1
|
Guo T, Wang X, Wu J, Schwieter WJ, Liu H. Effects of contextualized emotional conflict control on domain-general conflict control: fMRI evidence of neural network reconfiguration. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2024; 19:nsae001. [PMID: 38174430 PMCID: PMC10868129 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2023] [Revised: 10/24/2023] [Accepted: 01/03/2024] [Indexed: 01/05/2024] Open
Abstract
Domain-general conflict control refers to the cognitive process in which individuals suppress task-irrelevant information and extract task-relevant information. It supports both effective implementation of cognitive conflict control and emotional conflict control. The present study employed functional magnetic resonance imaging and adopted an emotional valence conflict task and the arrow version of the flanker task to induce contextualized emotional conflicts and cognitive conflicts, respectively. The results from the conjunction analysis showed that the multitasking-related activity in the pre-supplementary motor area, bilateral dorsal premotor cortices, the left posterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS), the left anterior IPS and the right inferior occipital gyrus represents common subprocesses for emotional and cognitive conflict control, either in parallel or in close succession. These brain regions were used as nodes in the domain-general conflict control network. The results from the analyses on the brain network connectivity patterns revealed that emotional conflict control reconfigures the domain-general conflict control network in a connective way as evidenced by different communication and stronger connectivity among the domain-general conflict control network. Together, these findings offer the first empirical-based elaboration on the brain network underpinning emotional conflict control and how it reconfigures the domain-general conflict control network in interactive ways.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tingting Guo
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
- Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Dalian, Liaoning Province 116029, China
| | - Xiyuan Wang
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
- Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Dalian, Liaoning Province 116029, China
| | - Junjie Wu
- Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300382, China
| | - W. John Schwieter
- Language Acquisition, Multilingualism, and Cognition Laboratory/Bilingualism Matters, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo N2L3C5, Canada
- Department of Linguistics and Languages, McMaster University, Hamilton L8S4L8, Canada
| | - Huanhuan Liu
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
- Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Dalian, Liaoning Province 116029, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bi T, Luo W, Wu J, Shao B, Tan Q, Kou H. Effect of facial emotion recognition learning transfers across emotions. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1310101. [PMID: 38312392 PMCID: PMC10834736 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1310101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2023] [Accepted: 01/05/2024] [Indexed: 02/06/2024] Open
Abstract
Introduction Perceptual learning of facial expression is shown specific to the train expression, indicating separate encoding of the emotional contents in different expressions. However, little is known about the specificity of emotional recognition training with the visual search paradigm and the sensitivity of learning to near-threshold stimuli. Methods In the present study, we adopted a visual search paradigm to measure the recognition of facial expressions. In Experiment 1 (Exp1), Experiment 2 (Exp2), and Experiment 3 (Exp3), subjects were trained for 8 days to search for a target expression in an array of faces presented for 950 ms, 350 ms, and 50 ms, respectively. In Experiment 4 (Exp4), we trained subjects to search for a target of a triangle, and tested them with the task of facial expression search. Before and after the training, subjects were tested on the trained and untrained facial expressions which were presented for 950 ms, 650 ms, 350 ms, or 50 ms. Results The results showed that training led to large improvements in the recognition of facial emotions only if the faces were presented long enough (Exp1: 85.89%; Exp2: 46.05%). Furthermore, the training effect could transfer to the untrained expression. However, when the faces were presented briefly (Exp3), the training effect was small (6.38%). In Exp4, the results indicated that the training effect could not transfer across categories. Discussion Our findings revealed cross-emotion transfer for facial expression recognition training in a visual search task. In addition, learning hardly affects the recognition of near-threshold expressions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taiyong Bi
- Research Center of Humanities and Medicine, Zunyi Medical University, Zunyi, China
| | - Wei Luo
- The Institute of Ethnology and Anthropology, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Jia Wu
- Research Center of Humanities and Medicine, Zunyi Medical University, Zunyi, China
| | - Boyao Shao
- Research Center of Humanities and Medicine, Zunyi Medical University, Zunyi, China
| | - Qingli Tan
- Research Center of Humanities and Medicine, Zunyi Medical University, Zunyi, China
| | - Hui Kou
