1
|
Abstract
Degeneracy in biological systems refers to a many-to-one mapping between physical structures and their functional (including psychological) outcomes. Despite the ubiquity of the phenomenon, traditional analytical tools for modeling degeneracy in neuroscience are extremely limited. In this study, we generated synthetic datasets to describe three situations of degeneracy in fMRI data to demonstrate the limitations of the current univariate approach. We describe a novel computational approach for the analysis referred to as neural topographic factor analysis (NTFA). NTFA is designed to capture variations in neural activity across task conditions and participants. The advantage of this discovery-oriented approach is to reveal whether and how experimental trials and participants cluster into task conditions and participant groups. We applied NTFA on simulated data, revealing the appropriate degeneracy assumption in all three situations and demonstrating NTFA's utility in uncovering degeneracy. Lastly, we discussed the importance of testing degeneracy in fMRI data and the implications of applying NTFA to do so.
Collapse
|
2
|
Wu A, Nastase SA, Baldassano CA, Turk-Browne NB, Norman KA, Engelhardt BE, Pillow JW. Brain kernel: A new spatial covariance function for fMRI data. Neuroimage 2021; 245:118580. [PMID: 34740792 PMCID: PMC11542064 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118580] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2021] [Revised: 07/30/2021] [Accepted: 09/14/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
A key problem in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is to estimate spatial activity patterns from noisy high-dimensional signals. Spatial smoothing provides one approach to regularizing such estimates. However, standard smoothing methods ignore the fact that correlations in neural activity may fall off at different rates in different brain areas, or exhibit discontinuities across anatomical or functional boundaries. Moreover, such methods do not exploit the fact that widely separated brain regions may exhibit strong correlations due to bilateral symmetry or the network organization of brain regions. To capture this non-stationary spatial correlation structure, we introduce the brain kernel, a continuous covariance function for whole-brain activity patterns. We define the brain kernel in terms of a continuous nonlinear mapping from 3D brain coordinates to a latent embedding space, parametrized with a Gaussian process (GP). The brain kernel specifies the prior covariance between voxels as a function of the distance between their locations in embedding space. The GP mapping warps the brain nonlinearly so that highly correlated voxels are close together in latent space, and uncorrelated voxels are far apart. We estimate the brain kernel using resting-state fMRI data, and we develop an exact, scalable inference method based on block coordinate descent to overcome the challenges of high dimensionality (10-100K voxels). Finally, we illustrate the brain kernel's usefulness with applications to brain decoding and factor analysis with multiple task-based fMRI datasets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anqi Wu
- Center for Theoretical Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York City, NY, USA.
| | - Samuel A Nastase
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA; Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
| | | | | | - Kenneth A Norman
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA; Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
| | | | - Jonathan W Pillow
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA; Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Owen LLW, Chang TH, Manning JR. High-level cognition during story listening is reflected in high-order dynamic correlations in neural activity patterns. Nat Commun 2021; 12:5728. [PMID: 34593791 PMCID: PMC8484677 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-25876-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2019] [Accepted: 08/24/2021] [Indexed: 02/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Our thoughts arise from coordinated patterns of interactions between brain structures that change with our ongoing experiences. High-order dynamic correlations in neural activity patterns reflect different subgraphs of the brain's functional connectome that display homologous lower-level dynamic correlations. Here we test the hypothesis that high-level cognition is reflected in high-order dynamic correlations in brain activity patterns. We develop an approach to estimating high-order dynamic correlations in timeseries data, and we apply the approach to neuroimaging data collected as human participants either listen to a ten-minute story or listen to a temporally scrambled version of the story. We train across-participant pattern classifiers to decode (in held-out data) when in the session each neural activity snapshot was collected. We find that classifiers trained to decode from high-order dynamic correlations yield the best performance on data collected as participants listened to the (unscrambled) story. By contrast, classifiers trained to decode data from scrambled versions of the story yielded the best performance when they were trained using first-order dynamic correlations or non-correlational activity patterns. We suggest that as our thoughts become more complex, they are reflected in higher-order patterns of dynamic network interactions throughout the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucy L W Owen
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA
| | - Thomas H Chang
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA
- Amazon.com, Seattle, WA, USA
| | - Jeremy R Manning
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Moretti A, Stirn A, Marks G, Pe'er I. Autoencoding Topographic Factors. J Comput Biol 2019; 26:546-560. [DOI: 10.1089/cmb.2018.0176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Antonio Moretti
- Department of Computer Science, Columbia University, New York, New York
| | - Andrew Stirn
- Department of Computer Science, Columbia University, New York, New York
| | - Gabriel Marks
- Department of Computer Science, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
| | - Itsik Pe'er
- Department of Computer Science, Columbia University, New York, New York
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Turner BM, Palestro JJ, Miletić S, Forstmann BU. Advances in techniques for imposing reciprocity in brain-behavior relations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 102:327-336. [PMID: 31128445 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.04.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2018] [Revised: 03/18/2019] [Accepted: 04/25/2019] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
To better understand human behavior, the emerging field of model-based cognitive neuroscience seeks to anchor psychological theory to the biological substrate from which behavior originates: the brain. Despite complex dynamics, many researchers in this field have demonstrated that fluctuations in brain activity can be related to fluctuations in components of cognitive models, which instantiate psychological theories. In this review, we discuss a number of approaches for relating brain activity to cognitive models, and expand on a framework for imposing reciprocity in the inference of mental operations from the combination of brain and behavioral data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brandon M Turner
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA.
