1
|
Piva A, Benvegnù G, Negri S, Commisso M, Ceccato S, Avesani L, Guzzo F, Chiamulera C. Whole Plant Extracts for Neurocognitive Disorders: A Narrative Review of Neuropsychological and Preclinical Studies. Nutrients 2024; 16:3156. [PMID: 39339756 PMCID: PMC11434991 DOI: 10.3390/nu16183156] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2024] [Revised: 09/09/2024] [Accepted: 09/13/2024] [Indexed: 09/30/2024] Open
Abstract
The incidence of neurodegenerative disorders like Alzheimer's or Parkinson's Disease, characterized by a progressive cognitive decline, is rising worldwide. Despite the considerable efforts to unveil the neuropsychological bases of these diseases, there is still an unmet medical need for effective therapies against cognitive deficits. In recent years, increasing laboratory evidence indicates the potential of phytotherapy as an integrative aid to improve cognitive functions. In this review, we describe the data of plant whole extracts or single compounds' efficacy on validated preclinical models and neuropsychological tests, aiming to correlate brain mechanisms underlying rodent behavioral responses to human findings. After a search of the literature, the overview was limited to the following plants: Dioscorea batatas, Ginkgo biloba, Melissa officinalis, Nigella sativa, Olea europaea, Panax ginseng, Punica granatum, and Vitis vinifera. Results showed significant improvements in different cognitive functions, such as learning and memory or visuospatial abilities, in both humans and rodents. However, despite promising laboratory evidence, clinical translation has been dampened by a limited pharmacological characterization of the single bioactive components of the herbal products. Depicting the contribution of the single phytochemicals to the phytocomplex's pharmacological efficacy could enable the comprehension of their potential synergistic activity, leading to phytotherapy inclusion in the existing therapeutic package against cognitive decline.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alessandro Piva
- Department of Diagnostics and Public Health, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy; (A.P.); (G.B.); (C.C.)
- NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, 90133 Palermo, Italy; (S.N.); (M.C.); (L.A.); (F.G.)
| | - Giulia Benvegnù
- Department of Diagnostics and Public Health, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy; (A.P.); (G.B.); (C.C.)
| | - Stefano Negri
- NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, 90133 Palermo, Italy; (S.N.); (M.C.); (L.A.); (F.G.)
- Department of Biotechnology, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy
| | - Mauro Commisso
- NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, 90133 Palermo, Italy; (S.N.); (M.C.); (L.A.); (F.G.)
- Department of Biotechnology, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy
| | - Sofia Ceccato
- Department of Diagnostics and Public Health, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy; (A.P.); (G.B.); (C.C.)
| | - Linda Avesani
- NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, 90133 Palermo, Italy; (S.N.); (M.C.); (L.A.); (F.G.)
- Department of Biotechnology, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy
| | - Flavia Guzzo
- NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, 90133 Palermo, Italy; (S.N.); (M.C.); (L.A.); (F.G.)
- Department of Biotechnology, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy
| | - Cristiano Chiamulera
- Department of Diagnostics and Public Health, University of Verona, 37134 Verona, Italy; (A.P.); (G.B.); (C.C.)
- NBFC, National Biodiversity Future Center, 90133 Palermo, Italy; (S.N.); (M.C.); (L.A.); (F.G.)