- Research Center of Humanities and Medicine, Zunyi Medical University, Zunyi, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Thierrée S, Raulin-Briot M, Legrand M, Le Gouge A, Vancappel A, Tudorache AC, Brizard B, Clarys D, Caille A, El-Hage W. Combining Trauma Script Exposure With rTMS to Reduce Symptoms of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder: Randomized Controlled Trial. Neuromodulation 2022; 25:549-557. [PMID: 35667770 DOI: 10.1111/ner.13505] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2021] [Revised: 06/20/2021] [Accepted: 06/29/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Innovative therapeutic interventions for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are required. We opted to facilitate fear extinction by combining trauma script exposure with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to reduce symptoms of PTSD. OBJECTIVE The efficacy and safety of 10 Hz rTMS of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex simultaneously with exposure to personal traumatic narrative were studied in patients with PTSD. MATERIALS AND METHODS This trial was a single-center randomized controlled trial (NCT02584894). Patients were randomly assigned 1:1 to receive eight daily sessions of 110% of motor threshold high frequency (HF) 10 Hz rTMS (110% HF rTMS) or 70% low frequency (LF) 1 Hz rTMS (70% LF rTMS) with trauma script exposure in both groups. Severity of PTSD, depression, and anxiety were assessed before and after study treatment (one month, three months) by an assessor masked to the trial group assignment. The primary outcome was the severity of PTSD assessed by the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS). We used mixed linear regression models for statistical comparisons. RESULTS Thirty-eight patients (65.8% females) were randomly assigned to 110% HF rTMS (n = 18, 31.3 ± 10.0 years, 13 females) or 70% LF rTMS (n = 20, 33.5 ± 11.1 years, 12 females). From baseline to three months, mean CAPS scores decreased by 51% in the 110% HF rTMS group (from 83.7 ± 14.4 to 41.8 ± 31.9) and by 36.9% in the 70% LF rTMS group (from 81.8 ± 15.6 to 51.6 ± 23.7), but with no significant difference in improvement (time by treatment interaction -3.61 [95% confidence interval (CI), -9.70 to 2.47]; p = 0.24; effect size 0.53). One serious adverse event occurred during the study (psychogenic nonepileptic seizure). CONCLUSION We found no evidence of difference in clinical improvement or remission rates between the 110% HF and 70% LF stimulation. These findings may reflect the importance of exposure procedure and that larger number of participants is needed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Thierrée
- UMR 1253, iBrain, Université de Tours, Inserm, Tours, France
| | | | - Marc Legrand
- UMR 1253, iBrain, Université de Tours, Inserm, Tours, France
| | | | - Alexis Vancappel
- UMR 1253, iBrain, Université de Tours, Inserm, Tours, France; Clinique Psychiatrique Universitaire, CHRU de Tours, Tours, France
| | - Andrei-Cristian Tudorache
- UMR CNRS 7295, Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l'Apprentissage, Université de Poitiers, Poitiers, France
| | - Bruno Brizard
- UMR 1253, iBrain, Université de Tours, Inserm, Tours, France
| | - David Clarys
- UMR CNRS 7295, Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l'Apprentissage, Université de Poitiers, Poitiers, France
| | - Agnès Caille
- CIC 1415, CHRU Tours, Inserm, Tours, France; SPHERE, UMR 1246, Université de Tours, Université de Nantes, Inserm, Tours, France
| | - Wissam El-Hage
- UMR 1253, iBrain, Université de Tours, Inserm, Tours, France; Clinique Psychiatrique Universitaire, CHRU de Tours, Tours, France; CIC 1415, CHRU Tours, Inserm, Tours, France; CHRU de Tours, CIC 1415, Inserm, Tours, France.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Tipura E, Pegna AJ. Subliminal emotional faces do not capture attention under high attentional load in a randomized trial presentation. VISUAL COGNITION 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2060397] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Eda Tipura
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Methodology and Data Analysis, Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- Laboratory of Behavioral Neurology and Imaging of Cognition, Department of Neuroscience, University Medical Center, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Alan J. Pegna
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Phills CE, Hahn A, Gawronski B. The Bidirectional Causal Relation Between Implicit Stereotypes and Implicit Prejudice. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2020; 46:1318-1330. [DOI: 10.1177/0146167219899234] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Although stereotypes and prejudice are commonly regarded as conceptually distinct but related constructs, previous research remains silent on the processes underlying their relation. Applying the balance-congruity principle to the concepts (a) group, (b) valence, and (c) attribute, we argue that the valence of attributes contained in a group-stereotype shapes evaluations of the group, while prejudice toward a group influences which attributes are stereotypically associated with the group. Using fictitious (Experiments 1 and 3) and real (Experiments 2 and 4) groups, the current studies demonstrate that (a) experimentally induced changes in the valence of semantic attributes associated with a group (stereotypes) influence implicit prejudice toward that group (Experiments 1 and 2), and (b) experimentally induced changes in the valence of a group (prejudice) influence implicit stereotyping of that group (Experiments 3 and 4). These findings demonstrate a bidirectional causal relation between prejudice and stereotypes.
