1
|
VanMeter F, Henderson H, Konovalov H, Karni-Visel Y, Blasbalg U. Children's Narrative Coherence in "Achieving Best Evidence" Forensic Interviews and Courtroom Testimony. PSYCHOLOGY, CRIME & LAW : PC & L 2021; 29:203-221. [PMID: 36949752 PMCID: PMC10027376 DOI: 10.1080/1068316x.2021.2018438] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2020] [Accepted: 09/26/2021] [Indexed: 06/18/2023]
Abstract
In the United Kingdom, Section 27 of the Youth Justice and Criminal Evidence Act permits "Achieving Best Evidence" (ABE) forensic interviews to replace the evidence-in-chief in cases involving children. It is therefore imperative that forensic interviewers elicit complete, reliable, and coherent narratives from children. The goal of the current research study was to assess the coherence of forensic interviews and whether the interviewers' emotional or cognitive support was associated with increases in the coherence of these interviews. Children's narrative coherence was examined in 80 transcripts of ABE investigative interviews with 7- to-15-year-olds who disclosed sexual abuse. Narrative coherence was assessed using the Narrative Coherence Coding Scheme, including three dimensions of narrative coherence: chronology, consistency, and theme (Reese et al., 2011). Findings revealed that first elicited events were more likely to be more coherent compared to subsequently elicited events, and child engagement was positively associated with all dimensions of narrative coherence. Interviewer support was positively associated with chronology, script accounts of abuse were associated with decreased consistency and chronology (but not theme), and cognitive support was not associated with any dimension of narrative coherence.
Collapse
|
2
|
Dilevski N, Paterson HM, van Golde C. Adult memory for instances of a repeated emotionally stressful event: does retention interval matter? Memory 2020; 29:98-116. [PMID: 33356908 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2020.1860227] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
This pre-registered study examined the impact of different retention intervals on remembering each instance of an emotionally stressful, repeated event. Eighty-nine adult female participants imagined being a victim of four similar domestic violence instances over a four-week period. Participants then completed recall and recognition memory questions about each instance either immediately, one-week, or three-weeks after the final instance. Overall, the findings showed that memory performance was often most accurate for the first and last instance compared to the middle instances. That is, participants reported more correct information, made fewer memory errors, and had better quality memory reports for the first and last instances compared to the middle instances. However, following a short delay (i.e., no delay and one-week), participants reported more correct information and were better at discriminating between correct and false details for the last instance relative to the others instances, while at a longer delay (i.e., three-weeks), more correct information was recalled for the first instance compared to other instances (there was no effect for memory discrimination). These findings suggest that memory for instances of a repeated event can depend on the position of an instance, and under some circumstances, the retention interval.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natali Dilevski
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Helen M Paterson
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Celine van Golde
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Dilevski N, Paterson HM, Walker SA, van Golde C. Adult memory for specific instances of a repeated event: a preliminary review. PSYCHIATRY, PSYCHOLOGY, AND LAW : AN INTERDISCIPLINARY JOURNAL OF THE AUSTRALIAN AND NEW ZEALAND ASSOCIATION OF PSYCHIATRY, PSYCHOLOGY AND LAW 2020; 28:711-732. [PMID: 35571600 PMCID: PMC9103626 DOI: 10.1080/13218719.2020.1837031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
In cases of repeated victimisation, a complainant's statement of abuse, and therefore memory, is often critical evidence for forensic investigations and legal proceedings. It is therefore important to understand the functioning of adults' memory for repeated events. As such, the purpose of this paper was to review the extant literature on adult memory for instances of a repeated event. The results of the review revealed a small number of heterogeneous studies on adult repeated-event memory (N = 12). The literature so far shows that while adults might have difficulty in recalling information specific to instances (narrow accuracy), they are capable of remembering information across multiple instances (broad accuracy). It was also found that several factors may impact recall of instances including age, the number of experienced instances, rehearsing an event, repeated retrieval and event distinctiveness. The discussion highlights the forensic implications of this research and future research directions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natali Dilevski
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | | | - Sarah A. Walker
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Celine van Golde
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
An Investigation of the Question-Types Teachers Use to Elicit Information From Children. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2020. [DOI: 10.1017/edp.2014.5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
5
|
Brubacher SP, Powell MB. Best-practice interviewing spans many contexts. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.07.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
6
|
