1
|
Atkin C, Stacey JE, Allen HA, Henshaw H, Roberts KL, Badham SP. Older adults do not show enhanced benefits from multisensory information on speeded perceptual discrimination tasks. Neurobiol Aging 2024; 142:65-72. [PMID: 39173227 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2024.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2023] [Revised: 07/04/2024] [Accepted: 08/12/2024] [Indexed: 08/24/2024]
Abstract
Some research has shown that older adults benefit more from multisensory information than do young adults. However, more recent evidence has shown that the multisensory age benefit varies considerably across tasks. In the current study, older (65 - 80) and young (18 - 30) adults (N = 191) completed a speeded perceptual discrimination task either online or face-to-face to assess task response speed. We examined whether presenting stimuli in multiple sensory modalities (audio-visual) instead of one (audio-only or visual-only) benefits older adults more than young adults. Across all three experiments, a consistent speeding of response was found in the multisensory condition compared to the unisensory conditions for both young and older adults. Furthermore, race model analysis showed a significant multisensory benefit across a broad temporal interval. Critically, there were no significant differences between young and older adults. Taken together, these findings provide strong evidence in favour of a multisensory benefit that does not differ across age groups, contrasting with prior research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Harriet A Allen
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK
| | - Helen Henshaw
- Hearing Sciences, Mental Health and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK; National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR), Nottingham Biomedical Research Centre, Nottingham, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Xu Y, Aung HL, Hesam-Shariati N, Keay L, Sun X, Phu J, Honson V, Tully PJ, Booth A, Lewis E, Anderson CS, Anstey KJ, Peters R. Contrast Sensitivity, Visual Field, Color Vision, Motion Perception, and Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review. J Am Med Dir Assoc 2024; 25:105098. [PMID: 38908397 DOI: 10.1016/j.jamda.2024.105098] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2023] [Revised: 02/07/2024] [Accepted: 05/12/2024] [Indexed: 06/24/2024]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES To examine relationships between visual function (ie, contrast sensitivity, visual field, color vision, and motion perception) and cognitive impairment, including any definition of "cognitive impairment," mild cognitive impairment, or dementia. DESIGN Systematic review and meta-analyses. SETTING AND PARTICIPANTS Any settings; participants with (cases) or without (controls) cognitive impairment. METHODS We searched 4 databases (to January 2024) and included published studies that compared visual function between cases and controls. Standardized mean differences (SMD) with 95% CIs were calculated where data were available. Data were sufficient for meta-analyses when cases were people with dementia. The Joanna Briggs Institute checklists were used for quality assessment. RESULTS Fifty-one studies/69 reports were included. Cross-sectional evidence shows that people with dementia had worse contrast sensitivity function and color vision than controls: measured by contrast sensitivity (log units) on letter charts, SMD -1.22 (95% CI -1.98, -0.47), or at varied spatial frequencies, -0.92 (-1.28, -0.57); and by pseudoisochromatic plates, -1.04 (-1.59, -0.49); color arrangement, -1.30 (-2.31, -0.29); or matching tests, -0.51 (-0.78, -0.24). They also performed more poorly on tests of motion perception, -1.20 (-1.73, -0.67), and visual field: mean deviation, -0.87 (-1.29, -0.46), and pattern standard deviation, -0.69 (-1.24, -0.15). Results were similar when cases were limited to participants with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer disease. Sources of bias included lack of clarity on study populations or settings and definitions of cognitive impairment. The 2 included longitudinal studies with follow-ups of approximately 10 years were of good quality but reported inconsistent results. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS In the lack of longitudinal data, cross-sectional studies indicate that individuals with cognitive impairment have poorer visual function than those with normal cognition. Additional longitudinal data are needed to understand whether poor visual function precedes cognitive impairment and the most relevant aspects of visual function, dementia pathologies, and domains of cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ying Xu
- Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia; School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; The George Institute for Global Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Faculty of Medicine and Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Ageing Futures Institute, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Centre for Health Systems and Safety Research, Australian Institute of Health Innovation, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.