Collapse
|
6
|
The effect of emotional primes on attentional focus in high versus low depression. Atten Percept Psychophys 2020; 82:377-382. [DOI: 10.3758/s13414-019-01833-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
7
|
Houston JR, Pollock JW, Lien MC, Allen PA. Emotional arousal deficit or emotional regulation bias? An electrophysiological study of age-related differences in emotion perception. Exp Aging Res 2018; 44:187-205. [PMID: 29578840 DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2018.1449585] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Background/Study context: Adult age differences in emotion processing have been attributed to age-related decline in earlier emotional perception and age-related bias in later emotional regulation. Yet, the relationship between the processes of early emotion perception and bias in emotional regulation and their influence on behavioral outcomes remains unclear. Event-related potentials (ERPs) have the temporal precision to allow for the online measure of neurophysiological activity and provide potential insight into the complex dynamics of emotion processing and aging. METHODS ERPs were used as the primary measure to examine the hypotheses that younger adults will differ in emotional arousal and emotional bias as represented by the early P1 waveform and later P3 waveform, respectively. Thirty-two younger and older adults (16 each) performed a facial emotion discrimination task in which they identified standardized angry, happy, or neutral expressions of faces from the NimStim database. RESULTS Younger adults showed a greater P1 ERP for angry faces relative to happy faces at parietal channels, while older adults did not exhibit any emotional modulation of the P1. In contrast, both younger and older adults showed a greater late P3 ERP for angry faces compared to happy faces. CONCLUSION The authors' results provide evidence for an age-related deficit in early emotion perception and autonomic arousal. Younger adults, but not older adults, exhibited a pattern of neurophysiological activity believed to reflect preconscious and reflexive identification of threat. Despite these age group differences in early emotion processing, younger and older adults did not exhibit differences in neurophysiological processes believed to reflect emotion regulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James R Houston
- a Department of Psychology , University of Akron , Akron , OH , USA
| | - Joshua W Pollock
- b Department of Sociology , Kent State University , Kent , OH , USA
| | - Mei-Ching Lien
- c Department of Psychology , Oregon State University , Corvallis , OR , USA
| | - Philip A Allen
- d Department of Psychology , University of Akron , Akron , OH , USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Trapp S, Kotz SA. Predicting Affective Information - An Evaluation of Repetition Suppression Effects. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1365. [PMID: 27667980 PMCID: PMC5016514 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01365] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2016] [Accepted: 08/26/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Both theoretical proposals and empirical studies suggest that the brain interprets sensory input based on expectations to mitigate computational burden. However, as social beings, much of sensory input is affectively loaded – e.g., the smile of a partner, the critical voice of a boss, or the welcoming gesture of a friend. Given that affective information is highly complex and often ambiguous, building up expectations of upcoming affective sensory input may greatly contribute to its rapid and efficient processing. This review points to the role of affective information in the context of the ‘predictive brain’. It particularly focuses on repetition suppression (RS) effects that have recently been linked to prediction processes. The findings are interpreted as evidence for more pronounced prediction processes with affective material. Importantly, it is argued that bottom-up attention inflates the neural RS effect, and because affective stimuli tend to attract more bottom-up attention, it thereby particularly overshadows the magnitude of RS effects for this information. Finally, anxiety disorders, such as social phobia, are briefly discussed as manifestations of modulations in affective prediction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sabrina Trapp
- Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan Israel
| | - Sonja A Kotz
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, LeipzigGermany; Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, MaastrichtNetherlands
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Sokka L, Huotilainen M, Leinikka M, Korpela J, Henelius A, Alain C, Müller K, Pakarinen S. Alterations in attention capture to auditory emotional stimuli in job burnout: An event-related potential study. Int J Psychophysiol 2014; 94:427-36. [PMID: 25448269 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.11.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2014] [Revised: 10/29/2014] [Accepted: 11/02/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Sokka
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland.