Danby MC, Sharman SJ, Brubacher SP, Powell MB. The effects of episode similarity on children's reports of a repeated event. Memory 2018; 27:561-567. [PMID: 30295155 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1529798] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Much research has tested techniques to improve children's reporting of episodes from a repeated event by interviewing children after they have experienced multiple episodes of a scripted event. However, these studies have not considered any effects of the similarity shared between event episodes on children's reports. In the current study, 5- to 9-year-olds experienced four episodes of a scripted repeated event that shared a high (n = 76) or low (n = 76) degree of similarity, and were subsequently interviewed about individual episodes. The proportional amount and accuracy of children's reported details were tallied. Children reported proportionally more details and more script deviations after experiencing the high, compared to low, similarity event. Conversely, children were more accurate in their episodic reports when they experienced the low, compared to high, similarity event. The current findings have implications for the generalisability and comparability of past results across laboratory studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meaghan C Danby
- a School of Psychology , Deakin University , Burwood , VIC , Australia
| | | | - Sonja P Brubacher
- b Centre for Investigative Interviewing , Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University , Mount Gravatt , Australia
| | - Martine B Powell
- b Centre for Investigative Interviewing , Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University , Mount Gravatt , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Stolzenberg SN, McWilliams K, Lyon TD. Children's Conversational Memory Regarding a Minor Transgression and a Subsequent Interview. PSYCHOLOGY, PUBLIC POLICY, AND LAW : AN OFFICIAL LAW REVIEW OF THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA COLLEGE OF LAW AND THE UNIVERSITY OF MIAMI SCHOOL OF LAW 2018; 24:379-392. [PMID: 30479470 PMCID: PMC6251411 DOI: 10.1037/law0000176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
Children's memories for their conversations are commonly explored in child abuse cases. In two studies, we examined conversational recall in 154 4- to 9-year-old children's reports of an interaction with a stranger, some of whom were complicit in a transgression and were admonished to keep it a secret. Immediately afterwards, all children were interviewed about their interaction. One week later, children were asked recall questions about their interaction with the stranger, their conversations with the stranger, and their conversations with the interviewer. Overall, interaction recall questions elicited few details about children's conversations, whereas conversation recall questions were effective in doing so. Accuracy was high in response to both the interaction and conversation recall questions, with no differences observed. Questions explicitly inquiring about coaching elicited higher error rates, as well as apparent attempts to maintain secrecy. Source errors were rare. Conversation recall questions elicited new transgression disclosures among a substantial percentage of children. The results provide tentative support for the use of recall questions in eliciting conversational information from children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Kelly McWilliams
- John Jay School of Criminal Justice, City University of New York
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Brubacher SP, Earhart B, Roberts KP, Powell MB. Effects of label training and recall order on children's reports of a repeated event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sonja P. Brubacher
- Department of Psychology; Wilfrid Laurier University; Waterloo Ontario Canada
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University; Mt. Gravatt Queensland Australia
| | - Becky Earhart
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University; Mt. Gravatt Queensland Australia
| | - Kim P. Roberts
- Department of Psychology; Wilfrid Laurier University; Waterloo Ontario Canada
| | - Martine B. Powell
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University; Mt. Gravatt Queensland Australia
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Hershkowitz I, Melkman EP, Zur R. When Is a Child's Forensic Statement Deemed Credible? A Comparison of Physical and Sexual Abuse Cases. CHILD MALTREATMENT 2018; 23:196-206. [PMID: 29034734 DOI: 10.1177/1077559517734059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
A large national sample of 4,775 reports of child physical and sexual abuse made in Israel in 2014 was analyzed in order to examine whether assessments of credibility would vary according to abuse type, physical or sexual, and whether child and event characteristics contributing to the probability that reports of abuse would be determined as credible would be similar or different in child physical abuse (CPA) and child sexual abuse (CSA) cases. Results revealed that CPA reports were less likely to be viewed as credible (41.9%) compared to CSA reports (56.7%). Multigroup path analysis, however, indicated equivalence in predicting factors. In a unified model for both types of abuse, salient predictors of a credible judgment were older age, lack of a cognitive delay, and the alleged abusive event being a onetime less severe act. Over and beyond the effects of these factors, abuse type significantly contributed to the prediction of credibility judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Eran P Melkman
- 2 Department of Education, Rees Centre for Research on Fostering and Education, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Ronit Zur
- 3 Child Investigations Service, Ministry of Labour and Social Affairs, Jerusalem, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Alsaif DM, Almadani OM, Almoghannam SA, Al-Farayedhi MA, Kharoshah MA. Teaching children about self-protection from sexual abuse: could it be a cause for source monitoring errors and fantasy? (Two case reports). EGYPTIAN JOURNAL OF FORENSIC SCIENCES 2018. [DOI: 10.1186/s41935-018-0058-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