| | - Htein Linn Aung
- Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia; Faculty of Medicine and Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Negin Hesam-Shariati
- Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia; School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Lisa Keay
- The George Institute for Global Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Ageing Futures Institute, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; School of Optometry and Vision Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Xiaodong Sun
- Department of Ophthalmology, Shanghai General Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China; Shanghai Key Laboratory of Fundus Diseases, Shanghai, China; Shanghai Engineering Center for Visual Science and Photomedicine, Shanghai, China; National Clinical Research Center for Ophthalmic Diseases, Shanghai, China
| | - Jack Phu
- School of Optometry and Vision Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Center for Eye Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Concord Clinical School, Concord Repatriation General Hospital, Sydney, Australia
| | - Vanessa Honson
- School of Optometry and Vision Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Phillip J Tully
- School of Psychology, The University of New England, Armidale, Australia
| | - Andrew Booth
- School of Health and Related Research, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
| | - Ebony Lewis
- Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia; School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Faculty of Medicine and Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Ageing Futures Institute, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Craig S Anderson
- The George Institute for Global Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Faculty of Medicine and Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; The George Institute China, Peking University Health Science Center, Beijing, China; Neurology Department, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital, Sydney, Australia
| | - Kaarin J Anstey
- Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia; School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Ageing Futures Institute, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Ruth Peters
- Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia; School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; The George Institute for Global Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Faculty of Medicine and Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Ageing Futures Institute, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, Australia; School of Public Health, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Abstract
Sensory loss in olfaction, vision, and hearing is a risk factor for dementia, but the reasons for this are unclear. This review presents the neurobiological evidence linking each sensory modality to specific dementias and explores the potential mechanisms underlying this. Olfactory deficits can be linked to direct neuropathologic changes in the olfactory system due to Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease, and may be a marker of disease severity. Visual deficits potentially increase dementia risk in a vulnerable individual by reducing resilience to dementia. Hearing deficits may indicate a susceptibility to Alzheimer disease through a variety of mechanisms. More generally, sensory impairment could be related to factors associated with resilience against dementia. Further research is needed to tease out the specific and synergistic effects of sensory impairment. Studying sensory loss in relation to neurodegenerative biomarkers is necessary to clarify the mechanisms involved. This could produce new monitoring and management strategies for people at risk of dementia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meher Lad
- Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| | - William Sedley
- Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| | - Timothy D Griffiths
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK
- Human Brain Research Laboratory, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Tigchelaar I, Waard D, Jansonius NM, Leinonen MT. Exploring the effect of glaucomatous visual field defects of current drivers on a neuropsychological test battery. Acta Ophthalmol 2022; 100:e463-e469. [PMID: 34328703 DOI: 10.1111/aos.14975] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/09/2021] [Revised: 04/29/2021] [Accepted: 07/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study explores the effect of glaucomatous visual field defects on several neuropsychological tests that are often used in research and in clinical settings. METHODS Nineteen glaucoma patients and nineteen healthy participants, which are current drivers and older than 65 years old were included. All participants completed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), the Trail Making Test (TMT), the Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT), the Snellgrove Maze Task (SMT) and the Digit Span Test (DST). All participants were also tested on contrast sensitivity and near and far visual acuity. For the glaucoma patients, visual field tests were downloaded from hospital servers. RESULTS On the MoCA test, glaucoma patients scored lower than the healthy group, but not significantly. On the MoCA-Blind, the difference was statistically significant. Glaucoma patients also had lower percentile scores on the TMT, with a significant difference in the TMT-A, but this difference largely disappeared in the calculated TMT B-A index, which isolates the cognitive component. The BVRT and SMT showed no significant differences between both groups. In the only non-visual test, the DST, glaucoma patients outperformed the healthy group. Glaucoma severity did not influence results, except for the BVRT on which the moderate/severe group has better scores. CONCLUSION Using visual items might lead to conclusions about cognition when it should be one about vision. Therefore, careful selection of tests is needed when examining cognition in glaucoma patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iris Tigchelaar
- Ocusweep Turku Finland
- Department of Ophthalmology University Medical Center Groningen University of Groningen Groningen the Netherlands
- Turku University Hospital and University of Turku Turku Finland
| | - Dick Waard
- Department of Neuropsychology University of Groningen Groningen the Netherlands
| | - Nomdo M. Jansonius
- Department of Ophthalmology University Medical Center Groningen University of Groningen Groningen the Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
McClannahan KS, Mainardi A, Luor A, Chiu YF, Sommers MS, Peelle JE. Spoken Word Recognition in Listeners with Mild Dementia Symptoms. J Alzheimers Dis 2022; 90:749-759. [PMID: 36189586 PMCID: PMC9885492 DOI: 10.3233/jad-215606] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Difficulty understanding speech is a common complaint of older adults. In quiet, speech perception is often assumed to be relatively automatic. However, higher-level cognitive processes play a key role in successful communication in noise. Limited cognitive resources in adults with dementia may therefore hamper word recognition. OBJECTIVE The goal of this study was to determine the impact of mild dementia on spoken word recognition in quiet and noise. METHODS Participants were 53-86 years with (n = 16) or without (n = 32) dementia symptoms as classified by the Clinical Dementia Rating scale. Participants performed a word identification task with two levels of word difficulty (few and many similar sounding words) in quiet and in noise at two signal-to-noise ratios, +6 and +3 dB. Our hypothesis was that listeners with mild dementia symptoms would have more difficulty with speech perception in noise under conditions that tax cognitive resources. RESULTS Listeners with mild dementia symptoms had poorer task accuracy in both quiet and noise, which held after accounting for differences in age and hearing level. Notably, even in quiet, adults with dementia symptoms correctly identified words only about 80% of the time. However, word difficulty was not a factor in task performance for either group. CONCLUSION These results affirm the difficulty that listeners with mild dementia may have with spoken word recognition, both in quiet and in background noise, consistent with a role of cognitive resources in spoken word identification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Amelia Mainardi
- Department of Otolaryngology, Washington University in St. Louis
| | - Austin Luor
- Department of Otolaryngology, Washington University in St. Louis
| | - Yi-Fang Chiu
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, Saint Louis University
| | - Mitchell S. Sommers