| | - James J Palestro
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA.
| | - Steven Miletić
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands.
| | - Birte U Forstmann
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Manning JR, Zhu X, Willke TL, Ranganath R, Stachenfeld K, Hasson U, Blei DM, Norman KA. A probabilistic approach to discovering dynamic full-brain functional connectivity patterns. Neuroimage 2018; 180:243-252. [PMID: 29448074 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.071] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2017] [Revised: 01/09/2018] [Accepted: 01/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent research shows that the covariance structure of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data - commonly described as functional connectivity - can change as a function of the participant's cognitive state (for review see Turk-Browne, 2013). Here we present a Bayesian hierarchical matrix factorization model, termed hierarchical topographic factor analysis (HTFA), for efficiently discovering full-brain networks in large multi-subject neuroimaging datasets. HTFA approximates each subject's network by first re-representing each brain image in terms of the activities of a set of localized nodes, and then computing the covariance of the activity time series of these nodes. The number of nodes, along with their locations, sizes, and activities (over time) are learned from the data. Because the number of nodes is typically substantially smaller than the number of fMRI voxels, HTFA can be orders of magnitude more efficient than traditional voxel-based functional connectivity approaches. In one case study, we show that HTFA recovers the known connectivity patterns underlying a collection of synthetic datasets. In a second case study, we illustrate how HTFA may be used to discover dynamic full-brain activity and connectivity patterns in real fMRI data, collected as participants listened to a story. In a third case study, we carried out a similar series of analyses on fMRI data collected as participants viewed an episode of a television show. In these latter case studies, we found that the HTFA-derived activity and connectivity patterns can be used to reliably decode which moments in the story or show the participants were experiencing. Further, we found that these two classes of patterns contained partially non-overlapping information, such that decoders trained on combinations of activity-based and dynamic connectivity-based features performed better than decoders trained on activity or connectivity patterns alone. We replicated this latter result with two additional (previously developed) methods for efficiently characterizing full-brain activity and connectivity patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Xia Zhu
- Intel Corporation, Hillsboro, OR, United States
| | | | | | | | - Uri Hasson
- Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, United States
| | - David M Blei
- Columbia University, New York, NY, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Huertas I, Oldehinkel M, van Oort ESB, Garcia-Solis D, Mir P, Beckmann CF, Marquand AF. A Bayesian spatial model for neuroimaging data based on biologically informed basis functions. Neuroimage 2017; 161:134-148. [PMID: 28782681 PMCID: PMC5692833 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2017] [Revised: 07/27/2017] [Accepted: 08/02/2017] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
The dominant approach to neuroimaging data analysis employs the voxel as the unit of computation. While convenient, voxels lack biological meaning and their size is arbitrarily determined by the resolution of the image. Here, we propose a multivariate spatial model in which neuroimaging data are characterised as a linearly weighted combination of multiscale basis functions which map onto underlying brain nuclei or networks or nuclei. In this model, the elementary building blocks are derived to reflect the functional anatomy of the brain during the resting state. This model is estimated using a Bayesian framework which accurately quantifies uncertainty and automatically finds the most accurate and parsimonious combination of basis functions describing the data. We demonstrate the utility of this framework by predicting quantitative SPECT images of striatal dopamine function and we compare a variety of basis sets including generic isotropic functions, anatomical representations of the striatum derived from structural MRI, and two different soft functional parcellations of the striatum derived from resting-state fMRI (rfMRI). We found that a combination of ∼50 multiscale functional basis functions accurately represented the striatal dopamine activity, and that functional basis functions derived from an advanced parcellation technique known as Instantaneous Connectivity Parcellation (ICP) provided the most