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Özdemir Ş, Şentürk YD, Ünver N, Demircan C, Olivers CNL, Egner T, Günseli E. Effects of Context Changes on Memory Reactivation. J Neurosci 2024; 44:e2096232024. [PMID: 39103222 PMCID: PMC11376331 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2096-23.2024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2023] [Revised: 06/26/2024] [Accepted: 07/10/2024] [Indexed: 08/07/2024] Open
Abstract
While the influence of context on long-term memory (LTM) is well documented, its effects on the interaction between working memory (WM) and LTM remain less understood. In this study, we explored these interactions using a delayed match-to-sample task, where participants (6 males, 16 females) encountered the same target object across six consecutive trials, facilitating the transition from WM to LTM. During half of these target repetitions, the background color changed. We measured the WM storage of the target using the contralateral delay activity in electroencephalography. Our results reveal that task-irrelevant context changes trigger the reactivation of long-term memories in WM. This reactivation may be attributed to content-context binding in WM and hippocampal pattern separation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Şahcan Özdemir
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| | | | - Nursima Ünver
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| | - Can Demircan
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| | - Christian N L Olivers
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam 1081 BT, the Netherlands
| | - Tobias Egner
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708
| | - Eren Günseli
- Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Biggs AT, Pettijohn KA, Blacker KJ. Contextual cueing during lethal force training: How target design and repetition can alter threat assessments. MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 36:353-365. [PMID: 38661462 PMCID: PMC11057649 DOI: 10.1080/08995605.2023.2178785] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2020] [Accepted: 04/27/2021] [Indexed: 03/06/2023]
Abstract
Lethal force training requires individuals to make threat assessments, which involves holistic scenario processing to identify potential threats. Photorealistic targets can make threat/non-threat judgments substantially more genuine and challenging compared to simple cardboard or silhouette targets. Unfortunately, repeated target use also brings unintended consequences that could invalidate threat assessment processes conducted during training. Contextually rich or unique targets could be implicitly memorable in a way that allows observers to recall weapon locations rather than forcing observers to conduct a naturalistic assessment. Experiment 1 demonstrated robust contextual cueing effects in a well-established shoot/don't-shoot stimulus set, and Experiment 2 extended this finding from complex scene stimuli to simple actor-only stimuli. Experiment 3 demonstrated that these effects also occurred among trained professionals using rifles rather than computer-based tasks. Taken together, these findings demonstrate the potential for uncontrolled target repetition to alter the fundamental processes of threat assessment during lethal force training.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam T. Biggs
- Medical Department, Naval Special Warfare Command, Coronado, California
| | - Kyle A. Pettijohn
- Aeromedical Department, Naval Medical Research Unit – Dayton, Wright-Patterson AFB, Dayton, Ohio
| | - Kara J. Blacker
- Aeromedical Department, Naval Medical Research Unit – Dayton, Wright-Patterson AFB, Dayton, Ohio
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Chen Z, Sun Q, Li X. Differences of resource allocation to active and passive states in visual working memory. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2023; 87:1761-1767. [PMID: 36436109 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01772-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2022] [Accepted: 11/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Scholars have long sought to understand the separation between the active and passive states in visual working memory. Results of recent behavioral studies have provided insight into the independence of storage resources in these two states. To explore how humans distribute these resources in the active and passive states in visual working memory, we adopted the classic double-retro-cue paradigm combined with a continuous reported color wheel to ascertain whether the precision of representations maintained in active and passive states are adjustable according to the frequency of spatial cues. The results showed that two distinct resource allocation mechanisms exist in these two states beyond traditional visual working memory theory and provide further support for the separation hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhen Chen
- Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, 688 Yingbin Road, Jinhua, China
| | - Qi Sun
- Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, 688 Yingbin Road, Jinhua, China.
- Key Laboratory of Intelligent Education Technology and Application of Zhejiang Province, Zhejiang Normal University, Zhejiang, China.
| | - Xinyu Li
- Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, 688 Yingbin Road, Jinhua, China.
- Key Laboratory of Intelligent Education Technology and Application of Zhejiang Province, Zhejiang Normal University, Zhejiang, China.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Wang H, Pan X, Wang J, Sun M, Wu C, Yu Q, Liu Z, Chen T, Liu Y. Dual functional states of working memory realized by memristor-based neural network. Front Neurosci 2023; 17:1192993. [PMID: 37351423 PMCID: PMC10282152 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1192993] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2023] [Accepted: 05/16/2023] [Indexed: 06/24/2023] Open
Abstract
Working memory refers to the brain's ability to store and manipulate information for a short period. It is disputably considered to rely on two mechanisms: sustained neuronal firing, and "activity-silent" working memory. To develop a highly biologically plausible neuromorphic computing system, it is anticipated to physically realize working memory that corresponds to both of these mechanisms. In this study, we propose a memristor-based neural network to realize the sustained neural firing and activity-silent working memory, which are reflected as dual functional states within memory. Memristor-based synapses and two types of artificial neurons are designed for the Winner-Takes-All learning rule. During the cognitive task, state transformation between the "focused" state and the "unfocused" state of working memory is demonstrated. This work paves the way for further emulating the complex working memory functions with distinct neural activities in our brains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hongzhe Wang