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Díaz-Santos M, Monge ZA, Salazar RD, Gilmore GC, Neargarder S, Cronin-Golomb A. Increasing Contrast Improves Object Perception in Parkinson's Disease with Visual Hallucinations. Mov Disord Clin Pract 2020; 8:51-59. [PMID: 33426159 DOI: 10.1002/mdc3.13104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2020] [Revised: 09/11/2020] [Accepted: 10/08/2020] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Deficits in basic vision are associated with visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. Of particular interest is contrast sensitivity loss in this disorder and its effect on object identification. Objectives Evaluate whether increased contrast improves object perception in persons with Parkinson's disease and visual hallucinations, without dementia. Methods We assessed 26 individuals with mild to moderate idiopathic Parkinson's disease, half of whom reported one or more episodes of hallucinations/unusual perceptual experiences in the past month, with a letter-identification task that determined the contrast level required to achieve 80% accuracy. Contrast sensitivity was further assessed with a chart that presented stimuli at multiple spatial frequencies. The groups were closely matched for demographic and clinical characteristics except for experience of hallucinations. Results Relative to participants without visual hallucinations, those with hallucinations had poorer spatial frequency contrast sensitivity and required significantly greater contrast to correctly identify the letters on the identification task. Specifically, participants with hallucinations required a mean contrast of 52.8%, whereas participants without hallucinations required 35.0%. When given sufficient contrast, the groups with and without hallucinations were equally accurate in letter identification. Conclusions Compared to those without hallucinations, individuals with Parkinson's disease and hallucinations without dementia showed poorer contrast sensitivity. Once contrast was individually enhanced, the groups were equally accurate at object identification. These findings suggest the potential of visual perception tests to predict, and perception-based interventions to reduce, hallucinations in Parkinson's disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mirella Díaz-Santos
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences Boston University Boston Massachusetts USA
| | - Zachary A Monge
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences Boston University Boston Massachusetts USA
| | - Robert D Salazar
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences Boston University Boston Massachusetts USA
| | - Grover C Gilmore
- Jack, Joseph, and Morton Mandel School of Applied Social Sciences Case Western Reserve University Cleveland Ohio USA
| | - Sandy Neargarder
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences Boston University Boston Massachusetts USA.,Department of Psychology Bridgewater State University Bridgewater Massachusetts USA
| | - Alice Cronin-Golomb
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences Boston University Boston Massachusetts USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Billig AR, Feng NC, Behforuzi H, McFeeley BM, Nicastri CM, Daffner KR. Capacity-limited resources are used for managing sensory degradation and cognitive demands: Implications for age-related cognitive decline and dementia. Cortex 2020; 133:277-294. [PMID: 33157347 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2020] [Revised: 07/27/2020] [Accepted: 09/07/2020] [Indexed: 01/11/2023]
Abstract
Older adults with sensory deficits are at higher risk for developing cognitive impairment and dementia. It remains uncertain if the link between sensory and cognitive functioning reflects a common underlying factor or whether sensory deficits directly undermine cognitive processing. This issue was addressed by comparing behavioral and event-related potential responses of 16 older and 16 young adults during a working memory paradigm that parametrically varied visual contrast level (100%, 69%, 22%) and cognitive task load (1-4 face pairs to remember). The groups were well-matched on demographic and neuropsychological variables; however, older adults had worse corrected visual acuity and contrast sensitivity. The study's major finding was an interaction between visual contrast level and task load on performance accuracy (percent of correct responses) and the allocation of resources for decision making/updating (as indexed by the P3b amplitude). The negative impact of degraded visual processing was greater at higher levels of task demand. This result suggests that a shared pool of processing resources is used to mediate cognitive operations and manage the processing of degraded images. The study also demonstrated that older adults reach the limits of their processing capacity at lower levels of task load. The interaction between visual degradation and task demand, accompanied by the age-related reduction in available processing resources highlight the increased vulnerability of older adults. Specifically, an age-associated decline in visual acuity and contrast sensitivity puts older adults at risk for depleting their limited resources in the service of processing degraded visual images. The results of this study underscore the potential importance of optimizing vision in older adults to help mitigate age-associated cognitive decline.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam R Billig
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States.
| | - Nicole C Feng
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States.
| | - Hura Behforuzi
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States.
| | - Brittany M McFeeley
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States.
| | - Casey M Nicastri
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States.
| | - Kirk R Daffner
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Brenowitz WD, Kaup AR, Yaffe K. Incident dementia and faster rates of cognitive decline are associated with worse multisensory function summary scores. Alzheimers Dement 2020; 16:1384-1392. [PMID: 32657033 DOI: 10.1002/alz.12134] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2020] [Revised: 04/20/2020] [Accepted: 05/19/2020] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION We created a summary score for multiple sensory (multisensory) impairment and evaluated its association with dementia. METHODS We studied 1794 adults aged 70 to 79 who were dementia-free at enrollment and followed for up to 10 years in the Health, Aging, and Body Composition Study. The multisensory function score (0 to 12 points) was based on sample quartiles of objectively measured vision, hearing, smell, and touch summed overall. Risk of incident dementia and cognitive decline (measured by two cognitive tests) associated with the score were assessed in regression models adjusting for demographics and health conditions. RESULTS Dementia risk was 2.05 times higher (95% confidence interval [CI] 1.50-2.81) comparing "poor" to "good" multisensory score tertiles and 1.45 times higher comparing the "middle" to "good" tertiles (95% CI 1.09-1.91). Each point worse in the multisensory function score was associated with faster rates of cognitive decline (P < .05). CONCLUSIONS Worsening multisensory function, even at mild levels, was associated with accelerated cognitive aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Willa D Brenowitz
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, USA
| | - Allison R Kaup
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, USA.,The Neurology Center of Southern California, Carlsbad, California, USA.,San Francisco VA Health Care System, San Francisco, California, USA
| | - Kristine Yaffe
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, USA.,San Francisco VA Health Care System, San Francisco, California, USA.,Department of Epidemiology & Biostatistics and Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Völter C, Götze L, Bruene-Cohrs U, Dazert S, Thomas JP. [Hearing and cognition: neurocognitive test batteries in otorhinolaryngology]. HNO 2019; 68:155-163. [PMID: 31628531 DOI: 10.1007/s00106-019-00762-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Hearing and cognition are closely related to each other. Particularly in suboptimal listening situations, cognitive abilities become important to enable speech comprehension. Besides, studies have indicated that hearing impairment is associated with a more rapid mental decline compared to persons with normal hearing. However, hearing loss also has an impact on neurocognitive testing, which is generally based on auditive stimuli. With increasing age, the risk of sensory but also of cognitive impairments increases. So far this comorbidity receives little consideration in otorhinolaryngology. MATERIALS AND METHODS The paper presents an overview and evaluation of widely used German neurocognitive test batteries for older patients, with regard to the different test modalities and their focus. RESULTS A multitude of different neurocognitive screening tests and detailed test batteries are available, particularly in the field of dementia. So far, sensory deficits have not been considered in neurocognitive testing, neither concerning application nor interpretation. Normative data adapted to the hearing impaired are still missing. CONCLUSION With regard to demographic changes and the well-known bias between hearing and cognition, screening of neurocognitive functions should be implemented in basic otorhinolaryngologic diagnostics. More comprehensive test batteries might be useful for research purposes or speech therapy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Völter
- Klinik für Hals‑, Nasen- und Ohrenheilkunde, Kopf- und Halschirurgie, St. Elisabeth-Hospital, Klinikum der Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bleichstr. 15, 44787, Bochum, Deutschland.