parsimonious models of dopamine function. Importantly, functional basis functions derived from resting fMRI were more accurate than both structural and generic basis sets in representing dopamine function in the striatum for a fixed model order. We demonstrate the translational validity of our framework by constructing classification models for discriminating parkinsonian disorders and their subtypes. Here, we show that ICP approach is the only basis set that performs well across all comparisons and performs better overall than the classical voxel-based approach. This spatial model constitutes an elegant alternative to voxel-based approaches in neuroimaging studies; not only are their atoms biologically informed, they are also adaptive to high resolutions, represent high dimensions efficiently, and capture long-range spatial dependencies, which are important and challenging objectives for neuroimaging data. A multivariate spatial model using brain parcellations as basis functions is proposed. Brain regions can be modeled as a superposition of multiscale basis functions. These basis functions are biologically meaningful and capture spatial dependencies. Our framework allows to develop accurate and parsimonious clinical models. The model is computationally efficient, enhances power and adapts to high resolutions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ismael Huertas
- Unidad de Trastornos del Movimiento, Servicio de Neurología y NeurofisiologíaClínica, Instituto de Biomedicina de Sevilla (IBiS), Hospital UniversitarioVirgen del Rocío/CSIC/Universidad de Sevilla, Seville, Spain
| | - Marianne Oldehinkel
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Erik S B van Oort
- Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - David Garcia-Solis
- Servicio de Medicina Nuclear, UDIM, Hospital UniversitarioVirgen del Rocío, Seville, Spain; Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red sobre Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas (CIBERNED), Spain
| | - Pablo Mir
- Unidad de Trastornos del Movimiento, Servicio de Neurología y NeurofisiologíaClínica, Instituto de Biomedicina de Sevilla (IBiS), Hospital UniversitarioVirgen del Rocío/CSIC/Universidad de Sevilla, Seville, Spain; Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red sobre Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas (CIBERNED), Spain
| | - Christian F Beckmann
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Oxford Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB), University of Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Andre F Marquand
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Department of Neuroimaging, Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Turner BM, Wang T, Merkle EC. Factor analysis linking functions for simultaneously modeling neural and behavioral data. Neuroimage 2017; 153:28-48. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2016] [Revised: 03/19/2017] [Accepted: 03/20/2017] [Indexed: 10/19/2022] Open
|
9
|
Turner BM, Forstmann BU, Love BC, Palmeri TJ, Van Maanen L. Approaches to Analysis in Model-based Cognitive Neuroscience. JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017; 76:65-79. [PMID: 31745373 PMCID: PMC6863443 DOI: 10.1016/j.jmp.2016.01.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/06/2023]
Abstract
Our understanding of cognition has been advanced by two traditionally nonoverlapping and non-interacting groups. Mathematical psychologists rely on behavioral data to evaluate formal models of cognition, whereas cognitive neuroscientists rely on statistical models to understand patterns of neural activity, often without any attempt to make a connection to the mechanism supporting the computation. Both approaches suffer from critical limitations as a direct result of their focus on data at one level of analysis (cf. Marr, 1982), and these limitations have inspired researchers to attempt to combine both neural and behavioral measures in a cross-level integrative fashion. The importance of solving this problem has spawned several entirely new theoretical and statistical frameworks developed by both mathematical psychologists and cognitive neuroscientists. However, with each new approach comes a particular set of limitations and benefits. In this article, we survey and characterize several approaches for linking brain and behavioral data. We organize these approaches on the basis of particular cognitive modeling goals: (1) using the neural data to constrain a behavioral model, (2) using the behavioral model to predict neural data, and (3) fitting both neural and behavioral data simultaneously. Within each goal, we highlight a few particularly successful approaches for accomplishing that goal, and discuss some applications. Finally, we provide a conceptual guide to choosing among various analytic approaches in performing model-based cognitive neuroscience.