|
11
|
Szojka ZA, Andrews SJ, Lamb ME, Stolzenberg SN, Lyon TD. Challenging the Credibility of Alleged Victims of Child Sexual Abuse in Scottish Courts. PSYCHOLOGY, PUBLIC POLICY, AND LAW : AN OFFICIAL LAW REVIEW OF THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA COLLEGE OF LAW AND THE UNIVERSITY OF MIAMI SCHOOL OF LAW 2017; 23:200-210. [PMID: 31555043 PMCID: PMC6760857 DOI: 10.1037/law0000117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
This study examined the effects of credibility-challenging questions (n = 2,729) on 62 5- to 17-year-olds' testimony in child sexual abuse cases in Scotland by categorizing the type, source, and content of the credibility-challenging questions defense lawyers asked and assessing how children responded. Credibility-challenging questions comprised 14.9% of all questions asked during cross-examination. Of defense lawyers' credibility-challenging questions, 77.8% focused generally on children's honesty, whereas the remainder referred to specific inconsistencies in the children's testimony. Children resisted credibility challenges 54% of the time, significantly more often than they provided compliant responses (26.8%). The tendency to resist was significantly lower for questions focused on specific rather than general inconsistencies, and peripheral rather than central content. Overall, children resisted credibility challenges more often when the aim and content of the question could be understood easily. As this was a field study, the accuracy of children's responses could not be assessed. The findings suggest that credibility-challenging questions that place unrealistic demands on children's memory capacities (e.g., questions focused on peripheral content or highly specific details) occur frequently, and that juries should be made aware of the disproportionate effects of such questioning on the consistency of children's testimony.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zsófia A Szojka
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Samantha J Andrews
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Michael E Lamb
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Stacia N Stolzenberg
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Thomas D Lyon
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Willén RM, Granhag PA, Strömwall LA. Factors Affecting Two Types of Memory Specificity: Particularization of Episodes and Details. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0166469. [PMID: 27851775 PMCID: PMC5112775 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166469] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2016] [Accepted: 10/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Memory for repeated events is relevant to legal investigations about repeated occurrences. We investigated how two measures of specificity (number of events referred to and amount of detail reported about the events) were influenced by interviewees' age, number of experienced events, interviewer, perceived unpleasantness, and memory rehearsal. Transcribed narratives consisting of over 40.000 utterances from 95 dental patients, and the corresponding dental records, were studied. Amount of detail was measured by categorizing the utterances as generic, specific, or specific-extended. We found that the two measures were affected differently by all five factors. For instance, number of experienced events positively influenced number of referred events but had no effect on amount of detail provided about the events. We make suggestions for future research and encourage reanalysis of the present data set and reuse of the material.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca M. Willén
- IGDORE, The Globally Distributed Institute for Open Research and Education
- * E-mail:
| | - Pär Anders Granhag
- Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
- Norwegian Police University College, Oslo, Norway
| | - Leif A. Strömwall
- Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Wright R, Powell MB. Investigative Interviewers' Perceptions of Their Difficulty in Adhering to Open-Ended Questions with Child Witnesses. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.1350/ijps.2006.8.4.316] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Best practice guidelines for conducting investigative interviews of children emphasise the importance of obtaining free narrative accounts with the use of open-ended questions. However, research indicates that most investigative interviewers underutilise open-ended questions, even following intensive training in their use. The aim of the current study was to explore investigative interviewers' perceptions of their difficulty in asking open-ended questions. During a training course on how to use open-ended questions, eight child abuse investigators were individually interviewed about why they had asked specific questions in a 10-minute mock interview conducted immediately earlier with a school child. Overall, three reasons were identified. These related to: 1. the specificity of the information required from children; 2. the unfamiliar nature of the open-ended discourse style; and 3. the complex distinction between open-ended versus specific questions. Each of these themes is discussed, along with the implications for trainers and researchers in child investigative interviewing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca Wright
- Faculty of Health Medicine Nursing & Behavioural Sciences, School of Psychology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Hwy, Burwood, VIC. 3125, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Alonzo-Proulx A, Cyr M. Factors Predicting Central Details in Alleged Child Sexual Abuse Victims’ Disclosure. JOURNAL OF FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY PRACTICE 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/15228932.2016.1172422] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