| | - L Götze
- Klinik für Hals‑, Nasen- und Ohrenheilkunde, Kopf- und Halschirurgie, St. Elisabeth-Hospital, Klinikum der Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bleichstr. 15, 44787, Bochum, Deutschland
| | - U Bruene-Cohrs
- Klinik für Psychiatrie, Psychotherapie und Präventivmedizin, LWL-Universitätsklinikum Bochum, Alexandrinentrasse 1, 44791, Bochum, Deutschland
| | - S Dazert
- Klinik für Hals‑, Nasen- und Ohrenheilkunde, Kopf- und Halschirurgie, St. Elisabeth-Hospital, Klinikum der Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bleichstr. 15, 44787, Bochum, Deutschland
| | - J P Thomas
- Klinik für Hals‑, Nasen- und Ohrenheilkunde, Kopf- und Halschirurgie, St. Elisabeth-Hospital, Klinikum der Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bleichstr. 15, 44787, Bochum, Deutschland
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Zawaly K, Moyes SA, Wood PC, Cheung G, Rolleston A, Buetow S, Tippett L, Kerse N. Diagnostic accuracy of a global cognitive screen for Māori and non-Māori octogenarians. ALZHEIMER'S & DEMENTIA (NEW YORK, N. Y.) 2019; 5:542-552. [PMID: 31650011 PMCID: PMC6804777 DOI: 10.1016/j.trci.2019.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION We assessed the sensitivity and specificity of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS) in predicting dementia and cognitive impairment in Māori (indigenous people of New Zealand) and non-Māori octogenarians. METHODS A subsample of participants from Life and Living in Advanced Age: a Cohort Study in New Zealand were recruited to determine the 3MS diagnostic accuracy compared with the reference standard. RESULTS Seventy-three participants (44% Māori) completed the 3MS and reference standard assessments. The 3MS demonstrated strong diagnostic accuracy to detect dementia with areas under the curve of 0.87 for Māori and 0.9 for non-Māori. Our cutoffs displayed ethnic variability and are approximately 5 points greater than those commonly applied. Cognitive impairment yielded low accuracy, and discriminatory power was not established. DISCUSSION Cutoffs that are not age or ethnically appropriate may compromise the accuracy of cognitive screens. Consequently, older age and indigeneity increase the risk of mislabeled cognitive status.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristina Zawaly
- Department of General Practice & Primary Health Care, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Simon A. Moyes
- Department of General Practice & Primary Health Care, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Phil C. Wood
- Department of Medicine, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Gary Cheung
- Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, University of Auckland, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Anna Rolleston
- The Centre for Health Ltd., Manawa Ora Centre, Tauranga, Bay of Plenty, New Zealand
| | - Stephen Buetow
- Department of General Practice & Primary Health Care, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Lynette Tippett
- School of Psychology, University of Auckland, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Ngaire Kerse
- Department of General Practice & Primary Health Care, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
|
12
|
Wu CC, Cao B, Dali V, Gagliardi C, Barthelemy OJ, Salazar RD, Pomplun M, Cronin-Golomb A, Yazdanbakhsh A. Eye movement control during visual pursuit in Parkinson's disease. PeerJ 2018; 6:e5442. [PMID: 30155357 PMCID: PMC6109371 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.5442] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2018] [Accepted: 07/24/2018] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Prior studies of oculomotor function in Parkinson's disease (PD) have either focused on saccades without considering smooth pursuit, or tested smooth pursuit while excluding saccades. The present study investigated the control of saccadic eye movements during pursuit tasksand assessed the quality of binocular coordinationas potential sensitive markers of PD. METHODS Observers fixated on a central cross while a target moved toward it. Once the target reached the fixation cross, observers began to pursue the moving target. To further investigate binocular coordination, the moving target was presented on both eyes (binocular condition), or on one eye only (dichoptic condition). RESULTS The PD group made more saccades than age-matched normal control adults (NC) both during fixation and pursuit. The difference between left and right gaze positions increased over time during the pursuit period for PD but not for NC. The findings were not related to age, as NC and young-adult control group (YC) performed similarly on most of the eye movement measures, and were not correlated with classical measures of PD severity (e.g., Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) score). DISCUSSION Our results suggest that PD may be associated with impairment not only in saccade inhibition, but also in binocular coordination during pursuit, and these aspects of dysfunction may be useful in PD diagnosis or tracking of disease course.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Chien Wu
- Center for Computational Neuroscience and Neural Technology, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Bo Cao
- Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine & Dentistry, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
| | - Veena Dali
- Center for Computational Neuroscience and Neural Technology, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Celia Gagliardi
- Center for Computational Neuroscience and Neural Technology, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| | | | - Robert D. Salazar
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Marc Pomplun
- Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts at Boston, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Alice Cronin-Golomb
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Arash Yazdanbakhsh