Collapse
|
10
|
Feng S, Holmes P. Will big data yield new mathematics? An evolving synergy with neuroscience. IMA JOURNAL OF APPLIED MATHEMATICS 2016; 81:432-456. [PMID: 27516705 PMCID: PMC4975073 DOI: 10.1093/imamat/hxw026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
New mathematics has often been inspired by new insights into the natural world. Here we describe some ongoing and possible future interactions among the massive data sets being collected in neuroscience, methods for their analysis and mathematical models of the underlying, still largely uncharted neural substrates that generate these data. We start by recalling events that occurred in turbulence modelling when substantial space-time velocity field measurements and numerical simulations allowed a new perspective on the governing equations of fluid mechanics. While no analogous global mathematical model of neural processes exists, we argue that big data may enable validation or at least rejection of models at cellular to brain area scales and may illuminate connections among models. We give examples of such models and survey some relatively new experimental technologies, including optogenetics and functional imaging, that can report neural activity in live animals performing complex tasks. The search for analytical techniques for these data is already yielding new mathematics, and we believe their multi-scale nature may help relate well-established models, such as the Hodgkin-Huxley equations for single neurons, to more abstract models of neural circuits, brain areas and larger networks within the brain. In brief, we envisage a closer liaison, if not a marriage, between neuroscience and mathematics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Feng
- Department of Applied Mathematics and Sciences, Khalifa University of Science, Technology, and Research, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates
| | - P Holmes
- Program in Applied and Computational Mathematics, Department of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering and Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, NJ 08544
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Kabisa S(T, Dunson DB, Morris JS. Online Variational Bayes Inference for High-Dimensional Correlated Data. J Comput Graph Stat 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/10618600.2014.998336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
12
|
Turner BM, Rodriguez CA, Norcia TM, McClure SM, Steyvers M. Why more is better: Simultaneous modeling of EEG, fMRI, and behavioral data. Neuroimage 2015; 128:96-115. [PMID: 26723544 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2015] [Revised: 11/13/2015] [Accepted: 12/18/2015] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
The need to test a growing number of theories in cognitive science has led to increased interest in inferential methods that integrate multiple data modalities. In this manuscript, we show how a method for integrating three data modalities within a single framework provides (1) more detailed descriptions of cognitive processes and (2) more accurate predictions of unobserved data than less integrative methods. Specifically, we show how combining either EEG and fMRI with a behavioral model can perform substantially better than a behavioral-data-only model in both generative and predictive modeling analyses. We then show how a trivariate model - a model including EEG, fMRI, and behavioral data - outperforms bivariate models in both generative and predictive modeling analyses. Together, these results suggest that within an appropriate modeling framework, more data can be used to better constrain cognitive theory, and to generate more accurate predictions for behavioral and neural data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | - Mark Steyvers
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, Irvine, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Topographic factor analysis: a Bayesian model for inferring brain networks from neural data. PLoS One 2014; 9:e94914. [PMID: 24804795 PMCID: PMC4012983 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094914] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2013] [Accepted: 03/21/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The neural patterns recorded during a neuroscientific experiment reflect complex interactions between many brain regions, each comprising millions of neurons. However, the measurements themselves are typically abstracted from that underlying structure. For example, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) datasets comprise a time series of three-dimensional images, where each voxel in an image (roughly) reflects the activity of the brain structure(s)–located at the corresponding point in space–at the time the image was collected. FMRI data often exhibit strong spatial correlations, whereby nearby voxels behave similarly over time as the underlying brain structure modulates its activity. Here we develop topographic factor analysis (TFA), a technique that exploits spatial correlations in fMRI data to recover the underlying structure that the images reflect. Specifically, TFA casts each brain image as a weighted sum of spatial functions. The parameters of those spatial functions, which may be learned by applying TFA to an fMRI dataset, reveal the locations and sizes of the brain structures activated while the data were collected, as well as the interactions between those structures.
Collapse
|
14
|
Gershman SJ, Blei DM, Norman KA, Sederberg PB. Decomposing spatiotemporal brain patterns into topographic latent sources. Neuroimage 2014; 98:91-102. [PMID: 24791745 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2013] [Revised: 04/14/2014] [Accepted: 04/21/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022] Open
Abstract
This paper extends earlier work on spatial modeling of fMRI data to the temporal domain, providing a framework for analyzing high temporal resolution brain imaging modalities such as electroencapholography (EEG). The central idea is to decompose brain imaging data into a covariate-dependent superposition of functions defined over continuous time and space (what we refer to as topographic latent sources). The continuous formulation allows us to parametrically model spatiotemporally localized activations. To make group-level inferences, we elaborate the model hierarchically by sharing sources across subjects. We describe a variational algorithm for parameter estimation that scales efficiently to large data sets. Applied to three EEG data sets, we find that the model produces good predictive performance and reproduces a number of classic findings. Our results suggest that topographic latent sources serve as an effective hypothesis space for interpreting spatiotemporal brain imaging data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel J Gershman
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA.