15
|
Cleveland KC, Quas JA, Lyon TD. Valence, Implicated Actor, and Children's Acquiescence to False Suggestions. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2016; 43:1-7. [PMID: 26955204 DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Although adverse effects of suggestive interviewing on children's accuracy are well documented, it remains unclear as to whether these effects vary depending on the valence of and the actor implicated in suggestions. In this study, 124 3-8-year-olds participated in a classroom activity and were later questioned about positive and negative false details. The interviewer provided positive reinforcement when children acquiesced to suggestions and negative feedback when they did not. Following reinforcement or feedback, young children were comparably suggestible for positive and negative details. With age, resistance to suggestions about negative details emerged first, followed by resistance to suggestions about positive details. Across age, more negative feedback was required to induce acquiescence to negative than positive false details. Finally, children were less willing to acquiesce when they (versus the confederate) were implicated. Findings highlight the interactive effects of valence and children's age on their eyewitness performance in suggestive contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyndra C Cleveland
- , , 4201 Social & Behavioral Sciences Gateway, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697-7085 United States
| | - Jodi A Quas
- , , 4328 Social & Behavioral Sciences Gateway, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697-7085 United States
| | - Thomas D Lyon
- , , University of Southern California, 699 Exposition Boulevard, Los Angeles, CA 90089 United States
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Data from Interviews with 95 Respondents Recollecting Repeated Dental Visits. JOURNAL OF OPEN PSYCHOLOGY DATA 2015. [DOI: 10.5334/jopd.an] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
17
|
Lorsbach TC, Friehe MJ, Teten AF, Reimer JF, Armendarez JJ. Controlled Retrieval of Specific Context Information in Children and Adults. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [PMID: 26219173 DOI: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.1.0043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
This study adapted a procedure used by Luo and Craik (2009) to examine whether developmental differences exist in the ability to use controlled retrieval processes to access the contextual details of memory representations. Participants from 3 age groups (mean ages 9, 12, and 25 years) were presented with words in 3 study contexts: with a black-and-white picture, with a color picture, or alone without a picture. Six recognition tests were then presented that varied in the demands (high or low) placed on the retrieval of specific contextual information. Each test consisted of a mixture of words that were old targets from 1 study context, distractors (i.e., previously studied words from a different context), and completely new words. A high-specificity and a low-specificity test list was paired with each test question, with high and low specificity being determined by the nature of the distractors used in a test list. High-specificity tests contained words that were studied in similar contexts: old targets (e.g., words studied with black-and-white pictures) and distractors (e.g., words studied with color pictures). In contrast, low-specificity tests contained words that were studied in dissimilar contexts: old targets (e.g., words studied with black-and-white pictures) and distractors (e.g., words previously studied without a picture). Relative to low-specificity tests, the retrieval conditions of high-specificity tests were assumed to place greater demands on the controlled access of specific contextual information. Analysis of recollection scores revealed that age differences were present on high-but not low-specificity tests, with the performance of 9-year-olds disproportionately affected by the retrieval demands of high-specificity tests.
Collapse
|
18
|
Roberts KP, Brubacher SP, Drohan-Jennings D, Glisic U, Powell MB, Friedman WJ. Developmental Differences in the Ability to Provide Temporal Information About Repeated Events. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3118] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Una Glisic
- Psychology; Wilfrid Laurier University; Waterloo Canada
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Willén RM, Granhag PA, Strömwall LA, Fisher RP. Facilitating particularization of repeated similar events with context-specific cues. Scand J Psychol 2014; 56:28-37. [PMID: 25382708 DOI: 10.1111/sjop.12180] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2014] [Accepted: 09/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Ninety-five dental care patients participated in a quasi-experiment in which they were interviewed twice about dental visits they had made during the past ten years. Objective truth was established by analysing their dental records. The main purpose of the study was to investigate to what extent context-specific cues could facilitate particularization (i.e., recollection of events and details) of repeated and similar events. A mixed design was employed and the effects of three types of cues were explored: two types of context-specific cues vs. cues commonly used in police practise when interviewing plaintiffs. In line with our hypothesis, context-specific cues tended to be more effective for recollection of individual events than the comparison cues. In addition, context-specific cues generated more details than the comparison cues and the difference was marginally significant. Rehearsal of the memories by telling them to others was associated with an increased number of recollected events and details. The results are discussed from a legal psychology perspective with focus on recollection of repeated abuse.