- State Key Laboratory of Electronic Thin Films and Integrated Devices, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | - Xinqiang Pan
- State Key Laboratory of Electronic Thin Films and Integrated Devices, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | - Junjie Wang
- State Key Laboratory of Electronic Thin Films and Integrated Devices, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | - Mingyuan Sun
- Beijing China Changfeng Electromechanical Technology Research and Design Institute, Beijing, China
| | - Chuangui Wu
- State Key Laboratory of Electronic Thin Films and Integrated Devices, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | - Qi Yu
- State Key Laboratory of Electronic Thin Films and Integrated Devices, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | - Zhen Liu
- School of Materials and Energy, Guangdong University of Technology, Guangzhou, China
| | - Tupei Chen
- School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Yang Liu
- State Key Laboratory of Electronic Thin Films and Integrated Devices, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
- Deepcreatic Technologies Ltd., Chengdu, China
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Wang K, Jiang Z, Huang S, Qian J. Increasing perceptual separateness affects working memory for depth - re-allocation of attention from boundaries to the fixated center. J Vis 2021; 21:8. [PMID: 34264289 PMCID: PMC8288055 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.7.8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2020] [Accepted: 05/25/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
For decades, working memory (WM) has been a heated research topic in the field of cognitive psychology. However, most studies on WM presented visual stimuli on a two-dimensional plane, rarely involving depth perception. Several previous studies have investigated how depth information is stored in WM, and found that WM for depth is even more limited in capacity and the memory performance is poor compared to visual WM. In the present study, we used a change detection task to investigate whether dissociating memory items by different visual features, thereby to increase their perceptual separateness, can improve WM performance for depth. Memory items presented at various depth planes were bound with different colors (Experiments 1 and 3) or sizes (Experiment 2). The memory performance for depth locations of visual stimuli with homogeneous and heterogeneous appearances were tested and compared. The results showed a consistent pattern that although separating items with various feature values did not affect the overall memory performance, the manipulation significantly improved memory performance for the middle depth locations but impaired the performance for the boundary locations when observers fixated at the center of the whole depth volume. The memory benefits of feature separation can be attributed to enhanced individuation of memory items, therefore facilitating a more balanced allocation of attention and memory resources.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaiyue Wang
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Zhuyuan Jiang
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Suqi Huang
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Jiehui Qian
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Raising awareness about measurement error in research on unconscious mental processes. Psychon Bull Rev 2021; 29:21-43. [PMID: 34131891 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-021-01923-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/23/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Experimental psychologists often neglect the poor psychometric properties of the dependent measures collected in their studies. In particular, a low reliability of measures can have dramatic consequences for the interpretation of key findings in some of the most popular experimental paradigms, especially when strong inferences are drawn from the absence of statistically significant correlations. In research on unconscious cognition, for instance, it is commonly argued that the lack of a correlation between task performance and measures of awareness or explicit recollection of the target stimuli provides strong support for the conclusion that the cognitive processes underlying performance must be unconscious. Using contextual cuing of visual search as a case study, we show that given the low reliability of the dependent measures collected in these studies, it is usually impossible to draw any firm conclusion about the unconscious character of this effect from correlational analyses. Furthermore, both a psychometric meta-analysis of the available evidence and a cognitive-modeling approach suggest that, in fact, we should expect to see very low correlations between performance and awareness at the empirical level, even if both constructs are perfectly related at the latent level. Convincing evidence for the unconscious character of contextual cuing and other effects will most likely demand richer and larger data sets, coupled with more powerful analytic approaches.
Collapse
|
8
|
Abstract
There has been considerable controversy in recent years as to whether information held in working memory (WM) is rapidly forgotten or automatically transferred to long-term memory (LTM). Although visual WM capacity is very limited, we appear able to store a virtually infinite amount of information in visual LTM. Still, LTM retrieval often fails. Some view visual WM as a mental sketchpad that is wiped clean when new information enters, but not a consistent precursor of LTM. Others view the WM and LTM systems as inherently linked. Distinguishing between these possibilities has been difficult, as attempts to directly manipulate the active holding of information in visual WM has typically introduced various confounds. Here, we capitalized on the WM system's capacity limitation to control the likelihood that visual information was actively held in WM. Our young-adult participants (N = 103) performed a WM task with unique everyday items, presented in groups of two, four, six, or eight items. Presentation time was adjusted according to the number of items. Subsequently, we tested participants' LTM for items from the WM task. LTM was better for items presented originally within smaller WM set sizes, indicating that WM limitations contribute to subsequent LTM failures, and that holding items in WM enhances LTM encoding. Our results suggest that a limit in WM capacity contributes to an LTM encoding bottleneck for trial-unique familiar objects, with a relatively large effect size.
