1
|
A nonspatial sound modulates processing of visual distractors in a flanker task. Atten Percept Psychophys 2020; 83:800-809. [PMID: 33083988 PMCID: PMC8577790 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-020-02161-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Successful navigation of information-rich, multimodal environments involves processing of both auditory and visual information. The extent to which information within each modality is processed varies due to many factors, but the influence of auditory stimuli on the processing of visual stimuli in these multimodal environments is not well understood. Previous research has shown that a preceding sound leads to decreased reaction times in visual tasks (Bertelson, 1967). The current study examines if a non-spatial, task-irrelevant sound additionally alters processing of visual distractors that flank a central target. We utilized a version of a flanker task in which participants responded to a central letter surrounded by two irrelevant flanker letters. When these flankers are associated with a conflicting response, a congruency effect occurs such that reaction time to the target is slowed (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). In two experiments using this task, results showed that a preceding tone caused general speeding of reaction time across flanker types, consistent with alerting. The tone also caused decreased variation in response time. Critically, the tone modulated the congruency effect, with a greater speeding for congruent flankers than for incongruent flankers. This suggests that the influence of flanker identity was more intense after tone presentation, consistent with a nonspatial sound increasing perceptual and/or response-association processing of flanking stimuli.
Collapse
|
2
|
Fisk GD, Haase SJ. Classic Stroop Color Words Produce No Stroop Effect When the Display Characteristics Are Based Upon Emotional Stroop Studies With Subliminal Presentations. Psychol Rep 2019; 123:1207-1225. [PMID: 31060460 DOI: 10.1177/0033294119843220] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Two forms of the Stroop task have produced contradictory findings regarding unconscious perceptual processing. Emotional Stroop task studies with prime words presented at an objective threshold (i.e., subliminal) produce Stroop-like effects, but comparable studies conducted with classic Stroop stimuli do not produce Stroop effects. We tested the possibility that differences in the display appearance might explain this discrepancy. Color word prime stimuli from the traditional Stroop task were used with display characteristics based upon the emotional Stroop studies. There was a Stroop effect for the relatively long prime stimulus durations (59, 87, or 108 milliseconds) but not for the brief durations (18, 24, or 38 milliseconds). Accordingly, the discrepancy in research findings cannot be attributed to simple differences in display methodology. The failure to find strong evidence of unconscious perceptual processing is consistent with the negative findings from some emotional Stroop studies that use subliminal stimulus presentations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gary D Fisk
- Department of Psychology and Sociology, Georgia Southwestern State University, Americus, GA, USA
| | - Steven J Haase
- Department of Psychology, Shippensburg University, Shippensburg, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Yeh SL, Chou WL, Ho P. Lexical processing of Chinese sub-character components: Semantic activation of phonetic radicals as revealed by the Stroop effect. Sci Rep 2017; 7:15782. [PMID: 29150618 PMCID: PMC5693949 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-15536-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2017] [Accepted: 10/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Most Chinese characters are compounds consisting of a semantic radical indicating semantic category and a phonetic radical cuing the pronunciation of the character. Controversy surrounds whether radicals also go through the same lexical processing as characters and, critically, whether phonetic radicals involve semantic activation since they can also be characters when standing alone. Here we examined these issues using the Stroop task whereby participants responded to the ink color of the character. The key finding was that Stroop effects were found when the character itself had a meaning unrelated to color, but contained a color name phonetic radical (e.g., “guess”, with the phonetic radical “cyan”, on the right) or had a meaning associated with color (e.g., “pity”, with the phonetic radical “blood” on the right which has a meaning related to “red”). Such Stroop effects from the phonetic radical within a character unrelated to color support that Chinese character recognition involves decomposition of characters into their constituent radicals; with each of their meanings including phonetic radicals activated independently, even though it would inevitably interfere with that of the whole character. Compared with the morphological decomposition in English whereby the semantics of the morphemes are not necessarily activated, the unavoidable semantic activation of phonetic radicals represents a unique feature in Chinese character processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Su-Ling Yeh
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan. .,Graduate Institute of Brain and Mind Sciences, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan. .,Neurobiology and Cognitive Science Center, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.
| | - Wei-Lun Chou
- Department of Psychology, Fo Guang University, Yilan, Taiwan
| | - Pokuan Ho
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
The cognitive loci of the display and task-relevant set size effects on distractor interference: Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Atten Percept Psychophys 2017; 80:337-351. [PMID: 29124674 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-017-1442-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The congruency effect of a task-irrelevant distractor has been found to be modulated by task-relevant set size and display set size. The present study used a psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm to examine the cognitive loci of the display set size effect (dilution effect) and the task-relevant set size effect (perceptual load effect) on distractor interference. A tone discrimination task (Task 1), in which a response was made to the pitch of the target tone, was followed by a letter discrimination task (Task 2) in which different types of visual target display were used. In Experiment 1, in which display set size was manipulated to examine the nature of the display set size effect on distractor interference in Task 2, the modulation of the congruency effect by display set size was observed at both short and long stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOAs), indicating that the display set size effect occurred after the target was selected for processing in the focused attention stage. In Experiment 2, in which task-relevant set size was manipulated to examine the nature of the task-relevant set size effect on distractor interference in Task 2, the effects of task-relevant set size increased with SOA, suggesting that the target selection efficiency in the preattentive stage was impaired with increasing task-relevant set size. These results suggest that display set size and task-relevant set size modulate distractor processing in different ways.
