1
|
Ho C, Jutras-Dubé L, Zhao ML, Mönke G, Kiss IZ, François P, Aulehla A. Nonreciprocal synchronization in embryonic oscillator ensembles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2024; 121:e2401604121. [PMID: 39190346 PMCID: PMC11388350 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2401604121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2024] [Accepted: 07/10/2024] [Indexed: 08/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Synchronization of coupled oscillators is a universal phenomenon encountered across different scales and contexts, e.g., chemical wave patterns, superconductors, and the unison applause we witness in concert halls. The existence of common underlying coupling rules defines universality classes, revealing a fundamental sameness between seemingly distinct systems. Identifying rules of synchronization in any particular setting is hence of paramount relevance. Here, we address the coupling rules within an embryonic oscillator ensemble linked to vertebrate embryo body axis segmentation. In vertebrates, the periodic segmentation of the body axis involves synchronized signaling oscillations in cells within the presomitic mesoderm (PSM), from which somites, the prevertebrae, form. At the molecular level, it is known that intact Notch-signaling and cell-to-cell contact are required for synchronization between PSM cells. However, an understanding of the coupling rules is still lacking. To identify these, we develop an experimental assay that enables direct quantification of synchronization dynamics within mixtures of oscillating cell ensembles, for which the initial input frequency and phase distribution are known. Our results reveal a "winner-takes-it-all" synchronization outcome, i.e., the emerging collective rhythm matches one of the input rhythms. Using a combination of theory and experimental validation, we develop a coupling model, the "Rectified Kuramoto" (ReKu) model, characterized by a phase-dependent, nonreciprocal interaction in the coupling of oscillatory cells. Such nonreciprocal synchronization rules reveal fundamental similarities between embryonic oscillators and a class of collective behaviors seen in neurons and fireflies, where higher-level computations are performed and linked to nonreciprocal synchronization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine Ho
- Developmental Biology Unit, European Molecular Biology Laboratory, Heidelberg 69117, Germany
| | | | - Michael L Zhao
- Developmental Biology Unit, European Molecular Biology Laboratory, Heidelberg 69117, Germany
| | - Gregor Mönke
- Developmental Biology Unit, European Molecular Biology Laboratory, Heidelberg 69117, Germany
| | - István Z Kiss
- Department of Chemistry, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, MO 63103
| | - Paul François
- Department of Physics, McGill University, Montreal, QC H3A 2T8, Canada
| | - Alexander Aulehla
- Developmental Biology Unit, European Molecular Biology Laboratory, Heidelberg 69117, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Koike Y, Ogata T, Nozawa T, Miyake Y. Effect of time delay on performance and timing control in dyadic rhythm coordination using finger tapping. Sci Rep 2024; 14:17382. [PMID: 39075177 PMCID: PMC11286935 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-68326-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2023] [Accepted: 07/22/2024] [Indexed: 07/31/2024] Open
Abstract
In musical ensembles, people synchronise with each other despite the presence of time delays such as those related to sound transmission. However, the ways in which time delays in synchronisation are overcome remain unclear. This study aimed to investigate the basic characteristics and mechanism of synchronisation with time delays using a dyadic synchronisation-continuation finger-tapping task with time delays ranging from 0 to 240 ms. The results reveal that synchronisation performance improved under time delays of 40-160 ms compared with in the other conditions. This tolerance to the time delay could have been because such a delay allowed both participants in each pair to tap before receiving the stimuli from their partner, as seen in synchronisation with a constant-tempo metronome. In addition, the dependency of the timing control on the partner's previous inter-tap interval decreased at a time delay of 80 ms, relating to the fact that the acceleration and deceleration of the tapping tempo reduced under certain time delays, while the synchronisation performance improved. Uncertainty in the timing of the partner's stimulus could induce greater anticipatory responses, making it possible to tolerate longer time delays in dyadic finger-tapping tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuka Koike
- Department of Systems and Control Engineering, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Yokohama, 226-8502, Japan
| | - Taiki Ogata
- Department of Computer Science, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Yokohama, 226-8502, Japan.