- Center for Computational Neuroscience and Neural Technology, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Rb is required for retinal angiogenesis and lamination. Cell Death Dis 2018; 9:370. [PMID: 29511172 PMCID: PMC5840357 DOI: 10.1038/s41419-018-0411-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2017] [Revised: 01/29/2018] [Accepted: 02/16/2018] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Retinoblastoma tumor suppressor (Rb) promotes cell cycle exit, survival, differentiation, and tumor suppression in the retina. Here, we show it is also essential for vascularization and lamination. Despite minimal effects on Hif1a target expression, intraretinal vascular plexi did not form in the Rb -/- murine retina. Deleting adenovirus E2 promoter binding factor 3 (E2f3), which rescues starburst amacrine cell differentiation, or E2f2, had no effect, but deleting E2f1, which promotes neuronal cell cycle exit and survival, restored retinal vasculature. We specifically linked cell loss to the defect because removing Bax rescued rod and bipolar neurons and the vasculature, but not cell cycle exit. Despite rescuing Rb -/- neurons, Bax deletion exacerbated a delay in outer retina lamination, and exposed a requirement for Rb in inner retina lamination. The latter resembled Sem5 or FAT atypical cadherin 3 (Fat3) mutants, but expression of Sem5/Fat3 pathway components, or that of Neogenin, which perturbs migration in the Rb -/- cortex, was unchanged. Instead, lamination defects correlated with ectopic division, and were E2f1-dependent, implicating the cell cycle machinery. These in vivo studies expose new developmental roles for Rb, pinpoint aberrant E2f1 and Bax activity in neuronal death and vascular loss, and further implicate E2f1 in defective lamination. Links between Rb, angiogenesis and lamination have implications for the treatment of neovascularization, neurodegeneration and cancer.
Collapse
|
14
|
Ben-David BM, Malkin G, Erel H. Ageism and Neuropsychological Tests. INTERNATIONAL PERSPECTIVES ON AGING 2018. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-73820-8_17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
15
|
Screening tools for the identification of dementia for adults with age-related acquired hearing or vision impairment: a scoping review. Int Psychogeriatr 2017; 29:1771-1784. [PMID: 28691649 DOI: 10.1017/s104161021700120x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Cognitive screening tests frequently rely on items being correctly heard or seen. We aimed to identify, describe, and evaluate the adaptation, validity, and availability of cognitive screening and assessment tools for dementia which have been developed or adapted for adults with acquired hearing and/or vision impairment. METHOD Electronic databases were searched using subject terms "hearing disorders" OR "vision disorders" AND "cognitive assessment," supplemented by exploring reference lists of included papers and via consultation with health professionals to identify additional literature. RESULTS 1,551 papers were identified, of which 13 met inclusion criteria. Four papers related to tests adapted for hearing impairment; 11 papers related to tests adapted for vision impairment. Frequently adapted tests were the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA). Adaptations for hearing impairment involved deleting or creating written versions for hearing-dependent items. Adaptations for vision impairment involved deleting vision-dependent items or spoken/tactile versions of visual tasks. No study reported validity of the test in relation to detection of dementia in people with hearing/vision impairment. Item deletion had a negative impact on the psychometric properties of the test. CONCLUSIONS While attempts have been made to adapt cognitive tests for people with acquired hearing and/or vision impairment, the primary limitation of these adaptations is that their validity in accurately detecting dementia among those with acquired hearing or vision impairment is yet to be established. It is likely that the sensitivity and specificity of the adapted versions are poorer than the original, especially if the adaptation involved item deletion. One solution would involve item substitution in an alternative sensory modality followed by re-validation of the adapted test.
Collapse
|
16
|
Mueller-Schotte S, van der Schouw YT, Bleijenberg N, Schuurmans MJ. Is visual function associated with cognitive activity engagement in middle-aged and elderly individuals? A cross-sectional study. Exp Gerontol 2016; 82:104-11. [DOI: 10.1016/j.exger.2016.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2016] [Revised: 06/14/2016] [Accepted: 06/15/2016] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
|
17
|
Linking cognitive and visual perceptual decline in healthy aging: The information degradation hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 69:166-73. [PMID: 27484869 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2016] [Revised: 07/25/2016] [Accepted: 07/26/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Several hypotheses attempt to explain the relation between cognitive and perceptual decline in aging (e.g., common-cause, sensory deprivation, cognitive load on perception, information degradation). Unfortunately, the majority of past studies examining this association have used correlational analyses, not allowing for these hypotheses to be tested sufficiently. This correlational issue is especially relevant for the information degradation hypothesis, which states that degraded perceptual signal inputs, resulting from either age-related neurobiological processes (e.g., retinal degeneration) or experimental manipulations (e.g., reduced visual contrast), lead to errors in perceptual processing, which in turn may affect non-perceptual, higher-order cognitive processes. Even though the majority of studies examining the relation between age-related cognitive and perceptual decline have been correlational, we reviewed several studies demonstrating that visual manipulations affect both younger and older adults' cognitive performance, supporting the information degradation hypothesis and contradicting implications of other hypotheses (e.g., common-cause, sensory deprivation, cognitive load on perception). The reviewed evidence indicates the necessity to further examine the information degradation hypothesis in order to identify mechanisms underlying age-related cognitive decline.
