity in a discrimination task. Combining traditional analyses with latent-states Markov-modeling, infant learning in these studies was characterized by a bend of overt behavior and attentional processes. Infants were able to learn both a color and form-based discrimination, but only generalized the form distinction to new stimuli. When presented with diversity on the irrelevant dimension, infants in the form condition learned quickly. However, infants in the color-condition did not show signs that they learned the distinction. The results show that infants could use both dimensions to distinguish between objects, but that form-based distinctions were easier and more likely to be generalized to new objects.
Collapse
|
23
|
Ropeter A, Pauen S. Relating 7-Month-Olds Visuo-Spatial Working Memory to Other Basic Mental Skills Assessed With Two Different Versions of the Habituation-Dishabituation Paradigm. INFANCY 2012. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00133.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
24
|
Arterberry ME, Bornstein MH. Categorization of real and replica objects by 14- and 18-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 2012; 35:606-12. [PMID: 22742988 PMCID: PMC3496250 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.04.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2011] [Revised: 02/17/2012] [Accepted: 04/30/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
In studies of infant categorization (especially sequential touching), replicas of real objects are often utilized on the assumption that infants treat replicas as they do reals. Do infants categorize replicas of objects in the same way as they categorize real objects? This question was addressed in a sequential touching task, where 14- and 18-month-olds were presented with four sets of objects: real telephones and hairbrushes, real lemons and pears, replica telephones and hairbrushes, and replica lemons and pears. On the whole, mean run length, number of contacts, and appropriate actions did not differ between real and replica objects. Moreover, mean run length was significantly greater than chance for telephones and hairbrushes, but not for lemons and pears; both ages categorized the former sets but not the latter. Infants of this age appear to treat replicas as equivalent to reals, and replicas are appropriate stimuli to use in the sequential touching procedure to address questions of infants' categorization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Marc H. Bornstein
- Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Public Health Service
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Childers JB, Heard ME, Ring K, Pai A, Sallquist J. Children Use Different Cues to Guide Noun and Verb Extensions. LANGUAGE LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT 2012; 8:233-254. [PMID: 24465184 PMCID: PMC3898709 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2011.585285] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Learning new words involves decoding both how a word fits the current situation and how it could be used in new situations. Three studies explore how two types of cues- sentence structure and the availability of multiple instances-- affect children's extensions of nouns and verbs. In each study, 2½-year-olds heard nouns, verbs or no new word while seeing the experimenter use a novel object to perform an action; at test, they were asked to extend the word. In Study 1, children hearing nouns in simple sentences used object shape as the basis for extension even though, during the learning phase, they saw multiple objects in motion; children in the other conditions responded randomly. Study 2 shows that by changing in the type of sentences used in the noun and verb conditions, not only is the shape bias disrupted but children are successful in extending new verbs. In a final study, access to multiple examples was replaced by a direct teaching context, and produced findings similar to those in Study 2. An implication of this result is that seeing multiple examples can be as effective as receiving direct instruction from an adult. Overall, the set of results suggests the mix of cues available during learning influences noun and verb extensions differently. The findings are important for understanding how the ability to extend words emerges in complex contexts.
Collapse
|
26
|
Vlach HA, Ankowski AA, Sandhofer CM. At the same time or apart in time? The role of presentation timing and retrieval dynamics in generalization. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2012; 38:246-54. [PMID: 21895392 PMCID: PMC3302959 DOI: 10.1037/a0025260] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Several bodies of research have found different results with regard to presentation timing, categorization, and generalization. Both presenting instances at the same time (simultaneous) and presenting instances apart in time (spacing) have been shown to facilitate generalization. In this study, we resolved these results by examining simultaneous, massed, and spaced presentations in 2-year-old children's (N = 144) immediate and long-term performance on a novel noun generalization task. Results revealed that, when tested immediately, children in the simultaneous condition outperformed children in all other conditions. However, when tested after 15 min, children in the spaced condition outperformed children in all other conditions. Results are discussed in terms of how retrieval dynamics during learning affect abstraction, retention, and generalization across time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Haley A Vlach
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Kovack-Lesh KA, Oakes LM, McMurray B. Contributions of attentional style and previous experience to 4-month-old infants' categorization. INFANCY 2011; 17:324-338. [PMID: 22523478 DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00073.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
We examined how infants' categorization is jointly influenced by previous experience and how much they shift their gaze back-and-forth between stimuli. Extending previous findings reported by Kovack-Lesh, Horst, and Oakes (2008), we found that 4-month-old infants' (N = 122) learning of the exclusive category of cats was related to whether they had cats at home and how much they shifted attention between two available stimuli during familiarization. Individual differences in attention assessed in an unrelated task were not related to their categorization. Thus, infants' learning is multiply influenced by past experience and on-line attentional style.