Collapse
|
9
|
Training with high perceptual difficulty improves the capacity and fidelity of internal representation in VWM. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2020; 85:2408-2419. [PMID: 32809086 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-020-01404-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2020] [Accepted: 08/06/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
It has been shown that the capacity of visual working memory (VWM) is a strong predictor of individual intelligence, and researchers have developed various training protocols to improve VWM capacity. However, it seems that whether the fidelity of internal representation in VWM can also be improved by training is largely overlooked in the past literature. Here, we introduced a new training approach that involved increasing the perceptual difficulty of training materials to enhance VWM, and both memory capacity and the fidelity of representation were examined to assess the training efficacy. Participants with normal vision and cognitive abilities received 3-week training on VWM using a change detection task, and the results showed that both the capacity and the fidelity of memory representations were improved for training with perceptually difficult stimuli, while only the fidelity was improved for training with perceptually normal stimuli. In addition, we found that the training effects on memory precision may be subject to capacity constraints. We suggest that long-term adaptive training with perceptually difficult stimuli may facilitate encoding efficiency through familiarizing trainees with an increased baseline of cognitive workload during the encoding process. The present study offers clear evidence that training with high perceptual difficulty is more effective and the improvements in VWM are more stable than training with perceptually normal materials, and the simple manipulation on training stimuli indicates that the method can be generalized to a wider range of training situations and populations.
Collapse
|
10
|
Xu Z, Wang LC, Liu D, Chen Y, Tao L. The Moderation Effect of Processing Efficiency on the Relationship Between Visual Working Memory and Chinese Character Recognition. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1899. [PMID: 32849112 PMCID: PMC7422728 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01899] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2020] [Accepted: 07/09/2020] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
To investigate the underlying mechanism of the relationship between visual working memory (VWM) and Chinese character recognition, and the moderation effect of processing efficiency on this relationship, 154 first-grade students were administered a battery of tasks for VWM, rapid temporal processing, and Chinese character reading. In the VWM task, the children were asked to remember the jumping routes of a frog and report these routes in reverse sequence. The longest span for which each participant could respond correctly at least four times out of six was the VWM index. In the task of temporal order judgement, the participants were asked to select which of two balls was presented first, with stimulus onset asynchronies varying from 8 to 492 ms according to an adaptive psychophysical procedure. Visual temporal order threshold (VTOT) was utilized as an indicator of processing efficiency. The participants were asked to read 100 characters aloud to measure their word-level reading abilities in Chinese character recognition. After controlling age, non-verbal intelligence, visual short-term memory, morphological awareness, and orthographic awareness, the results of a moderation effect analysis showed that (1) both VWM and visual VTOT predicted Chinese character reading, and (2) the moderation effect of VTOT on the VWM-reading link was significant (p = 0.001). The correlation between VWM and Chinese character reading was positive and significant when VTOTs were above average (i.e., smaller than 87.14 ms); however, the correlation was negative at relatively poor levels of VTOTs (i.e., larger than 231.44 ms).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhengye Xu
- Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
| | - Li-Chih Wang
- Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
- Integrated Centre for Wellbeing, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
- Centre for Child and Family Science, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
| | - Duo Liu
- Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
- Integrated Centre for Wellbeing, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
- Centre for Child and Family Science, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
- *Correspondence: Duo Liu,
| | | | - Li Tao
- Xuefu Middle School, Shenzhen, China
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Abstract
We are capable of storing a virtually infinite amount of visual information in visual long-term memory (VLTM) storage. At the same time, the amount of visual information we can encode and maintain in visual short-term memory (VSTM) at a given time is severely limited. How do these two memory systems interact to accumulate vast amount of VLTM? In this series of experiments, we exploited interindividual and intraindividual differences VSTM capacity to examine the direct involvement of VSTM in determining the encoding rate (or "bandwidth") of VLTM. Here, we found that the amount of visual information encoded into VSTM at a given moment (i.e., VSTM capacity), but neither the maintenance duration nor the test process, predicts the effective encoding "bandwidth" of VLTM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Keisuke Fukuda
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Rd., Mississauga, ON, L5L 1C6, Canada.