| | - Minna Huotilainen
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Marianne Leinikka
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Jussi Korpela
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Andreas Henelius
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Claude Alain
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, 3560 Bathurst Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M6A 2E1; Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Kiti Müller
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland
| | - Satu Pakarinen
- Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Blanchette I, El-Deredy W. An ERP investigation of conditional reasoning with emotional and neutral contents. Brain Cogn 2014; 91:45-53. [PMID: 25222293 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2013] [Revised: 07/25/2014] [Accepted: 08/06/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
In two experiments we investigate conditional reasoning using event-related potentials (ERPs). Our goal was to examine the time course of inference making in two conditional forms, one logically valid (Modus Ponens, MP) and one logically invalid (Affirming the Consequent, AC). We focus particularly on the involvement of semantically-based inferential processes potentially marked by modulations of the N400. We also compared reasoning about emotional and neutral contents with separate sets of stimuli of differing linguistic complexity across the two experiments. Both MP and AC modulated the N400 component, suggesting the involvement of a semantically-based inferential mechanism common across different logical forms, content types, and linguistic features of the problems. Emotion did not have an effect on early components, and did not interact with components related to inference making. There was a main effect of emotion in the 800-1050 ms time window, consistent with an effect on sustained attention. The results suggest that conditional reasoning is not a purely formal process but that it importantly implicates semantic processing, and that the effect of emotion on reasoning does not primarily operate through a modulation of early automatic stages of information processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Wael El-Deredy
- School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Caparos S, Blanchette I. Emotional Stroop interference in trauma-exposed individuals: A contrast between two accounts. Conscious Cogn 2014; 28:104-12. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.06.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2013] [Revised: 05/16/2014] [Accepted: 06/28/2014] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
12
|
Valdivia-Salas S, Forsyth JP, Berghoff CR, Ritzert TR. Using panicogenic inhalations of carbon dioxide enriched air to induce attentional bias for threat: Implications for the development of anxiety disorders. Cogn Emot 2014; 28:1474-82. [DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.883964] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
13
|
Selinger L, Domínguez-Borràs J, Escera C. Phasic boosting of auditory perception by visual emotion. Biol Psychol 2013; 94:471-8. [PMID: 24060548 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.09.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2012] [Revised: 06/26/2013] [Accepted: 09/06/2013] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Emotionally negative stimuli boost perceptual processes. There is little known, however, about the timing of this modulation. The present study aims at elucidating the phasic effects of, emotional processing on auditory processing within subsequent time-windows of visual emotional, processing in humans. We recorded the electroencephalogram (EEG) while participants responded to a, discrimination task of faces with neutral or fearful expressions. A brief complex tone, which subjects, were instructed to ignore, was displayed concomitantly, but with different asynchronies respective to, the image onset. Analyses of the N1 auditory event-related potential (ERP) revealed enhanced brain, responses in presence of fearful faces. Importantly, this effect occurred at picture-tone asynchronies of, 100 and 150ms, but not when these were displayed simultaneously, or at 50ms or 200ms asynchrony. These results confirm the existence of a fast-operating crossmodal effect of visual emotion on auditory, processing, suggesting a phasic variation according to the time-course of emotional processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lenka Selinger
- Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior (IR3C), University of Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain; Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Zhou P, Liu X. Attentional modulation of emotional conflict processing with flanker tasks. PLoS One 2013; 8:e60548. [PMID: 23544155 PMCID: PMC3609783 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060548] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2012] [Accepted: 02/27/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotion processing has been shown to acquire priority by biasing allocation of attentional resources. Aversive images or fearful expressions are processed quickly and automatically. Many existing findings suggested that processing of emotional information was pre-attentive, largely immune from attentional control. Other studies argued that attention gated the processing of emotion. To tackle this controversy, the current study examined whether and to what degrees attention modulated processing of emotion using a stimulus-response-compatibility (SRC) paradigm. We conducted two flanker experiments using color scale faces in neutral expressions or gray scale faces in emotional expressions. We found SRC effects for all three dimensions (color, gender, and emotion) and SRC effects were larger when the conflicts were task relevant than when they were task irrelevant, suggesting that conflict processing of emotion was modulated by attention, similar to those of color and face identity (gender). However, task modulation on color SRC effect was significantly greater than that on gender or emotion SRC effect, indicating that processing of salient information was modulated by attention to a lesser degree than processing of non-emotional stimuli. We proposed that emotion processing can be influenced by attentional control, but at the same time salience of emotional information may bias toward bottom-up processing, rendering less top-down modulation than that on non-emotional stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pingyan Zhou