Collapse
|
28
|
Arterberry ME, Bornstein MH, Haynes OM. Assessing categorization performance at the individual level: a comparison of Monte Carlo simulation and probability estimate model procedures. Infant Behav Dev 2011; 34:321-6. [PMID: 21402410 PMCID: PMC3426788 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2010] [Revised: 09/10/2010] [Accepted: 02/03/2011] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Two analytical procedures for identifying young children as categorizers, the Monte Carlo Simulation and the Probability Estimate Model, were compared. Using a sequential touching method, children aged 12, 18, 24, and 30 months were given seven object sets representing different levels of categorical classification. From their touching performance, the probability that children were categorizing was then determined independently using Monte Carlo Simulation and the Probability Estimate Model. The two analytical procedures resulted in different percentages of children being classified as categorizers. Results using the Monte Carlo Simulation were more consistent with group-level analyses than results using the Probability Estimate Model. These findings recommend using the Monte Carlo Simulation for determining individual categorizer classification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martha E Arterberry
- Department of Psychology, Colby College, 5553 Mayflower Hill, Waterville, ME 04901, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Mather E, Plunkett K. Same items, different order: effects of temporal variability on infant categorization. Cognition 2011; 119:438-47. [PMID: 21382616 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2010] [Revised: 02/06/2011] [Accepted: 02/11/2011] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
How does variability between members of a category influence infants' category learning? We explore the impact of the order in which different items are sampled on category formation. Two groups of 10-months-olds were presented with a series of exemplars to be organized into a single category. In a low distance group, the order of presentation minimized the perceptual distance between consecutive exemplars. In a high distance group, the order of presentation maximized the distance between successive exemplars. At test, only infants in the High Distance condition reliably discriminated between the category prototype and an atypical exemplar. Hence, the order in which infants learnt about the exemplars impacted their categorization performance. Our findings demonstrate the importance of moment-to-moment variations in similarity during infants' category learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Mather
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, United Kingdom.
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Cicchino JB, Aslin RN, Rakison DH. Correspondences between what infants see and know about causal and self-propelled motion. Cognition 2011; 118:171-92. [PMID: 21122832 PMCID: PMC3038602 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/12/2010] [Revised: 11/05/2010] [Accepted: 11/08/2010] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
The associative learning account of how infants identify human motion rests on the assumption that this knowledge is derived from statistical regularities seen in the world. Yet, no catalog exists of what visual input infants receive of human motion, and of causal and self-propelled motion in particular. In this manuscript, we demonstrate that the frequency with which causal agency and self-propelled motion appear in the visual environment predicts infants' understanding of these motions. In an observational study, an infant wearing a head-mounted camera saw people act as agents in causal events three times more often than he saw people engaged in self-propelled motion. Subsequent experiments with the habituation paradigm revealed that infants begin to generalize self-propulsion to agents in causal events between 10 and 14 months of age. However, infants cannot generalize causal agency to a self-propelled object at 14 or 18 months unless the object exhibits additional cues to animacy. The results are discussed within a domain-general framework of learning about human action.
Collapse
|
31
|
Hurley KB, Kovack-Lesh KA, Oakes LM. The influence of pets on infants' processing of cat and dog images. Infant Behav Dev 2010; 33:619-28. [PMID: 20728223 PMCID: PMC2991405 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2010] [Revised: 05/18/2010] [Accepted: 07/16/2010] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
We examined how experience at home with pets is related to infants' processing of animal stimuli in a standard laboratory procedure. We presented 6-month-old infants with photographs of cats or dogs and found that infants with pets at home (N=40) responded differently to the pictures than infants without pets (N=40). These results suggest that infants' experience in one context (at home) contributes to their processing of similar stimuli in a different context (the laboratory), and have implications for how infants' early experience shapes basic cognitive processing.