| | - Edward K Vogel
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
|
13
|
|
14
|
Abstract
Long-term memory (LTM) can influence many aspects of short-term memory (STM), including increased STM span. However, it is unclear whether LTM enhances the quantitative or qualitative aspect of STM. That is, do we retain a larger number of representations or more precise representations in STM for familiar stimuli than unfamiliar stimuli? This study took advantage of participants' prior rich multimedia experience with Pokémon, without investing on laboratory training to examine how prior LTM influenced visual STM. In a Pokémon visual STM change detection task, participants remembered more first-generation Pokémon characters that they were more familiar with than recent-generation Pokémon characters that they were less familiar with. No significant difference in memory quality was found when quantitative and qualitative effects of LTM were isolated using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analyses. Critically, these effects were absent in participants who were unfamiliar with first-generation Pokémon. Furthermore, several alternative interpretations were ruled out, including general video-gaming experience, subjective Pokémon preference, and verbal encoding. Together, these results demonstrated a strong link between prior stimulus familiarity in LTM and visual STM storage capacity.
Collapse
|
15
|
Training Change Detection Leads to Substantial Task-Specific Improvement. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE ENHANCEMENT 2017. [DOI: 10.1007/s41465-017-0055-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
16
|
Affiliation(s)
- Bo-Yeong Won
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Andrew B. Leber
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Shin E, Lee H, Yoo SA, Chong SC. Training improves the capacity of visual working memory when it is adaptive, individualized, and targeted. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0121702. [PMID: 25836651 PMCID: PMC4383536 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121702] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2014] [Accepted: 02/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The current study investigated whether training improves the capacity of visual working memory using individualized adaptive training methods. Two groups of participants were trained for two targeted processes, filtering and consolidation. Before and after the training, the participants, including those with no training, performed a lateralized change detection task in which one side of the visual display had to be selected and the other side ignored. Across ten-day training sessions, the participants performed two modified versions of the lateralized change detection task. The number of distractors and duration of the consolidation period were adjusted individually to increase the task difficulty of the filtering and consolidation training, respectively. Results showed that the degree of improvement shown during the training was positively correlated with the increase in memory capacity, and training-induced benefits were most evident for larger set sizes in the filtering training group. These results suggest that visual working memory training is effective, especially when it is adaptive, individualized, and targeted.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eunsam Shin
- Center for Cognitive Science, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Hunjae Lee
- Graduate Program in Cognitive Science, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Sang-Ah Yoo
- Graduate Program in Cognitive Science, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Sang Chul Chong
- Graduate Program in Cognitive Science, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
- Department of Psychology, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Fudali-Czyż A, Francuz P, Augustynowicz P. Determinants of attentive blank stares. An EFRP study. Conscious Cogn 2014; 29:1-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2014] [Revised: 06/30/2014] [Accepted: 07/01/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
19
|
Abstract
Visual environments often contain multiple elements, some of which are similar to one another or spatially grouped together. In the current study we investigated how one can use perceptual groups in representing ensemble features of the groups. In experiment 1 we found that participants' performance improved when items were easily segmented by a grouping cue based on proximity, suggesting that spatial grouping facilitates extracting and remembering ensemble representations from visual arrays consisting of multiple elements. In experiment 2 we found that spatial grouping improved performance only when the grouped subsets were tested for the memory task, whereas it impaired performance when other subsets that were not grouped were tested, suggesting that the benefit from grouping may come from better extraction for storage, rather than later decision processes such as accessibility. Taken together, our results suggest that perceptual grouping of multiple items by proximity facilitates extraction of ensemble statistics from groups of items, enhancing visual memory of the ensembles in a visual array.
Collapse
|
20
|
Object grouping based on real-world regularities facilitates perception by reducing competitive interactions in visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014; 111:11217-22. [PMID: 25024190 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1400559111] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In virtually every real-life situation humans are confronted with complex and cluttered visual environments that contain a multitude of objects. Because of the limited capacity of the visual system, objects compete for neural representation and cognitive processing resources. Previous work has shown that such attentional competition is partly object based, such that competition among elements is reduced when these elements perceptually group into an object based on low-level cues. Here, using functional MRI (fMRI) and behavioral measures, we show that the attentional benefit of grouping extends to higher-level grouping based on the relative position of objects as experienced in the real world. An fMRI study designed to measure competitive interactions among objects in human visual cortex revealed reduced neural competition between objects when these were presented in commonly experienced configurations, such as a lamp above a table, relative to the same objects presented in other configurations. In behavioral visual search studies, we then related this reduced neural competition to improved target detection when distracter objects were shown in regular configurations. Control studies showed that low-level grouping could not account for these results. We interpret these findings as reflecting the grouping of objects based on higher-level spatial-relational knowledge acquired through a lifetime of seeing objects in specific configurations. This interobject grouping effectively reduces the number of objects that compete for representation and thereby contributes to the efficiency of real-world perception.