| | - Takayuki Nozawa
- Department of Intellectual Information Systems Engineering, University of Toyama, Toyama, 930-8555, Japan
| | - Yoshihiro Miyake
- Department of Computer Science, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Yokohama, 226-8502, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Wolf T, Novák T, Knoblich G. Videos posted on the internet provide evidence for joint rushing in naturalistic social interactions. Sci Rep 2023; 13:10584. [PMID: 37391441 PMCID: PMC10313718 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-37247-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2022] [Accepted: 06/19/2023] [Indexed: 07/02/2023] Open
Abstract
When people engage in rhythmic joint actions, they unintentionally increase their tempo. However, this phenomenon of joint rushing has so far been investigated only under very specific and somewhat artificial conditions. Therefore, it remains unclear whether joint rushing generalizes to other instances of rhythmic joint action. In this study our aim was to investigate whether joint rushing can also be observed in a wider range of naturalistic rhythmic social interactions. To achieve this, we retrieved videos of a wide range of rhythmic interactions from an online video-sharing platform. The data suggest that joint rushing indeed can also be observed in more naturalistic social interactions. Furthermore, we provide evidence that group size matters for how tempo unfolds in social interactions with larger groups showing a stronger tempo increase than smaller groups. Comparing the data from naturalistic interactions with data collected in a lab study further showed that unintended tempo changes in social interactions are reduced in naturalistic interactions compared to interactions in a lab context. It is an open question which factors led to this reduction. One possibility is that humans might have come up with strategies to reduce the effects of joint rushing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Wolf
- Central European University, Vienna, Austria.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Kang K, Orlandi S, Lorenzen N, Chau T, Thaut MH. Does music induce interbrain synchronization between a non-speaking youth with cerebral palsy (CP), a parent, and a neurologic music therapist? A brief report. Dev Neurorehabil 2022; 25:426-432. [PMID: 35341463 DOI: 10.1080/17518423.2022.2051628] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
Shared emotional experiences during musical activities among musicians can be coupled with brainwave synchronization. For non-speaking individuals with CP, verbal communication may be limited in expressing mutual empathy. Therefore, this case study explored interbrain synchronization among a non-speaking CP (female, 18 yrs), her parent, and a music therapist by measuring their brainwaves simultaneously during four music and four storytelling sessions. In only the youth-parent dyad, we observed a significantly higher level of interbrain synchronization during music rather than story-telling condition. However, in both the youth-parent and youth-therapist dyad, regardless of condition type, significant interbrain synchronization emerged in frontal and temporal lobes in the low-frequency bands, which are associated with socio-emotional responses. Although interbrain synchronization may have been induced by multiple factors (e.g., external stimuli, shared empathetic experiences, and internal physiological rhythms), the music activity setting deserves further study as a potential facilitator of neurophysiological synchrony between youth with CP and caregivers/healthcare providers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyurim Kang
- Music and Health Science Research Collaboratory (MaHRC), Faculty of Music, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Center for Music and Medicine, Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Silvia Orlandi
- Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Department of Electrical, Electronic, and Information Engineering "Guglielmo Marconi"- DEI, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | - Nicole Lorenzen
- School of Engineering, University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, Canada
| | - Tom Chau
- Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Institute of Biomedical Engineering, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Michael H Thaut
- Music and Health Science Research Collaboratory (MaHRC), Faculty of Music, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Faculty of Medicine, Institute of Medical Science and Rehabilitation Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Crombé K, Denys M, Maes PJ. The Role of a Mechanical Coupling in (Spontaneous) Interpersonal Synchronization: a Human Version of Huygens’ Clock Experiments. TIMING & TIME PERCEPTION 2022. [DOI: 10.1163/22134468-bja10054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Interpersonal musical interaction typically relies on the mutual exchange of auditory and visual information. Inspired by the finding of Christiaan Huygens that two pendulum clocks spontaneously synchronize when hanging from a common, movable wooden beam, we explored the possible use of mechanical coupling as an alternative coupling modality between people to strengthen (spontaneous and instructed) joint (musical) synchronization. From a coupled oscillator viewpoint, we hypothesized that dyads standing on a common movable platform would cause bidirectional passive body motion (and corresponding proprioceptive, vestibular and somatosensory sensations), leading to enhanced interpersonal coordination and mutual entrainment. To test this hypothesis, we asked dyads to perform a musical synchronization–continuation task, while standing on a movable platform. Their rhythmic movements were compared under different conditions: mechanically coupled/decoupled platforms, and spontaneous/instructed synchronization. Additionally, we investigated the effects of performing an additional collaborative conversation task, and of initial tempo and phase differences in the instructed rhythms. The analysis was based on cross wavelet and synchrosqueezed transforms. The overall conclusion was that a mechanical coupling was effective in support of interpersonal synchronization, specifically when dyads were explicitly instructed to synchronize using the movable platform (instructed synchronization). On the other hand, results showed that mechanical coupling led only minimally to spontaneous interpersonal synchronization. The collaborative task and the initial phase and tempo have no strong effect. Although more research is required, possible applications can be found in the domains of music education, dance and music performance, sports, and well-being.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristel Crombé