| | - David M Blei
- Department of Computer Science, Princeton University, 35 Olden Street, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA.
| | - Kenneth A Norman
- Department of Psychology and Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA.
| | - Per B Sederberg
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Shen Y, Mayhew SD, Kourtzi Z, Tiňo P. Spatial-temporal modelling of fMRI data through spatially regularized mixture of hidden process models. Neuroimage 2014; 84:657-71. [PMID: 24041873 PMCID: PMC4066951 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2012] [Revised: 07/12/2013] [Accepted: 09/03/2013] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous work investigated a range of spatio-temporal constraints for fMRI data analysis to provide robust detection of neural activation. We present a mixture-based method for the spatio-temporal modelling of fMRI data. This approach assumes that fMRI time series are generated by a probabilistic superposition of a small set of spatio-temporal prototypes (mixture components). Each prototype comprises a temporal model that explains fMRI signals on a single voxel and the model's "region of influence" through a spatial prior over the voxel space. As the key ingredient of our temporal model, the Hidden Process Model (HPM) framework proposed in Hutchinson et al. (2009) is adopted to infer the overlapping cognitive processes triggered by stimuli. Unlike the original HPM framework, we use a parametric model of Haemodynamic Response Function (HRF) so that biological constraints are naturally incorporated in the HRF estimation. The spatial priors are defined in terms of a parameterised distribution. Thus, the total number of parameters in the model does not depend on the number of voxels. The resulting model provides a conceptually principled and computationally efficient approach to identify spatio-temporal patterns of neural activation from fMRI data, in contrast to most conventional approaches in the literature focusing on the detection of spatial patterns. We first verify the proposed model in a controlled experimental setting using synthetic data. The model is further validated on real fMRI data obtained from a rapid event-related visual recognition experiment (Mayhew et al., 2012). Our model enables us to evaluate in a principled manner the variability of neural activations within individual regions of interest (ROIs). The results strongly suggest that, compared with occipitotemporal regions, the frontal ones are less homogeneous, requiring two HPM prototypes per region. Despite the rapid event-related experimental design, the model is capable of disentangling the perceptual judgement and motor response processes that are both activated in the frontal ROIs. Spatio-temporal heterogeneity in the frontal regions seems to be associated with diverse dynamic localizations of the two hidden processes in different subregions of frontal ROIs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuan Shen
- School of Computer Science, The University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
| | | | - Zoe Kourtzi
- School of Psychology, The University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
- Laboratory for Neuro- and Psychophysiology, K.U. Leuven, Belgium
| | - Peter Tiňo
- School of Computer Science, The University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Turner BM, Sederberg PB, Brown SD, Steyvers M. A method for efficiently sampling from distributions with correlated dimensions. Psychol Methods 2013; 18:368-84. [PMID: 23646991 DOI: 10.1037/a0032222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Bayesian estimation has played a pivotal role in the understanding of individual differences. However, for many models in psychology, Bayesian estimation of model parameters can be difficult. One reason for this difficulty is that conventional sampling algorithms, such as Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC), can be inefficient and impractical when little is known about the target distribution--particularly the target distribution's covariance structure. In this article, we highlight some reasons for this inefficiency and advocate the use of a population MCMC algorithm, called differential evolution Markov chain Monte Carlo (DE-MCMC), as a means of efficient proposal generation. We demonstrate in a simulation study that the performance of the DE-MCMC algorithm is unaffected by the correlation of the target distribution, whereas conventional MCMC performs substantially worse as the correlation increases. We then show that the DE-MCMC algorithm can be used to efficiently fit a hierarchical version of the linear ballistic accumulator model to response time data, which has proven to be a difficult task when conventional MCMC is used.