Collapse
|
21
|
Apter BJ. Do computerised training programmes designed to improve working memory work? EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY IN PRACTICE 2012. [DOI: 10.1080/02667363.2012.712915] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
22
|
Sørensen TA, Kyllingsbæk S. Short-term storage capacity for visual objects depends on expertise. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2012; 140:158-63. [PMID: 22627160 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2011] [Revised: 03/27/2012] [Accepted: 04/16/2012] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Visual short-term memory (VSTM) has traditionally been thought to have a very limited capacity of around 3-4 objects. However, recently several researchers have argued that VSTM may be limited in the amount of information retained rather than by a specific number of objects. Here we present a study of the effect of long-term practice on VSTM capacity. We investigated four age groups ranging from pre-school children to adults and measured the change in VSTM capacity for letters and pictures. We found a clear increase in VSTM capacity for letters with age but not for pictures. Our results indicate that VSTM capacity is dependent on the level of expertise for specific types of stimuli.
Collapse
|
23
|
Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA. A review of visual memory capacity: Beyond individual items and toward structured representations. J Vis 2011; 11:4. [PMID: 21617025 PMCID: PMC3405498 DOI: 10.1167/11.5.4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 262] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Traditional memory research has focused on identifying separate memory systems and exploring different stages of memory processing. This approach has been valuable for establishing a taxonomy of memory systems and characterizing their function but has been less informative about the nature of stored memory representations. Recent research on visual memory has shifted toward a representation-based emphasis, focusing on the contents of memory and attempting to determine the format and structure of remembered information. The main thesis of this review will be that one cannot fully understand memory systems or memory processes without also determining the nature of memory representations. Nowhere is this connection more obvious than in research that attempts to measure the capacity of visual memory. We will review research on the capacity of visual working memory and visual long-term memory, highlighting recent work that emphasizes the contents of memory. This focus impacts not only how we estimate the capacity of the system--going beyond quantifying how many items can be remembered and moving toward structured representations--but how we model memory systems and memory processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Timothy F. Brady
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
| | - Talia Konkle
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
| | - George A. Alvarez
- Vision Sciences Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Harvard University
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Umemoto A, Scolari M, Vogel EK, Awh E. Statistical learning induces discrete shifts in the allocation of working memory resources. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2011; 36:1419-29. [PMID: 20718564 DOI: 10.1037/a0019324] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Observers can voluntarily select which items are encoded into working memory, and the efficiency of this process strongly predicts memory capacity. Nevertheless, the present work suggests that voluntary intentions do not exclusively determine what is encoded into this online workspace. Observers indicated whether any items from a briefly stored sample display had changed. Unbeknown to observers, these changes were most likely to occur in a specific quadrant of the display (the dominant quadrant). Across 84 subjects and 5 groups of observers, change detection accuracy was significantly higher for items in the dominant quadrant, suggesting that memory encoding was biased towards the dominant quadrant. Only 9 of the 84 subjects were able to correctly specify the dominant quadrant when asked whether any location was more likely to contain the changed item, but more sensitive forced-choice procedures did reveal above-chance discrimination of the dominant quadrant. Nevertheless, because forced choice performance was unrelated to the size of the bias and no observer reported a biased encoding strategy, the bias was unlikely to depend on voluntary encoding strategies. The encoding bias was not due to a reduction in the response threshold for indicating changes in the dominant quadrant (Experiment 2). Finally, separate measures of the number and resolution of the representations in memory suggested that encoding was biased in a discrete slot-based fashion (Experiment 3). That is, although items in the dominant quadrant were more likely to be encoded into memory, mnemonic resolution for the favored items was not affected.
Collapse
|
25
|
Abstract
When multiple objects are presented briefly and simultaneously in a visual array, visual short-term memory (VSTM) can maintain only a limited number of these items. The present research report reveals that splitting the to-be-remembered items into two sequential arrays significantly increases VSTM performance relative to the simultaneous presentation of the same items. A memory benefit also emerges when the full object array is flashed twice (repeated) rather than being presented continuously for the same duration. Moreover, the sequential and repetition benefits are specifically pronounced for individuals with low performance for simultaneously presented items. Our results suggest that the conventional, simultaneous presentation mode may underestimate VSTM performance due to attentional limitations and/or competition between stimulus representations. In contrast, temporal segregation of the stimulus input may help participants maximize their performance and utilize their full VSTM capacity.