Collapse
|
18
|
The Implications of Cognitive Aging for Listening and the Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening (FUEL). Ear Hear 2016; 37 Suppl 1:44S-51S. [DOI: 10.1097/aud.0000000000000309] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
19
|
Abstract
RÉSUMÉL’objectif était d’investiguer l’impact de la présence d’un bruit de fond sur la performance au Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). Deux versions du MoCA ont été administrées, utilisant écouteurs, avec des niveaux bas et élevés de bruit de fond à deux groupes de personnes âgées (un groupe présentant une audition cliniquement normale, le second présentant une perte d’audition) ainsi qu’à un groupe de jeunes adultes. Les niveaux d’intensité utilisés pour présenter la parole et le bruit étaient personnalisés en fonction des habiletés des participants présentant une perte de l’ouïe, et ce en vue de créer un niveau de difficulté uniforme à travers les participants dans la condition de bruit plus élevé. Les deux groupes de personnes âgées ont obtenu des scores plus faibles au MoCA en comparaison aux jeunes adultes. Il est également important de souligner que tous les participants ont obtenu des scores plus faibles au MoCA lorsque le test était administré dans un contexte de bruit élevé (M = 22,7/30), en comparaison à un contexte de bruit faible (M = 25,7/30, p < .001). Ces résultats suggèrent que le bruit de fond présent dans un contexte d’évaluation devrait être pris en considération au moment de l’administration de tests cognitifs ainsi que dans l’interprétation des résultats, en particulier lors de l’essai des adultes plus âgés.
Collapse
|
20
|
Peelle JE, Wingfield A. The Neural Consequences of Age-Related Hearing Loss. Trends Neurosci 2016; 39:486-497. [PMID: 27262177 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 152] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2016] [Revised: 05/04/2016] [Accepted: 05/09/2016] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
During hearing, acoustic signals travel up the ascending auditory pathway from the cochlea to auditory cortex; efferent connections provide descending feedback. In human listeners, although auditory and cognitive processing have sometimes been viewed as separate domains, a growing body of work suggests they are intimately coupled. Here, we review the effects of hearing loss on neural systems supporting spoken language comprehension, beginning with age-related physiological decline. We suggest that listeners recruit domain general executive systems to maintain successful communication when the auditory signal is degraded, but that this compensatory processing has behavioral consequences: even relatively mild levels of hearing loss can lead to cascading cognitive effects that impact perception, comprehension, and memory, leading to increased listening effort during speech comprehension.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan E Peelle
- Department of Otolaryngology, Washington University in St Louis, St Louis, MO, USA.
| | - Arthur Wingfield
- Volen National Center for Complex Systems, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Porto FHG, Tusch ES, Fox AM, Alperin BR, Holcomb PJ, Daffner KR. One of the most well-established age-related changes in neural activity disappears after controlling for visual acuity. Neuroimage 2016; 130:115-122. [PMID: 26825439 PMCID: PMC4808353 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2015] [Revised: 12/29/2015] [Accepted: 01/16/2016] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Numerous studies using a variety of imaging techniques have reported age-related differences in neural activity while subjects carry out cognitive tasks. Surprisingly little attention has been paid to the potential impact of age-associated changes in sensory acuity on these findings. Studies in the visual modality frequently report that their subjects had "normal or corrected- to-normal vision." However, in most cases, there is no indication that visual acuity was actually measured, and it is likely that the investigators relied largely on self-reported visual status of subjects, which is often inaccurate. We investigated whether differences in visual acuity influence one of the most commonly observed findings in the event-related potentials literature on cognitive aging, a reduction in posterior P3b amplitude, which is an index of cognitive decision-making/updating. Well-matched young (n=26) and old adults (n=29) participated in a visual oddball task. Measured visual acuity with corrective lenses was worse in old than young adults. Results demonstrated that the robust age-related decline in P3b amplitude to visual targets disappeared after controlling for visual acuity, but was unaffected by accounting for auditory acuity. Path analysis confirmed that the relationship between age and diminished P3b to visual targets was mediated by visual acuity, suggesting that conveyance of suboptimal sensory data due to peripheral, rather than central, deficits may undermine subsequent neural processing. We conclude that until the relationship between age-associated differences in visual acuity and neural activity during experimental tasks is clearly established, investigators should exercise caution attributing results to differences in cognitive processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fábio H G Porto
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Erich S Tusch
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Anne M Fox
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Brittany R Alperin
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Phillip J Holcomb
- Department of Psychology, Tufts University, 490 Boston Avenue, Medford, MA 02155, USA.