Collapse
|
17
|
Turner BM, Forstmann BU, Wagenmakers EJ, Brown SD, Sederberg PB, Steyvers M. A Bayesian framework for simultaneously modeling neural and behavioral data. Neuroimage 2013; 72:193-206. [PMID: 23370060 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 95] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2012] [Revised: 01/21/2013] [Accepted: 01/23/2013] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Scientists who study cognition infer underlying processes either by observing behavior (e.g., response times, percentage correct) or by observing neural activity (e.g., the BOLD response). These two types of observations have traditionally supported two separate lines of study. The first is led by cognitive modelers, who rely on behavior alone to support their computational theories. The second is led by cognitive neuroimagers, who rely on statistical models to link patterns of neural activity to experimental manipulations, often without any attempt to make a direct connection to an explicit computational theory. Here we present a flexible Bayesian framework for combining neural and cognitive models. Joining neuroimaging and computational modeling in a single hierarchical framework allows the neural data to influence the parameters of the cognitive model and allows behavioral data, even in the absence of neural data, to constrain the neural model. Critically, our Bayesian approach can reveal interactions between behavioral and neural parameters, and hence between neural activity and cognitive mechanisms. We demonstrate the utility of our approach with applications to simulated fMRI data with a recognition model and to diffusion-weighted imaging data with a response time model of perceptual choice.
Collapse
|
18
|
Nichols TE. Multiple testing corrections, nonparametric methods, and random field theory. Neuroimage 2012; 62:811-5. [PMID: 22521256 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 134] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2012] [Revised: 04/03/2012] [Accepted: 04/09/2012] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
I provide a selective review of the literature on the multiple testing problem in fMRI. By drawing connections with the older modalities, PET in particular, and how software implementations have tracked (or lagged behind) theoretical developments, my narrative aims to give the methodological researcher a historical perspective on this important aspect of fMRI data analysis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas E Nichols
- Warwick Manufacturing Group & Department of Statistics, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
van Gerven MAJ, Chao ZC, Heskes T. On the decoding of intracranial data using sparse orthonormalized partial least squares. J Neural Eng 2012; 9:026017. [PMID: 22414639 DOI: 10.1088/1741-2560/9/2/026017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
It has recently been shown that robust decoding of motor output from electrocorticogram signals in monkeys over prolonged periods of time has become feasible (Chao et al 2010 Front. Neuroeng. 3 1-10). In order to achieve these results, multivariate partial least-squares (PLS) regression was used. PLS uses a set of latent variables, referred to as components, to model the relationship between the input and the output data and is known to handle high-dimensional and possibly strongly correlated inputs and outputs well. We developed a new decoding method called sparse orthonormalized partial least squares (SOPLS) which was tested on a subset of the data used in Chao et al (2010) (freely obtainable from neurotycho.org (Nagasaka et al 2011 PLoS ONE 6 e22561)). We show that SOPLS reaches the same decoding performance as PLS using just two sparse components which can each be interpreted as encoding particular combinations of motor parameters. Furthermore, the sparse solution afforded by the SOPLS model allowed us to show the functional involvement of beta and gamma band responses in premotor and motor cortex for predicting the first component. Based on the literature, we conjecture that this first component is involved in the encoding of movement direction. Hence, the sparse and compact representation afforded by the SOPLS model facilitates interpretation of which spectral, spatial and temporal components are involved in successful decoding. These advantages make the proposed decoding method an important new tool in neuroprosthetics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marcel A J van Gerven
- Radboud University Nijmegen, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Donders Centre for Cognition, Montessorilaan 3, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
de Brecht M, Yamagishi N. Combining sparseness and smoothness improves classification accuracy and interpretability. Neuroimage 2012; 60:1550-61. [PMID: 22261376 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2011] [Revised: 12/13/2011] [Accepted: 12/24/2011] [Indexed: 10/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Sparse logistic regression (SLR) has been shown to be a useful method for decoding high-dimensional fMRI and MEG data by automatically selecting relevant feature dimensions. However, when applied to signals with high spatio-temporal correlations, SLR often over-prunes the feature space, which can result in overfitting and weight vectors that are difficult to interpret. To overcome this problem, we investigate a modification of ℓ₁-normed sparse logistic regression, called smooth sparse logistic regression (SSLR), which has a spatio-temporal "smoothing" prior that encourages weights that are close in time and space to have similar values. This causes the classifier to select spatio-temporally continuous groups of features, whereas SLR classifiers often select a scattered collection of independent features. We applied the method to both simulation data and real MEG data. We found that SSLR consistently increases classification accuracy, and produces weight vectors that are more meaningful from a neuroscientific perspective.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew de Brecht
- National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, 2-2-2 Hikaridai, Keihanna Science City, Kyoto 619-0288, Japan.
| | | |
Collapse
|