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Xun Liu
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Cognitive and Emotional Abnormalities in Systemic Lupus Erythematosus: Evidence for Amygdala Dysfunction. Neuropsychol Rev 2012; 22:252-70. [DOI: 10.1007/s11065-012-9213-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2012] [Accepted: 07/27/2012] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
|
16
|
Emotional processing and its impact on unilateral neglect and extinction. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50:1054-71. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2011] [Revised: 02/29/2012] [Accepted: 03/01/2012] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
17
|
Domínguez-Borràs J, Garcia-Garcia M, Escera C. Phase re-setting of gamma neural oscillations during novelty processing in an appetitive context. Biol Psychol 2012; 89:545-52. [PMID: 22212281 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2011] [Revised: 10/27/2011] [Accepted: 12/14/2011] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
Based on the previous study where phase-synchronization (PS) of gamma-band responses (GBRs) proved a reliable cerebral correlate of involuntary attention and its enhancement under threat, we measured gamma-PS elicited by novel sounds from human electroencephalogram (EEG) scalp-recordings when participants responded to visual stimuli displaying either highly motivational or neutral sceneries. We then tested the modulatory effect of the emotional conditions on auditory responses. Novel distractor sounds did not affect behavioural accuracy on subjects' visual task performance in neutral context but markedly decreased hit rate in the appetitive one. Similarly, gamma-PS to novel sounds remained intact in neutral context, whereas it showed an increase, within the 35-Hz sub-range, in the appetitive context. These results suggest that a context of processing positive emotional stimuli results into an enhanced processing of task-irrelevant novel auditory events, and, furthermore, that gamma-PS is tuned under conditions that could promote long-term survival.
Collapse
|
18
|
Sauer S, Walach H, Schmidt S, Hinterberger T, Horan M, Kohls N. Implicit and explicit emotional behavior and mindfulness. Conscious Cogn 2011; 20:1558-69. [PMID: 21885296 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2010] [Revised: 07/24/2011] [Accepted: 08/01/2011] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
The objective of this study was to examine whether the "step back and watch" attitude of mindfulness manifests in less emotional behavior. We hypothesized that the "acceptance" facet of mindfulness, but not the "presence" facet, is negatively associated with the magnitude of emotional behavior in four tests, i.e., (1) rating of words, (2) rating of aversive and neutral pictures, and (3) evaluative conditioning (EC). Additionally, we hypothesized that (4) the acceptance facet is associated with increased reaction time (RT) in an emotional Stroop test, and that the presence facet is associated with decreased RT and lower error rate. The sample consisted of N=247 non-clinical adults and was tested in a cross-sectional study. The results provide partial evidence that the acceptance facet of mindfulness may be associated with less aversive reactions towards aversive stimuli. Future studies should substantiate these findings but also determine their clinical relevance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Sauer
- Generation Research Program, Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, München, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Lv JY, Wang T, Tu S, Zheng F, Qiu J. The effect of different negative emotional context on involuntary attention: An ERP study. Brain Res Bull 2011; 86:106-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.06.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2011] [Revised: 06/22/2011] [Accepted: 06/23/2011] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
20
|
Melcher T, Born C, Gruber O. How negative affect influences neural control processes underlying the resolution of cognitive interference: an event-related fMRI study. Neurosci Res 2011; 70:415-27. [PMID: 21620907 DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2011.05.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2010] [Revised: 03/17/2011] [Accepted: 05/13/2011] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
In this event-related fMRI study, we sought to investigate the influence of negative affect on the processing of two kinds of cognitive interference: Stroop-interference and oddball interference. For our purpose, we adopted an oddball variant of the Stroop task in which Stroop-interference and oddball interference conditions were created by presenting incongruent and rarely occurring word meanings, respectively. Immediately preceding the target stimuli, we presented pictures of the International Affective Picture System which were either emotionally negative and arousing or emotionally neutral, providing two affective conditions under which the cognitive task was administered. Both the behavioral and the neuroimaging data exhibited an interaction effect between emotional and cognitive condition. First, the emotion induction selectively impaired behavioral performance on interference trials while behavioral measures on non-interference trials were roughly identical in both emotional conditions. Second, in the negative emotional condition there was incremental interference-related activation in control-related regions (fronto-parietal cortices). Taken together, findings suggest that negative affect specifically disturbs the neural control processes that in a neutral affective state allow to select task-relevant information and to shield its processing from task-irrelevant distraction. Accordingly, agents in a negative affective state have to exert enhanced control efforts to resolve cognitive interference. Additional connectivity analyses revealed that a negative coupling between lateral PFC on the one hand and amygdala and OFC on the other is related to enhanced interference resolution which can be tentatively interpreted as evidence that emotional regulation is an integrated part of an agent's efforts to preserve cognitive performance in affective situations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tobias Melcher