- Department of Art, Music and Theatre Sciences, Faculty of Arts and Philosophy, Ghent University, 9000 Gent, Belgium
| | - Marlies Denys
- Department of Art, Music and Theatre Sciences, Faculty of Arts and Philosophy, Ghent University, 9000 Gent, Belgium
| | - Pieter-Jan Maes
- Department of Art, Music and Theatre Sciences, Faculty of Arts and Philosophy, Ghent University, 9000 Gent, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Tomassini A, Laroche J, Emanuele M, Nazzaro G, Petrone N, Fadiga L, D'Ausilio A. Interpersonal synchronization of movement intermittency. iScience 2022; 25:104096. [PMID: 35372806 PMCID: PMC8971945 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104096] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2021] [Revised: 02/02/2022] [Accepted: 03/14/2022] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Most animal species group together and coordinate their behavior in quite sophisticated manners for mating, hunting, or defense purposes. In humans, coordination at a macroscopic level (the pacing of movements) is evident both in daily life (e.g., walking) and skilled (e.g., music and dance) behaviors. By examining the fine structure of movement, we here show that interpersonal coordination is established also at a microscopic – submovement – level. Natural movements appear as marked by recurrent (2–3 Hz) speed breaks, i.e., submovements, that are traditionally considered the result of intermittency in (visuo)motor feedback-based control. In a series of interpersonal coordination tasks, we show that submovements produced by interacting partners are not independent but alternate tightly over time, reflecting online mutual adaptation. These findings unveil a potential core mechanism for behavioral coordination that is based on between-persons synchronization of the intrinsic dynamics of action-perception cycles. Movements show intermittent speed pulses occurring at 2–3 Hz, called submovements Submovements are actively coordinated in counter-phase by interacting partners Submovements coordination depends on spatial alignment but not movement congruency Behavioral coordination occurs both at macro- and microscopic movement scales
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice Tomassini
- Center for Translational Neurophysiology of Speech and Communication (CTNSC), Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy
| | - Julien Laroche
- Center for Translational Neurophysiology of Speech and Communication (CTNSC), Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy
| | - Marco Emanuele
- Center for Translational Neurophysiology of Speech and Communication (CTNSC), Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, University of Ferrara, Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy
| | - Giovanni Nazzaro
- Center for Translational Neurophysiology of Speech and Communication (CTNSC), Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, University of Ferrara, Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy
| | - Nicola Petrone
- Department of Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, University of Ferrara, Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy
| | - Luciano Fadiga
- Center for Translational Neurophysiology of Speech and Communication (CTNSC), Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, University of Ferrara, Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy
| | - Alessandro D'Ausilio
- Center for Translational Neurophysiology of Speech and Communication (CTNSC), Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, University of Ferrara, Via Fossato di Mortara, 17-19, 44121 Ferrara, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Joint rushing alters internal timekeeping in non-musicians and musicians. Sci Rep 2022; 12:1190. [PMID: 35075243 PMCID: PMC8786930 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-05298-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2021] [Accepted: 01/03/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent studies have shown that people engaging in joint rhythmic activity unintentionally increase their tempo. The same tempo increase does not occur when the same rhythmic activity is performed alone. This phenomenon is known as joint rushing. In two experiments, we investigated whether joint rushing is caused by correction mechanisms that facilitate sensorimotor synchronization. Because such correction mechanisms require perceptual input, joint rushing should discontinue when auditory feedback in a joint rhythmic activity is interrupted. This prediction was clearly supported in two experiments, one with musicians and one with non-musicians. Surprisingly, there was no indication that the amount of joint rushing differed between musicians and non-musicians. Furthermore, neither musicians nor non-musicians were able to return to the initially instructed tempo after feedback had been interrupted. This result indicates that joint rushing has a lasting effect on an internal timekeeper. An important question for future research is whether joint rushing is only a dysfunctional side effect of the way sensorimotor synchronization works or whether it has a function in enabling precise temporal coordination between different individuals.