| | - Kirk R Daffner
- Laboratory of Healthy Cognitive Aging, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Lavallée MM, Gandini D, Rouleau I, Vallet GT, Joannette M, Kergoat MJ, Busigny T, Rossion B, Joubert S. A Qualitative Impairment in Face Perception in Alzheimer’s Disease: Evidence from a Reduced Face Inversion Effect. J Alzheimers Dis 2016; 51:1225-36. [DOI: 10.3233/jad-151027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Marie Maxime Lavallée
- Département de psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Centre de recherche Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, Canada
| | - Delphine Gandini
- Département de psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Centre de recherche Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, Canada
| | - Isabelle Rouleau
- Département de psychologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Centre de recherche du Centre hospitalier universitaire de Montréal (CHUM), Montréal, Canada
| | - Guillaume T. Vallet
- Département de psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Centre de recherche Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, Canada
| | - Maude Joannette
- Département de psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Centre de recherche Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, Canada
| | - Marie-Jeanne Kergoat
- Département de médecine, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Clinique de cognition, Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
| | - Thomas Busigny
- CHU Purpan, Toulouse, France
- Institut de Recherche en Sciences Psychologique et institut de Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Bruno Rossion
- Institut de Recherche en Sciences Psychologique et institut de Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Sven Joubert
- Département de psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Centre de recherche Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (CRIUGM), Montréal, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Mokrisova I, Laczo J, Andel R, Gazova I, Vyhnalek M, Nedelska Z, Levcik D, Cerman J, Vlcek K, Hort J. Real-space path integration is impaired in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Behav Brain Res 2016; 307:150-8. [PMID: 27038766 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.03.052] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2015] [Revised: 03/07/2016] [Accepted: 03/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Path integration (PI) is an important component of spatial navigation that integrates self-motion cues to allow the subject to return to a starting point. PI depends on the structures affected early in the course of Alzheimer's disease (AD) such as the medial temporal lobe and the parietal cortex. OBJECTIVES To assess whether PI is impaired in patients with mild AD and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and to investigate the role of the hippocampus, entorhinal and inferior parietal cortex in this association. METHODS 27 patients with aMCI, 14 with mild AD and 18 controls completed eight trials of Arena Path Integration Task. The task required subjects with a mask covering their eyes to follow an enclosed triangle pathway through two previously seen places: start-place1-place2-start. Brains were scanned at 1.5T MRI and respective volumes and thicknesses were derived using FreeSurfer algorithm. RESULTS Controlling for age, education, gender and Mini-Mental State Examination score the aMCI and AD subjects were impaired in PI accuracy on the pathway endpoint (p=0.042 and p=0.013) compared to controls. Hippocampal volume and thickness of entorhinal and parietal cortices explained separately 36-45% of the differences in PI accuracy between controls and aMCI and 28-31% of the differences between controls and AD subjects. CONCLUSIONS PI is affected in aMCI and AD, possibly as a function of neurodegeneration in the medial temporal lobe structures and the parietal cortex. PI assessment (as a part of spatial navigation testing) may be useful for identification of patients with incipient AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I Mokrisova
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University in Prague, 2nd Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Motol, Prague, Czech Republic; International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic
| | - J Laczo
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University in Prague, 2nd Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Motol, Prague, Czech Republic; International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic.
| | - R Andel
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic; School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | - I Gazova
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University in Prague, 2nd Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Motol, Prague, Czech Republic; International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic
| | - M Vyhnalek
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University in Prague, 2nd Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Motol, Prague, Czech Republic; International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic
| | - Z Nedelska
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University in Prague, 2nd Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Motol, Prague, Czech Republic; International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic
| | - D Levcik
- Department of Neurophysiology of Memory, Institute of Physiology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Prague, Czech Republic
| | - J Cerman
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University in Prague, 2nd Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Motol, Prague, Czech Republic; International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic
| | - K Vlcek
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic; Department of Neurophysiology of Memory, Institute of Physiology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Prague, Czech Republic.
| | - J Hort
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University in Prague, 2nd Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Motol, Prague, Czech Republic; International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne's University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Stuart S, Lord S, Hill E, Rochester L. Gait in Parkinson's disease: A visuo-cognitive challenge. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 62:76-88. [PMID: 26773722 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2015] [Revised: 12/15/2015] [Accepted: 01/05/2016] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Vision and cognition have both been related to gait impairment in Parkinson's disease (PD) through separate strands of research. The cumulative and interactive effect of both (which we term visuo-cognition) has not been previously investigated and little is known about the influence of cognition on vision with respect to gait. Understanding the role of vision, cognition and visuo-cognition in gait in PD is critical for data interpretation and to infer and test underlying mechanisms. The purpose of this comprehensive narrative review was to examine the interdependent and interactive role of cognition and vision in gait in PD and older adults. Evidence from a broad range of research disciplines was reviewed and summarised. A key finding was that attention appears to play a pivotal role in mediating gait, cognition and vision, and should be considered emphatically in future research in this field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel Stuart
- Institute of Neuroscience/Newcastle University Institute of Ageing, Clinical Ageing Research Unit, Campus for Ageing and Vitality Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Sue Lord
- Institute of Neuroscience/Newcastle University Institute of Ageing, Clinical Ageing Research Unit, Campus for Ageing and Vitality Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Elizabeth Hill