- Centre for Translational Research in Systems Neuroscience and Clinical Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Georg August University, Goettingen, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Sharma D, Money S. Carryover effects to addiction-associated stimuli in a group of marijuana and cocaine users. J Psychopharmacol 2010; 24:1309-16. [PMID: 19939860 DOI: 10.1177/0269881109350079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Addiction has been characterized as an attentional bias towards drug-related cues. In two experiments we investigate the effects of non-words that have been associatively trained to addiction-related images in a group of marijuana and cocaine users. These associated non-words were presented along with unstudied non-words in a subsequent addiction Stroop task. Results indicate a slowdown in responding to the colour of non-words that were paired with cocaine-related images compared with non-cocaine related images. The slowdown was also characterized as a carryover effect, with the largest effect occurring on trials following the addiction-associated non-word. No effects were found for marijuana images associated with non-words.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dinkar Sharma
- Department of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Kent, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Algom D, Zakay D, Monar O, Chajut E. Wheel chairs and arm chairs: A novel experimental design for the emotional Stroop effect. Cogn Emot 2009. [DOI: 10.1080/02699930802490243] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
23
|
Witthöft M, Rist F, Bailer J. Abnormalities in cognitive-emotional information processing in idiopathic environmental intolerance and somatoform disorders. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2009; 40:70-84. [PMID: 18501333 DOI: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2007] [Revised: 03/31/2008] [Accepted: 04/10/2008] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Idiopathic environmental intolerance (IEI) represents a functional somatic syndrome marked by diverse bodily complaints attributed to various substances in the environment. Evidence for abnormalities in affective information processing similar to somatoform disorders (SFD) has recently been found in people with IEI. In order to further investigate these cognitive-emotional abnormalities, we compared people with IEI (n=49), SFD only (n=43), and non-somatoform controls (n=54) with respect to their performance in the extrinsic affective Simon task (EAST). This task allowed us to dissociate indicators of automatic affective associations and emotional intrusion effects of both bodily complaints and IEI-trigger words. Negative association effects toward IEI-trigger words were strongest for IEI participants. Emotional intrusion effects of symptom words were larger both in IEI and SFD than in controls. The results of enhanced negative automatic evaluations of IEI-trigger words and greater attention allocation to symptom words support cognitive models of IEI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Witthöft
- Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Johannes Gutenberg-University, Staudingerweg 9, D-55099 Mainz, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Mather M, Knight M. The emotional harbinger effect: poor context memory for cues that previously predicted something arousing. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2009; 8:850-60. [PMID: 19102596 DOI: 10.1037/a0014087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
A key function of memory is to use past experience to predict when something important might happen next. Indeed, cues that previously predicted arousing events (emotional harbingers) garner more attention than other cues. However, the current series of five experiments demonstrates that people have poorer memory for the context of emotional harbinger cues than of neutral harbinger cues. Participants first learned that some harbinger cues (neutral tones or faces) predicted emotionally arousing pictures and others predicted neutral pictures. Then they studied associations between the harbinger cues and new contextual details. They were worse at remembering associations with emotional harbingers than with neutral harbingers. Memory was impaired not only for the association between emotional harbingers and nearby digits, but also for contextual details that overlapped with or were intrinsic to the emotional harbingers. However, new cues that were inherently emotionally arousing did not yield the same memory impairments as the emotional harbingers. Thus, emotional harbinger cues seem to suffer more from proactive interference than do neutral harbinger cues, impairing formation of new associations with cues that previously predicted something arousing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mara Mather
- Davis School of Gerontology and Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-0191, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Domínguez-Borràs J, Trautmann SA, Erhard P, Fehr T, Herrmann M, Escera C. Emotional Context Enhances Auditory Novelty Processing in Superior Temporal Gyrus. Cereb Cortex 2008; 19:1521-9. [DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhn188] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
26
|
Sommer M, Döhnel K, Meinhardt J, Hajak G. Decoding of affective facial expressions in the context of emotional situations. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46:2615-21. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.04.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/29/2007] [Revised: 03/18/2008] [Accepted: 04/21/2008] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
27
|