Collapse
|
20
|
Themeli O, Panagiotaki M. Forensic Interviews With Children Victims of Sexual Abuse: The Role of the Counselling Psychologist. THE EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY 2014. [DOI: 10.5964/ejcop.v3i1.17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
21
|
Brubacher SP, La Rooy D. Witness recall across repeated interviews in a case of repeated abuse. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2014; 38:202-211. [PMID: 23906673 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.06.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2013] [Revised: 06/12/2013] [Accepted: 06/26/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
In this illustrative case study we examine the three forensic interviews of a girl who experienced repeated sexual abuse from ages 7 to 11. She disclosed the abuse after watching a serialized television show that contained a storyline similar to her own experience. This triggered an investigation that ended in successful prosecution of the offender. Because this case involved abuse that was repeated on a weekly basis for 4 years we thus investigated the degree to which the child's narrative reflected specific episodes or generic accounts, and both the interviewer's and child's attempts to elicit and provide, respectively, specific details across the 3 interviews collected in a 1 month period. Across the 3 interviews, the child's account was largely generic, yet on a number of occasions she provided details specific to individual incidents (episodic leads) that could have been probed further. As predicted: earlier interviews were characterized more by episodic than generic prompts and the reverse was true for the third interview; the child often responded using the same style of language (episodic or generic) as the interviewer; and open questions yielded narrative information. We discuss the importance of adopting children's words to specify occurrences, and the potential benefits of permitting generic recall in investigative interviews on children's ability to provide episodic leads. Despite the fact that the testimony was characterized by generic information about what usually happened, rather than specific episodic details about individual occurrences, this case resulted in successful prosecution.
Collapse
|
22
|
Stolzenberg SN, Lyon TD. How Attorneys Question Children About the Dynamics of Sexual Abuse and Disclosure in Criminal Trials. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2014; 20:19-30. [PMID: 25242881 DOI: 10.1037/a0035000] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Little is known about how the dynamics of sexual abuse and disclosure are discussed in criminal court. We examined how attorneys ask child witnesses in sexual abuse cases (N = 72, 6 -16 years of age) about their prior conversations, both with suspects and with disclosure recipients. Prosecutors' questions were more open-ended than defense attorneys, but most questions asked by either attorney were yes/no questions, and children tended to provide unelaborated responses. Prosecutors were more inclined to ask about children's prior conversations with suspects than defense attorneys, but focused on the immediate abuse rather than on grooming behavior or attempts to silence the victim. Prosecutors were also more inclined to ask about children's motives for disclosing or for failing to disclose than defense attorneys, but in most cases, failed to ask. Both types of attorney asked children about prior disclosures, although defense attorneys were more inclined to ask children to recall specific content in particular disclosures. On average, children were asked about five disclosure recipients, and denied disclosing some information in 93% of cases. Attorneys exhibited little sensitivity to the age of the child in selecting their questions. The implications of the results for improving the process by which abuse cases are tried in court are discussed.
Collapse
|
23
|
Azad A, Leander L. Experts' beliefs about child testimony: Do they match the research or the recommendations? NORDIC PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1080/19012276.2012.768032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
24
|
Brubacher SP, Malloy LC, Lamb ME, Roberts KP. How Do Interviewers and Children Discuss Individual Occurrences of Alleged Repeated Abuse in Forensic Interviews? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.2920] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Michael E. Lamb
- Department of Psychology; University of Cambridge; Cambridge; UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Wandrey L, Lyon TD, Quas JA, Friedman WJ. MALTREATED CHILDREN'S ABILITY TO ESTIMATE TEMPORAL LOCATION AND NUMEROSITY OF PLACEMENT CHANGES AND COURT VISITS. PSYCHOLOGY, PUBLIC POLICY, AND LAW : AN OFFICIAL LAW REVIEW OF THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA COLLEGE OF LAW AND THE UNIVERSITY OF MIAMI SCHOOL OF LAW 2012; 18:79-104. [PMID: 22347789 PMCID: PMC3280883 DOI: 10.1037/a0024812] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Research examining children's temporal knowledge has tended to utilize brief temporal intervals and singular, neutral events, and is not readily generalizable to legal settings in which maltreated children are asked temporal questions about salient, repeated abuse that often occurred in the distant past. To understand how well maltreated children can describe temporal location and numerosity of documented, personal experiences, we assessed 167 6- to 10-year-old maltreated children's temporal memory for changes in their living arrangements and prior visits to court. Small percentages of children were capable of providing exact temporal location information (age, month, or season) regarding their first or last placement or court experience, or numerosities for placements or court visits. Greater knowledge of current temporal locations did not predict better performance. However, older children's performance for several temporal judgments was better than chance, and their reports were not largely discrepant from the truth. Findings suggest caution when questioning maltreated children about when and how many times prior events occurred.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lindsay Wandrey
- Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Feltis BB, Powell MB, Roberts KP. The effect of event repetition on the production of story grammar in children's event narratives. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2011; 35:180-187. [PMID: 21481456 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/22/2008] [Revised: 11/24/2010] [Accepted: 11/25/2010] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This study examined the effect of event repetition on the amount and nature of story-grammar produced by children when recalling the event. METHOD Children aged 4 years (N=50) and 7 years (N=56) participated in either 1 or 6 occurrences of a highly similar event where details varied across the occurrences. Half the children in each age and event group recalled the last/single occurrence 5-6 days later and the other half recalled the last/single occurrence after 5-6 weeks (the final and single occurrence was the same). Children's free recall responses were classified according to the number and proportion of story-grammar elements (Stein & Glenn, 1979-setting, initiating event, internal response, plan, attempt, direct consequence, and resolution) as well as the prevalence of causal links between the individual story-grammar elements. RESULTS More story-grammar detail and more links between individual story-grammar elements were reported about the final compared to single occurrence. The amount of story-grammar increased with age and decreased over time. Further, an interaction was revealed such that the effect of retention interval on the production of story-grammar was negligible for older children who experienced the repeated event. CONCLUSIONS Event repetition has a beneficial effect on the production of children's story-grammar content in situations where event details varied from occasion to occasion. This study highlights the importance of eliciting free recall when conducting evidential interviews with child witnesses about repeated events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brooke B Feltis
- School of Psychology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Hwy, Burwood, Victoria 3125, Australia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
L’Entretien Cognitif est-il efficace pour aider de très jeunes enfants à témoigner d’un Événement répété dans le temps ? ANNEE PSYCHOLOGIQUE 2011. [DOI: 10.4074/s0003503310004033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
28
|
Verkampt F, Ginet M, Colomb C. L’Entretien Cognitif est-il efficace pour aider de très jeunes enfants à témoigner d’un Événement répété dans le temps ? ANNEE PSYCHOLOGIQUE 2010. [DOI: 10.3917/anpsy.104.0541] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
|
29
|
Brubacher SP, Glisic U, Roberts KP, Powell M. Children's ability to recall unique aspects of one occurrence of a repeated event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2010. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1696] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
30
|
Odegard TN, Cooper CM, Lampinen JM, Reyna VF, Brainerd CJ. Children’s Eyewitness Memory for Multiple Real-Life Events. Child Dev 2009; 80:1877-90. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01373.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
31
|
Larsson AS, Lamb ME. Making the most of information-gathering interviews with children. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2009. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.573] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
32
|
Powell MB, Fisher RP, Hughes-Scholes CH. The effect of using trained versus untrained adult respondents in simulated practice interviews about child abuse. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2008; 32:1007-1016. [PMID: 19027162 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2006] [Revised: 03/11/2008] [Accepted: 05/15/2008] [Indexed: 05/27/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE A single study tested the hypothesis that simulated practice interviews for investigative interviewers of children are more effective when the role of the child respondent is played by trained actors (i.e., postgraduate psychology students) than untrained fellow participants (i.e., child protection workers). METHOD The interviewers included 50 child protection service workers. Each interviewer received instruction in the use of open-ended questions and then engaged in two simulated practice interviews. The role of the child respondent in the practice interviews was played by either a trained psychology student or an untrained fellow participant. The key outcome measure was the proportion of open-ended questions, which was assessed immediately prior to and after the practice sessions, as well as 12 weeks post-training. RESULTS Interviewers who had practiced with trained actors had higher post-training performance (M=.83, SD=.12) compared to those who had practiced with untrained fellow participants (M=.73, SD=.13, p<.05), even at the 12-week follow up (M actors=.66, SD=.25; M untrained actors=.49, SD=.23, p<.05). CONCLUSIONS Training programs that make better use of practice opportunities (e.g., by using trained respondents) will be more effective in improving the performance of investigative interviewers. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS A single study investigated the relative effectiveness of two simulated practice exercises for professionals who interview children about abuse. This research is relevant to professionals who design investigative interviewer training programs because it indicates that practical exercises, which are currently chosen on an 'ad hoc' or convenience basis, can vary markedly in their effectiveness in encouraging adherence to open questions.