Collapse
|
26
|
Beck MR, Angelone BL, Levin DT, Peterson MS, Varakin DA. Implicit learning for probable changes in a visual change detection task. Conscious Cogn 2008; 17:1192-208. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2007] [Revised: 06/25/2008] [Accepted: 06/26/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
27
|
Hartshorne JK. Visual working memory capacity and proactive interference. PLoS One 2008; 3:e2716. [PMID: 18648493 PMCID: PMC2447156 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002716] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2008] [Accepted: 06/20/2008] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Visual working memory capacity is extremely limited and appears to be relatively immune to practice effects or the use of explicit strategies. The recent discovery that visual working memory tasks, like verbal working memory tasks, are subject to proactive interference, coupled with the fact that typical visual working memory tasks are particularly conducive to proactive interference, suggests that visual working memory capacity may be systematically under-estimated. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS Working memory capacity was probed behaviorally in adult humans both in laboratory settings and via the Internet. Several experiments show that although the effect of proactive interference on visual working memory is significant and can last over several trials, it only changes the capacity estimate by about 15%. CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE This study further confirms the sharp limitations on visual working memory capacity, both in absolute terms and relative to verbal working memory. It is suggested that future research take these limitations into account in understanding differences across a variety of tasks between human adults, prelinguistic infants and nonlinguistic animals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joshua K Hartshorne
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America.
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
Although it is intuitive that familiarity with complex visual objects should aid their preservation in visual working memory (WM), empirical evidence for this is lacking. This study used a conventional change-detection procedure to assess visual WM for unfamiliar and famous faces in healthy adults. Across experiments, faces were upright or inverted and a low- or high-load concurrent verbal WM task was administered to suppress contribution from verbal WM. Even with a high verbal memory load, visual WM performance was significantly better and capacity estimated as significantly greater for famous versus unfamiliar faces. Face inversion abolished this effect. Thus, neither strategic, explicit support from verbal WM nor low-level feature processing easily accounts for the observed benefit of high familiarity for visual WM. These results demonstrate that storage of items in visual WM can be enhanced if robust visual representations of them already exist in long-term memory.
Collapse
|
29
|
Patterson MD, Bly BM, Porcelli AJ, Rypma B. Visual working memory for global, object, and part-based information. Mem Cognit 2007; 35:738-51. [PMID: 17848031 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193311] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
We investigated visual working memory for novel objects and parts of novel objects. After a delay period, participants showed strikingly more accurate performance recognizing a single whole object than the parts of that object. This bias to remember whole objects, rather than parts, persisted even when the division between parts was clearly defined and the parts were disconnected from each other so that, in order to remember the single whole object, the participants needed to mentally combine the parts. In addition, the bias was confirmed when the parts were divided by color. These experiments indicated that holistic perceptual-grouping biases are automatically used to organize storage in visual working memory. In addition, our results suggested that the bias was impervious to top-down consciously directed control, because when task demands were manipulated through instruction and catch trials, the participants still recognized whole objects more quickly and more accurately than their parts. This bias persisted even when the whole objects were novel and the parts were familiar. We propose that visual working memory representations depend primarily on the global configural properties of whole objects, rather than part-based representations, even when the parts themselves can be clearly perceived as individual objects. This global configural bias beneficially reduces memory load on a capacity-limited system operating in a complex visual environment, because fewer distinct items must be remembered.
Collapse
|
30
|
Abstract
A critical question in visual working or short-term memory (VSTM) research is whether the ability to remember briefly presented visual stimuli can be increased. Here we test whether VSTM for locations and shapes is improved by training that allows one to utilize another memory system, visual longterm memory (VLTM). Training was done by repeatedly presenting a subset of memory displays, creating long-term memory traces for these displays. Surprisingly, VSTM performance for repeated displays was not higher than for nonrepeated ones, even though participants recognized repeated displays on a forced-choice test given at the end of the experiment. We suggest that the fidelity of information held by VLTM is inferior to that of information held by VSTM and thus provides no additional benefit over what is extracted on the fly by VSTM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ingrid R Olson
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104-6241, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|