Domínguez-Borràs J, Garcia-Garcia M, Escera C. Emotional context enhances auditory novelty processing: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Eur J Neurosci 2008; 28:1199-206. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06411.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
28
|
|
29
|
Enhanced Early Emotional Intrusion Effects and Proportional Habituation of Threat Response for Symptom and Illness Words in College Students with Elevated Health Anxiety. COGNITIVE THERAPY AND RESEARCH 2007. [DOI: 10.1007/s10608-007-9159-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
30
|
Abstract
It has been proposed that the functional role of the mismatch negativity (MMN) generating process is to issue a call for focal attention toward any auditory change violating the preceding acoustic regularity. This paper reviews the evidence supporting such a functional role and outlines a model of how the attentional system controls the flow of bottom-up auditory information with regard to ongoing-task demands to organize goal-oriented behavior. Specifically, the data obtained in auditory-auditory and auditory-visual distraction paradigms demonstrated that the unexpected occurrence of deviant auditory stimuli or novel sounds captures attention involuntarily, as they distract current task performance. These data indicate that such a process of distraction takes place in three successive stages associated, respectively, to MMN, P3a/novelty-P3, and reorienting negativity (RON), and that the latter two are modulated by the demands of the task at hand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carles Escera
- Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
| | - M.J. Corral
- Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Smith SD, Most SB, Newsome LA, Zald DH. An emotion-induced attentional blink elicited by aversively conditioned stimuli. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2006; 6:523-7. [PMID: 16938093 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.523] [Citation(s) in RCA: 98] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The current study examines whether aversively conditioned stimuli can modulate attention to such a degree that they impair the perception of subsequently presented nonemotional targets. In the initial phase of this study, participants viewed 3 categories of photographs, 1 of which was paired with an aversive noise. Following conditioning, participants searched for a target embedded within a series of 17 rapidly presented images on each trial. Critically, a conditioned or unconditioned item from the initial phase appeared 200 ms or 800 ms before the target. At 200-ms lags but not 800-ms lags, the conditioned images impaired target detection relative to the other distractors. Thus, temporary visual deficits can be induced by otherwise neutral distractors whose aversive associations have only recently been learned.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen D Smith
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Jiang Y, Costello P, Fang F, Huang M, He S. A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006; 103:17048-52. [PMID: 17075055 PMCID: PMC1636576 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0605678103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 183] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Human observers are constantly bombarded with a vast amount of information. Selective attention helps us to quickly process what is important while ignoring the irrelevant. In this study, we demonstrate that information that has not entered observers' consciousness, such as interocularly suppressed (invisible) erotic pictures, can direct the distribution of spatial attention. Furthermore, invisible erotic information can either attract or repel observers' spatial attention depending on their gender and sexual orientation. While unaware of the suppressed pictures, heterosexual males' attention was attracted to invisible female nudes, heterosexual females' attention was attracted to invisible male nudes, gay males behaved similarly to heterosexual females, and gay/bisexual females performed in-between heterosexual males and females.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yi Jiang
- *Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Parkway, Minneapolis, MN 55455
| | - Patricia Costello
- Department of Psychology, Gustavus Adolphus College, 800 West College Avenue, St. Peter, MN 56082; and
| | - Fang Fang
- *Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Parkway, Minneapolis, MN 55455
| | - Miner Huang
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, West Xingang Road, Guangzhou 510275, People's Republic of China
| | - Sheng He
- *Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Parkway, Minneapolis, MN 55455
- To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Blanchette I. Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes: how specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli? Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2006; 59:1484-504. [PMID: 16846972 DOI: 10.1080/02724980543000204] [Citation(s) in RCA: 127] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
In three experiments, the efficiency in detecting fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant visual stimuli are compared. A visual search paradigm is used where participants are presented with matrices of different sizes (4 objects/9 objects) and must determine whether all objects are taken from the same category or whether there is a discrepant one. Results from all experiments were consistent with the threat-superiority effect. Participants were quicker when the target was threatening than when it was not. Other indicators confirmed that the detection of threatening targets involves more efficient processes (reduced slopes, absence of position effects). A crucial aspect of these experiments was the comparison of evolutionary-relevant (snakes, spiders, etc.) and modern (guns, syringes, etc.) threats. The threat-superiority effect was repeatedly found for both types of target. Stronger effects were sometimes observed for modern than for evolutionary-relevant threats. The implications for evolutionary explanations of the effect of fear on visual attention are discussed.