- Institute of Neuroscience/Newcastle University Institute of Ageing, Clinical Ageing Research Unit, Campus for Ageing and Vitality Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Lynn Rochester
- Institute of Neuroscience/Newcastle University Institute of Ageing, Clinical Ageing Research Unit, Campus for Ageing and Vitality Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Díaz-Santos M, Cao B, Mauro SA, Yazdanbakhsh A, Neargarder S, Cronin-Golomb A. Effect of visual cues on the resolution of perceptual ambiguity in Parkinson's disease and normal aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2015; 21:146-55. [PMID: 25765890 PMCID: PMC5433847 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617715000065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Parkinson's disease (PD) and normal aging have been associated with changes in visual perception, including reliance on external cues to guide behavior. This raises the question of the extent to which these groups use visual cues when disambiguating information. Twenty-seven individuals with PD, 23 normal control adults (NC), and 20 younger adults (YA) were presented a Necker cube in which one face was highlighted by thickening the lines defining the face. The hypothesis was that the visual cues would help PD and NC to exert better control over bistable perception. There were three conditions, including passive viewing and two volitional-control conditions (hold one percept in front; and switch: speed up the alternation between the two). In the Hold condition, the cue was either consistent or inconsistent with task instructions. Mean dominance durations (time spent on each percept) under passive viewing were comparable in PD and NC, and shorter in YA. PD and YA increased dominance durations in the Hold cue-consistent condition relative to NC, meaning that appropriate cues helped PD but not NC hold one perceptual interpretation. By contrast, in the Switch condition, NC and YA decreased dominance durations relative to PD, meaning that the use of cues helped NC but not PD in expediting the switch between percepts. Provision of low-level cues has effects on volitional control in PD that are different from in normal aging, and only under task-specific conditions does the use of such cues facilitate the resolution of perceptual ambiguity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mirella Díaz-Santos
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Bo Cao
- Center for Computational Neuroscience and Neural Technology, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Samantha A. Mauro
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Arash Yazdanbakhsh
- Center for Computational Neuroscience and Neural Technology, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Sandy Neargarder
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
- Department of Psychology, Hart Hall, Bridgewater State University, Bridgewater, Massachusetts
| | - Alice Cronin-Golomb
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
At the interface of sensory and motor dysfunctions and Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement 2015; 11:70-98. [PMID: 25022540 PMCID: PMC4287457 DOI: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.514] [Citation(s) in RCA: 379] [Impact Index Per Article: 42.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2013] [Revised: 03/13/2014] [Accepted: 04/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Recent evidence indicates that sensory and motor changes may precede the cognitive symptoms of Alzheimer's disease (AD) by several years and may signify increased risk of developing AD. Traditionally, sensory and motor dysfunctions in aging and AD have been studied separately. To ascertain the evidence supporting the relationship between age-related changes in sensory and motor systems and the development of AD and to facilitate communication between several disciplines, the National Institute on Aging held an exploratory workshop titled "Sensory and Motor Dysfunctions in Aging and AD." The scientific sessions of the workshop focused on age-related and neuropathologic changes in the olfactory, visual, auditory, and motor systems, followed by extensive discussion and hypothesis generation related to the possible links among sensory, cognitive, and motor domains in aging and AD. Based on the data presented and discussed at this workshop, it is clear that sensory and motor regions of the central nervous system are affected by AD pathology and that interventions targeting amelioration of sensory-motor deficits in AD may enhance patient function as AD progresses.
Collapse
|
27
|
Dupuis K, Pichora-Fuller MK, Chasteen AL, Marchuk V, Singh G, Smith SL. Effects of hearing and vision impairments on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. AGING NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2014; 22:413-37. [DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2014.968084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 143] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Kate Dupuis
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - M. Kathleen Pichora-Fuller
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Rotman Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | | | - Veronica Marchuk
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Gurjit Singh
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Phonak AG, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Sherri L. Smith
- Audiologic Rehabilitation Clinic, Auditory and Vestibular Dysfunction Research Enhancement Award Program, James H. Quillen VA Medical Center, Mountain Home, TN, USA
- Department of Audiology & Speech-Language Pathology, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Wong AA, Brown RE. Prevention of vision loss protects against age-related impairment in learning and memory performance in DBA/2J mice. Front Aging Neurosci 2013; 5:52. [PMID: 24065919 PMCID: PMC3776152 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00052] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2013] [Accepted: 08/29/2013] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
The DBA/2J mouse is a model of pigmentary glaucoma in humans as it shows age-related increases in intraocular pressure (IOP), retinal ganglion cell death and visual impairment. Previously, we showed that visual ability declines from 9 to 12 months of age and visual impairment is correlated with poor learning and memory performance in visuo-spatial tasks but not in tasks that do not depend on visual cues. To test the “sensory impairment” hypothesis of aging, which postulates that sensory impaired individuals are disadvantaged in their performance on psychometric tests as a direct result of difficulties in sensory perception, we treated DBA/2J mice with a conventional glaucoma medication used in humans (Timoptic-XE, 0.00, 0.25, or 0.50%) daily from 9 weeks to 12 months of age to determine whether prevention of vision loss prevented the decline in visuo-spatial learning and memory performance. At all ages tested (3, 6, 9, and 12 months of age), mice treated with Timoptic-XE (0.25 and 0.50%) maintained a high level of performance, while 12 month old control mice (0.00%) exhibited impaired performance in visually-dependent, but not non-visual tasks. These results demonstrate that when sensory function is preserved, cognitive performance is normalized. Thus, as in many aging humans, DBA/2J mice show age-related decrements in performance on visually presented cognitive tests, not because of cognitive impairment but as a direct consequence of poor visual ability. Our results demonstrate that age-related impairment in performance in visuo-spatial tasks in DBA/2J mice can be prevented by the preservation of visual ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aimée A Wong
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University Halifax, NS, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|