Collapse
|
32
|
Booth AE, Schuler K, Zajicek R. Specifying the role of function in infant categorization. Infant Behav Dev 2010; 33:672-84. [PMID: 20951437 PMCID: PMC2997880 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2010] [Revised: 05/11/2010] [Accepted: 09/01/2010] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Research demonstrates that object functions facilitate artifact categorization in infancy. To explicate the nature and magnitude of this effect, 16-month-olds participated in three studies. In Experiment 1, categorization was facilitated more by object functions than by distinctive motions, suggesting that the motion properties of function cannot fully explain its influence. In Experiment 2, infants failed to categorize when each category exemplar performed a different function, thus revealing the importance of shared functionality in facilitating categorization. In Experiment 3, infants were tested after each new exemplar was introduced. When object functions were provided during training, infants were more likely to appropriately extend the novel categories on the very first trial. This suggests that function reduces the need for exposure to multiple exemplars in forming categories. Together, these findings confirm the conceptual nature of the facilitative effect of function on early categorization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amy E Booth
- Roxelyn and Richard Pepper Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60028, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Andrews JK, Livingston KR, Kurtz KJ. Category learning in the context of co-presented items. Cogn Process 2010; 12:161-75. [PMID: 21080030 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-010-0377-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2010] [Accepted: 10/22/2010] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
A series of four studies explore how the presentation of multiple items on each trial of a categorization task affects the course of category learning. In a three-category supervised classification task involving multi-dimensionally varying artificial organism-like stimuli, learners are shown a target plus two context items on every trial, with the context items' category membership explicitly identified. These triads vary in whether one, two, or all three categories are represented. This presentation context can support within-category comparison and/or between-category contrast. The most successful learning occurs when all categories are represented in each trial. This pattern occurs across two different underlying category structures and across variations in learners' prior knowledge of the relationship between the target and context items. These results appear to contrast with some other recent findings and make clear the potential importance of context-based inter-item evaluation in human category learning, which has implications for psychological theory and for real-world learning environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Janet K Andrews
- Vassar College, 124 Raymond Ave., Box 146, Poughkeepsie, NY 12604-0146, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Abstract
Research indicates that object perception involves the decomposition of images into parts. A critical principle that governs part decomposition by adults is the short-cut rule, which states that, all else being equal, the visual system parses objects using the shortest possible cuts. We examined whether 6.5-month-olds' parsing of images also follows the short-cut rule. Infants in the experimental conditions were habituated to cross shapes and then tested for their preference between segregated patterns produced using long cuts versus short cuts. Infants in the control conditions were directly tested with the segregated patterns. Infants in the experimental conditions exhibited a greater novelty preference for the long-cut over the short-cut patterns than did those in the control conditions, thereby indicating that they are more likely to segregate cross shapes using short cuts rather than long cuts. This sensitivity to the short-cut rule was evident when two alternative parameters, part area and protrusion, were controlled in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. Thus, a critical principle that governs part segregation in adulthood is operational by 6.5 months of age.
Collapse
|
35
|
Bornstein MH, Arterberry ME. The development of object categorization in young children: hierarchical inclusiveness, age, perceptual attribute, and group versus individual analyses. Dev Psychol 2010; 46:350-65. [PMID: 20210495 PMCID: PMC2856652 DOI: 10.1037/a0018411] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Multiple levels of category inclusiveness in 4 object domains (animals, vehicles, fruit, and furniture) were examined using a sequential touching procedure and assessed in both individual and group analyses in eighty 12-, 18-, 24-, and 30-month-olds. The roles of stimulus discriminability and child motor development, fatigue, and actions were also investigated. More inclusive levels of categorization systematically emerged before less inclusive levels, and a consistent advantage for categorizing high versus low perceptual contrasts was found. Group and individual analyses generally converged, but individual analyses added information about child categorization over group analyses. The development of object categorization in young children is discussed in light of efficiency of processing and similarity-differentiation theories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc H Bornstein
- Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Rockledge 1, Suite 8030, 6705 Rockledge Drive, MSC 7971, Bethesda MD 20892-7971, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Booth AE, Waxman SR. A horse of a different color: specifying with precision infants' mappings of novel nouns and adjectives. Child Dev 2009; 80:15-22. [PMID: 19236389 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01242.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 78] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
A precisely controlled automated procedure confirms a developmental decalage: Infants acquiring English link count nouns to object categories well before they link adjectives to properties. Fourteen- and 18-month-olds (n= 48 at each age) extended novel words presented as count nouns based on category membership rather than shared properties. When the same words were presented as adjectives, infants revealed no preference for either category- or property-based extensions. The convergence between performance in this automated procedure and in more interactive tasks is striking. Perhaps more importantly, the automated task provides a methodological foundation for (a) exploring the development of form - meaning links in infants acquiring languages other than English and (b) investigating the time course underlying infants' mapping of novel words to meaning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amy E Booth
- Roxelyn and Richard Pepper Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208-3540, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Oakes LM, Messenger IM, Ross-Sheehy S, Luck SJ. New evidence for rapid development of color-location binding in infants' visual short-term memory. VISUAL COGNITION 2009; 17:67-82. [PMID: 20046220 PMCID: PMC2631359 DOI: 10.1080/13506280802151480] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Change-detection tasks reveal that infants' ability to bind color to location in visual short-term memory (VSTM) develops rapidly: Seven-month-old infants, but not 6-month-old infants, detect that successive arrays of 3 objects are different if they contain the same colors in different locations (Oakes et al., 2006). Here we test a counterintuitive consequence of the hypothesis that six-month-old infants are unable to bind colors to locations: When comparing two successive stimulus arrays, these infants will often compare noncorresponding items, making it impossible for them to distinguish between identical arrays and nonidentical arrays. As a result, they will not show a preference for changing arrays over nonchanging arrays even when all of the items change. We tested this prediction by presenting 6- and 7-month-old infants (N = 36) with nonchanging displays of three items and changing displays in which all three items simultaneoulsy changed colors. As predicted, 7-month-old infants, but not 6-month-old infants, responded to the difference between these changing and nonchanging displays, providing additional evidence that the ability to bind colors to locations develops rapidly across this age range.
Collapse
|
38
|
Kovack-Lesh KA, Horst JS, Oakes LM. The Cat is out of the Bag: The Joint Influence of Previous Experience and Looking Behavior on Infant Categorization. INFANCY 2008. [DOI: 10.1080/15250000802189428] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|