Collapse
|
34
|
Zetteler JI, Stollery BT, Weinstein AM, Lingford-Hughes AR. ATTENTIONAL BIAS FOR ALCOHOL-RELATED INFORMATION IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ALCOHOL-DEPENDENT PARENTS. Alcohol Alcohol 2006; 41:426-30. [PMID: 16624838 DOI: 10.1093/alcalc/agl026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
AIMS to assess the attentional bias for alcohol-related information in adolescents with (n = 15), and without (n = 15), a parental history of alcohol dependence. METHODS participants completed questionnaires assessing depression, weekly alcohol consumption, anxiety, and concerns about alcohol consumption and undertook subliminal and supraliminal computerized Stroop tasks using colour-words, alcohol-related words, and control words. RESULTS adolescents with alcohol-dependent parents showed supraliminal interference for alcohol-related words. The magnitude of this interference was correlated with higher trait and state anxiety, and lower levels of weekly alcohol consumption. No interference was found on the subliminal alcohol Stroop task. CONCLUSIONS while it is likely that this attentional bias for alcohol-related cues reflects the concerns regarding parental drinking, it is also possible that this might underlie the increased risk of future alcohol dependence in the children of alcohol-dependent parents.
Collapse
|
35
|
Keil A. Macroscopic brain dynamics during verbal and pictorial processing of affective stimuli. UNDERSTANDING EMOTIONS 2006; 156:217-32. [PMID: 17015082 DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(06)56011-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
Abstract
Emotions can be viewed as action dispositions, preparing an individual to act efficiently and successfully in situations of behavioral relevance. To initiate optimized behavior, it is essential to accurately process the perceptual elements indicative of emotional relevance. The present chapter discusses effects of affective content on neural and behavioral parameters of perception, across different information channels. Electrocortical data are presented from studies examining affective perception with pictures and words in different task contexts. As a main result, these data suggest that sensory facilitation has an important role in affective processing. Affective pictures appear to facilitate perception as a function of emotional arousal at multiple levels of visual analysis. If the discrimination between affectively arousing vs. nonarousing content relies on fine-grained differences, amplification of the cortical representation may occur as early as 60-90 ms after stimulus onset. Affectively arousing information as conveyed via visual verbal channels was not subject to such very early enhancement. However, electrocortical indices of lexical access and/or activation of semantic networks showed that affectively arousing content may enhance the formation of semantic representations during word encoding. It can be concluded that affective arousal is associated with activation of widespread networks, which act to optimize sensory processing. On the basis of prioritized sensory analysis for affectively relevant stimuli, subsequent steps such as working memory, motor preparation, and action may be adjusted to meet the adaptive requirements of the situation perceived.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andreas Keil
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, PO Box D23, D-78457 Konstanz, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9:585-94. [PMID: 16289871 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1299] [Impact Index Per Article: 68.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2005] [Revised: 09/20/2005] [Accepted: 10/21/2005] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Emotional processes not only serve to record the value of sensory events, but also to elicit adaptive responses and modify perception. Recent research using functional brain imaging in human subjects has begun to reveal neural substrates by which sensory processing and attention can be modulated by the affective significance of stimuli. The amygdala plays a crucial role in providing both direct and indirect top-down signals on sensory pathways, which can influence the representation of emotional events, especially when related to threat. These modulatory effects implement specialized mechanisms of 'emotional attention' that might supplement but also compete with other sources of top-down control on perception. This work should help to elucidate the neural processes and temporal dynamics governing the integration of cognitive and affective influences in attention and behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrik Vuilleumier
- Laboratory for Neurology and Imaging of Cognition, Department of Neurosciences and Clinic of Neurology, University Medical Center, Geneva, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|