Collapse
|
33
|
Murfett R, Powell MB, Snow PC. The effect of intellectual disability on the adherence of child witnesses to a "story grammar" framework. JOURNAL OF INTELLECTUAL & DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITY 2008; 33:2-11. [PMID: 18300162 DOI: 10.1080/13668250701829811] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND This study examined the ability of 78 children (aged 9-12 years) with an intellectual disability (ID) to provide a narrative account of a staged event they had participated in four days earlier. METHOD The children were interviewed using open-ended questions. The quality of their responses (using a story grammar framework) was compared with that of two control groups: mainstream children matched for mental and chronological age. RESULTS While the children with an ID and those matched for mental age provided narratives of similar length and used similar proportions of each story grammar element, the ID group was less likely than both control groups to provide a narrative account at all. Among those children with an ID who did provide a narrative account, their accounts included proportionately fewer story grammar elements than those of both control groups. CONCLUSION Children with an ID are disadvantaged as witnesses with respect to their ability to provide a detailed and coherent narrative account of events under optimal investigative interviewing conditions.
Collapse
|
34
|
Roberts KP, Powell MB. The roles of prior experience and the timing of misinformation presentation on young children's event memories. Child Dev 2007; 78:1137-52. [PMID: 17650130 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01057.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
The current study addressed how the timing of interviews affected children's memories of unique and repeated events. Five- to six-year-olds (N=125) participated in activities 1 or 4 times and were misinformed either 3 or 21 days after the only or last event. Although single-experience children were subsequently less accurate in the 21- versus 3-day condition, the timing of the misinformation session did not affect memories of repeated-experience children regarding invariant details. Children were more suggestible in the 21- versus 3-day condition for variable details when the test occurred soon after misinformation presentation. Thus, timing differentially affected memories of single and repeated events and depended on the combination of event-misinformation and misinformation-test delays rather than the overall retention interval.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kim P Roberts
- Wilfrid Laurier University, Department of Psychology, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Powell MB, Roberts KP, Thomson DM, Ceci SJ. The impact of experienced versus non-experienced suggestions on children's recall of repeated events. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2007. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1299] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
36
|
Powell MB, Lancaster S. Guidelines for interviewing children during child custody evaluations. AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2006. [DOI: 10.1080/00050060310001707017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
37
|
Roberts KP, Powell MB. The consistency of false suggestions moderates children’s reports of a single instance of a repeated event: Predicting increases and decreases in suggestibility. J Exp Child Psychol 2006; 94:68-89. [PMID: 16513130 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2005] [Revised: 12/09/2005] [Accepted: 12/15/2005] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Participants (6- and 7-year-olds, N=130) participated in classroom activities four times. Children were interviewed about the final occurrence (target event) either 1 week or 4 weeks later, during which half of the event items were described inaccurately. Half of these suggestions were consistent with the theme of the detail across the occurrences (e.g., always sat on a kind of floor mat) or were inconsistent (e.g., sat on a chair). When memory for the target event was tested 1 day later, children falsely recognized fewer inconsistent suggestions than consistent suggestions, especially compared with a control group of children who experienced the event just one time. Furthermore, the longer delay reduced accuracy only for consistent suggestions. Source-monitoring ability was strongly and positively related to resistance to suggestions, and encouraging children to identify the source of false suggestions allowed them to retract a significant proportion of their reports of inconsistent suggestions but not of consistent suggestions. The results suggest that the gist consistency of suggestions determines whether event repetition increases or decreases suggestibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kim P Roberts
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ont., Canada N2L 3C5.
| | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Trocmé N, Bala N. False allegations of abuse and neglect when parents separate. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2005; 29:1333-45. [PMID: 16293307 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.06.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2003] [Revised: 06/13/2004] [Accepted: 06/20/2004] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The 1998 Canadian Incidence Study of Reported Child Abuse and Neglect (CIS-98) is the first national study to document the rate of intentionally false allegations of abuse and neglect investigated by child welfare services in Canada. This paper provides a detailed summary of the characteristics associated with intentionally false reports of child abuse and neglect within the context of parental separation. METHOD A multistage sampling design was used, first to select a representative sample of 51 child welfare service areas across Canada. Child maltreatment investigations conducted in the selected sites during the months of October-December 1998 were tracked, yielding a final sample of 7,672 child maltreatment investigations reported to child welfare authorities because of suspected child abuse or neglect. RESULTS Consistent with other national studies of reported child maltreatment, CIS-98 data indicate that more than one-third of maltreatment investigations are unsubstantiated, but only 4% of all cases are considered to be intentionally fabricated. Within the subsample of cases wherein a custody or access dispute has occurred, the rate of intentionally false allegations is higher: 12%. Results of this analysis show that neglect is the most common form of intentionally fabricated maltreatment, while anonymous reporters and noncustodial parents (usually fathers) most frequently make intentionally false reports. Of the intentionally false allegations of maltreatment tracked by the CIS-98, custodial parents (usually mothers) and children were least likely to fabricate reports of abuse or neglect. CONCLUSIONS While the CIS-98 documents that the rate of intentionally false allegations is relatively low, these results raise important clinical and legal issues, which require further consideration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nico Trocmé
- University of Toronto, Faculty of Social Work, 246 Bloor Street, West, Toronto, Ont., Canada M5S 1A1
| | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Peterson C, Parsons B. Interviewing former 1- and 2-year olds about medical emergencies 5 years later. LAW AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2005; 29:743-54. [PMID: 16382359 DOI: 10.1007/s10979-005-8378-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Abstract
Five years earlier, 1- and 2-year-old children who had been injured seriously enough to require hospital ER treatment had been recruited from the ER. For this study, as many of these children as could be found participated. The majority of former 1-year olds recalled nothing about these highly stressful events, whereas most former 2-year olds recalled a great deal. For those former 1-year olds who did recall the target events, quality of recall was problematic. In particular, they made considerable source confusions or intrusions into their accounts of details from other related events, producing an account that amalgamated various events into one recollection. Forensic implications are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carole Peterson
- Psychology Department, Memorial University, St John's, Newfoundland, Canada, A1B 3X9.
| | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Roberts KP, Powell MB. The relation between inhibitory control and children's eyewitness memory. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
41
|
Thomsen Y, Berntsen D. Knowing that I didn't know: preschoolers' understanding of their own false belief is a predictor of assents to fictitious events. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
42
|
Roberts KP, Powell MB. Evidence of metacognitive awareness in young children who have experienced a repeated event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
43
|
|
44
|
Clarke-Stewart KA, Malloy LC, Allhusen VD. Verbal ability, self-control, and close relationships with parents protect children against misleading suggestions. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1076] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
45
|
Jones DPH. Consistency in children's accounts of maltreatment. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2002; 26:975-976. [PMID: 12433140 DOI: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00366-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
|
46
|
Roberts KP. Children’s ability to distinguish between memories from multiple sources: Implications for the quality and accuracy of eyewitness statements. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2002. [DOI: 10.1016/s0273-2297(02)00005-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 76] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
47
|
McFarlane F, Powell MB. The Video Suggestibility Scale for Children: how generalizable is children's performance to other measures of suggestibility? BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES & THE LAW 2002; 20:699-716. [PMID: 12465135 DOI: 10.1002/bsl.492] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
This study explored the generalizability of the Video Suggestibility Scale for Children (VSSC), which was developed by Scullin and colleagues (Scullin & Ceci, 2001; Scullin & Hembrooke, 1998) as a tool for discriminating among children (aged three to five years) who have different levels of suggestibility. The VSSC consists of two subscales; Yield (a measure of children's willingness to acquiesce to misleading questions) and Shift (a measure of children's tendency to change their responses after feedback from the interviewer). Children's (N = 77) performance on each of the subscales was compared with their performance using several other measures of suggestibility. These measures included children's willingness to assent to a false event as well as the number of false interviewer suggestions and false new details that the children provided when responding to cued-recall questions about an independent true-biased and an independent false (non-experienced) event. An independent samples t-test revealed that those children who assented to the false event generated higher scores on the Yield measure. Hierarchical regression analyses revealed that Yield was a significant predictor of the number of false details reported about the false activity, but not the true-biased activity. There was no significant relationship between the Shift subscale and any of the dependent variables. The potential contribution of the VSSC for forensic researchers and practitioners is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Felicity McFarlane
- School of Psychology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, 3125, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
48
|
Jones DP. Interviewing children about individual incidents of sexual abuse. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2001; 25:1641-1642. [PMID: 11814160 DOI: 10.1016/s0145-2134(01)00297-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- D P Jones
- Park Hospital for Children, Headington, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|