Collapse
|
8
|
Müller V, Ohström KRP, Lindenberger U. Interactive brains, social minds: Neural and physiological mechanisms of interpersonal action coordination. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 128:661-677. [PMID: 34273378 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.07.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2020] [Revised: 05/14/2021] [Accepted: 07/13/2021] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
It is now widely accepted that inter-brain synchronization is an important and inevitable mechanism of interpersonal action coordination and social interaction behavior. This review of the current literature focuses first on the forward model for interpersonal action coordination and functional system theory for biological systems, two broadly similar concepts for adaptive system behavior. Further, we review interacting-brain and/or hyper-brain dynamics studies, to show the interplay between intra- and inter-brain connectivity resulting in hyper-brain network structure and network topology dynamics, and consider the functioning of interacting brains as a superordinate system. The concept of a superordinate system, or superorganism, is then evaluated with respect to neuronal and physiological systems group dynamics, which show further accompanying mechanisms of interpersonal interaction. We note that fundamental problems need to be resolved to better understand the neural mechanisms of interpersonal action coordination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Viktor Müller
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, Berlin, 14195, Germany.
| | - Kira-Rahel P Ohström
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, Berlin, 14195, Germany
| | - Ulman Lindenberger
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, Berlin, 14195, Germany; Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, London, England, and Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Goupil L, Saint-Germier P, Rouvier G, Schwarz D, Canonne C. Musical coordination in a large group without plans nor leaders. Sci Rep 2020; 10:20377. [PMID: 33230245 PMCID: PMC7683723 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-77263-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/08/2020] [Accepted: 11/09/2020] [Indexed: 11/03/2022] Open
Abstract
A widespread belief is that large groups engaged in joint actions that require a high level of flexibility are unable to coordinate without the introduction of additional resources such as shared plans or hierarchical organizations. Here, we put this belief to a test, by empirically investigating coordination within a large group of 16 musicians performing collective free improvisation-a genre in which improvisers aim at creating music that is as complex and unprecedented as possible without relying on shared plans or on an external conductor. We show that musicians freely improvising within a large ensemble can achieve significant levels of coordination, both at the level of their musical actions (i.e., their individual decisions to play or to stop playing) and at the level of their directional intentions (i.e., their intentions to change or to support the music produced by the group). Taken together, these results invite us to reconsider the range and scope of actions achievable by large groups, and to explore alternative organizational models that emphasize decentralized and unscripted forms of collective behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louise Goupil
- STMS UMR 9912 (CNRS/IRCAM/SU), Paris, France
- University of East London, London, UK
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
When Agents Become Partners: A Review of the Role the Implicit Plays in the Interaction with Artificial Social Agents. MULTIMODAL TECHNOLOGIES AND INTERACTION 2020. [DOI: 10.3390/mti4040081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
The way we interact with computers has significantly changed over recent decades. However, interaction with computers still falls behind human to human interaction in terms of seamlessness, effortlessness, and satisfaction. We argue that simultaneously using verbal, nonverbal, explicit, implicit, intentional, and unintentional communication channels addresses these three aspects of the interaction process. To better understand what has been done in the field of Human Computer Interaction (HCI) in terms of incorporating the type channels mentioned above, we reviewed the literature on implicit nonverbal interaction with a specific emphasis on the interaction between humans on the one side, and robot and virtual humans on the other side. These Artificial Social Agents (ASA) are increasingly used as advanced tools for solving not only physical but also social tasks. In the literature review, we identify domains of interaction between humans and artificial social agents that have shown exponential growth over the years. The review highlights the value of incorporating implicit interaction capabilities in Human Agent Interaction (HAI) which we believe will lead to satisfying human and artificial social agent team performance. We conclude the article by presenting a case study of a system that harnesses subtle nonverbal, implicit interaction to increase the state of relaxation in users. This “Virtual Human Breathing Relaxation System” works on the principle of physiological synchronisation between a human and a virtual, computer-generated human. The active entrainment concept behind the relaxation system is generic and can be applied to other human agent interaction domains of implicit physiology-based interaction.