Collapse
|
5
|
Amer T, Gozli DG, Pratt J. Biasing spatial attention with semantic information: an event coding approach. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2017; 82:840-858. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-017-0867-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2016] [Accepted: 04/08/2017] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
|
6
|
Wang J, Tang H, Deng Y. Novel Symbol Learning-Induced Stroop Effect: Evidence for a Strategy-Based, Utility Learning Model. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2016; 45:1161-1171. [PMID: 26435316 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-015-9397-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
The automaticity level and attention priority/strategy are two major theories that have attempted to explain the mechanism underlying the Stroop effect. Training is an effective way to manipulate the experience with the two dimensions (ink color and color word) in the Stroop task. In order to distinguish the above two factors (the automaticity or attention/strategy), we revised the training paradigm of MacLeod's study (J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14(1):126-135, 1988) by adding a control condition for the Stroop task on Chinese. We found that with training, the changing pattern for the Stroop effect was similar in Stroop tasks in novel symbols and in Chinese, showing markedly increasing interference and marginally decreasing facilitation. The current findings support the strategy-based learning account at early stages of novel learning of written symbols.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jin Wang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 16# Lincui Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing, 100101, People's Republic of China
- University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, People's Republic of China
| | - Huijun Tang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 16# Lincui Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing, 100101, People's Republic of China
- University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, People's Republic of China
| | - Yuan Deng
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 16# Lincui Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing, 100101, People's Republic of China.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Garcia-Marques T, Fernandes A, Fonseca R, Prada M. Social presence and the composite face effect. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 158:61-6. [PMID: 25939138 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2014] [Revised: 04/01/2015] [Accepted: 04/06/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022] Open
Abstract
A robust finding in social psychology research is that performance is modulated by the social nature of a given context, promoting social inhibition or facilitation effects. In the present experiment, we examined if and how social presence impacts holistic face perception processes by asking participants, in the presence of others and alone, to perform the composite face task. Results suggest that completing the task in the presence of others (i.e., mere co-action) is associated with better performance in face recognition (less bias and higher discrimination between presented and non-presented targets) and with a reduction in the composite face effect. These results make clear that social presence impact on the composite face effect does not occur because presence increases reliance on holistic processing as a "dominant" well-learned response, but instead, because it increases monitoring of the interference produced by automatic response.
Collapse
|
8
|
A Stroop effect emerges in the processing of complex Chinese characters that contain a color-related radical. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2014; 79:221-9. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-014-0553-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2013] [Accepted: 02/19/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
9
|
Fackrell K, Edmondson-Jones M, Hall DA. A controlled approach to the emotional dilution of the Stroop effect. PLoS One 2013; 8:e80141. [PMID: 24223219 PMCID: PMC3819280 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2013] [Accepted: 10/01/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
We re-examined a modified emotional Stroop task that included an additional colour-word alongside the emotional word, providing the response conflict of the traditional Stroop task. Negative emotionally salient (i.e. unpleasant') words are claimed to capture attention, producing a smaller Stroop effect for negative words compared to neutral words; this phenomenon is called the emotional dilution of the Stroop effect. To address previous limitations, this study compared negative words with lexically matched neutral words in a powered sample of 45 participants. Results demonstrated an emotional Stroop effect (slower colour-naming responses for negative words) and a traditional Stroop effect but not an emotional dilution of the Stroop effect. This finding is at odds with claims that other processing resources are diminished through the failure to disengage attention from emotional information. No matter how attention towards emotional information builds up over time, our findings indicate that attentional resources are not fully captured by negative words.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn Fackrell
- Division of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, United Kingdom
- NIHR Nottingham Hearing Biomedical Research Unit, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Mark Edmondson-Jones
- NIHR Nottingham Hearing Biomedical Research Unit, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
| | - Deborah A. Hall
- Division of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, United Kingdom
- NIHR Nottingham Hearing Biomedical Research Unit, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Spatial Stroop interference occurs in the processing of radicals of ideogrammic compounds. Psychon Bull Rev 2013; 21:715-20. [PMID: 24186269 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0533-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
11
|
Likelihood of attending to the color word modulates Stroop interference. Atten Percept Psychophys 2011; 74:416-29. [PMID: 22147533 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-011-0250-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
12
|
|
13
|
Even frequent and expected words are not identified without spatial attention. Atten Percept Psychophys 2010; 72:973-88. [PMID: 20436194 DOI: 10.3758/app.72.4.973] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies have disagreed about the extent to which people extract meaning from words presented outside the focus of spatial attention. The present study examined a possible explanation for such discrepancies inspired by attenuation theory: Unattended words can be read more automatically when they are expected within a given context (e.g., due to frequent repetition). We presented a brief prime word in lowercase, followed by a target word in uppercase. Participants indicated whether the target word belonged to a particular category (e.g., "sports"). When we used a visual cue to draw attention to the location of the prime word, it produced substantial priming effects on target responses (i.e., especially fast responses when the prime and target words were identical or from the same category). When prime words were not attended, however, they produced no priming effects. This finding replicated even when there were only four words, each repeated 160 times during the experiment. It appears that very little word processing is possible without spatial attention, even for words that are expected and frequently presented.
Collapse
|
14
|
Are spatial and dimensional attention separate? Evidence from Posner, Stroop, and Eriksen tasks. Mem Cognit 2009; 37:924-34. [PMID: 19679870 DOI: 10.3758/mc.37.6.924] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
15
|
Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return. Mem Cognit 2009; 37:935-44. [PMID: 19679871 DOI: 10.3758/mc.37.6.935] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
16
|
Is attention needed for word identification? Evidence from the Stroop paradigm. Psychon Bull Rev 2008; 15:950-5. [DOI: 10.3758/pbr.15.5.950] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
17
|
Brebner JL, Macrae CN. Faces, flowers and football boots: Capacity limits in distractor processing. Cognition 2008; 107:718-28. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.08.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2007] [Revised: 07/11/2007] [Accepted: 08/20/2007] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|