Collapse
|
11
|
Yang SG, Park JI, Kim BJ. Discontinuous phase transition in the Kuramoto model with asymmetric dynamic interaction. Phys Rev E 2020; 102:052207. [PMID: 33327129 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.102.052207] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2020] [Accepted: 10/20/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
We investigate the critical behavior of the modified Kuramoto model with an asymmetric dynamic interaction which has been proposed to explain the difference between the synchronized frequency and the average intrinsic frequency. We find that the discontinuous phase transition arises when oscillators interact only with other oscillators whose phases are ahead. From the comparison with the conventional Kuramoto model in which the interaction possesses phase reflection symmetry, we conclude that the dynamical symmetry breaking and the dynamic change in interaction structure play important roles in changing the transition nature from continuous to discontinuous ones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Seong-Gyu Yang
- Department of Physics, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon 16419, Republic of Korea
| | - Jong Il Park
- Department of Physics, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon 16419, Republic of Korea
| | - Beom Jun Kim
- Department of Physics, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon 16419, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Um E, Kim M, Kim H, Kang JH, Stone HA, Jeong J. Phase synchronization of fluid-fluid interfaces as hydrodynamically coupled oscillators. Nat Commun 2020; 11:5221. [PMID: 33060604 PMCID: PMC7562928 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-18930-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2020] [Accepted: 09/15/2020] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Hydrodynamic interactions play a role in synchronized motions of coupled oscillators in fluids, and understanding the mechanism will facilitate development of applications in fluid mechanics. For example, synchronization phenomenon in two-phase flow will benefit the design of future microfluidic devices, allowing spatiotemporal control of microdroplet generation without additional integration of control elements. In this work, utilizing a characteristic oscillation of adjacent interfaces between two immiscible fluids in a microfluidic platform, we discover that the system can act as a coupled oscillator, notably showing spontaneous in-phase synchronization of droplet breakup. With this observation of in-phase synchronization, the coupled droplet generator exhibits a complete set of modes of coupled oscillators, including out-of-phase synchronization and nonsynchronous modes. We present a theoretical model to elucidate how a negative feedback mechanism, tied to the distance between the interfaces, induces the in-phase synchronization. We also identify the criterion for the transition from in-phase to out-of-phase oscillations. The robust production of droplets by microfluidic T-junctions is a well-established technique. Um et al. demonstrate how the mutual interaction between droplets can be used to achieve additional control including the simultaneous release of droplets caused by synchronization phenomena.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eujin Um
- Department of Physics, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology (UNIST), Ulsan, 44919, Republic of Korea.
| | - Minjun Kim
- Department of Physics, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology (UNIST), Ulsan, 44919, Republic of Korea
| | - Hyoungsoo Kim
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST), Daejeon, 34141, Republic of Korea
| | - Joo H Kang
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology (UNIST), Ulsan, 44919, Republic of Korea
| | - Howard A Stone
- Department of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, 08544, USA
| | - Joonwoo Jeong
- Department of Physics, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology (UNIST), Ulsan, 44919, Republic of Korea.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Goldenberg A, Garcia D, Halperin E, Gross JJ. Collective Emotions. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2020. [DOI: 10.1177/0963721420901574] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
When analyzing situations in which multiple people are experiencing emotions together—whether the emotions are positive or negative and whether the situations are online or offline—we are intuitively drawn to the emotions of each individual in the situation. However, this type of analysis often seems incomplete. In many of the cases in which people experience emotions together, there appear to be emergent macrolevel affective processes that cannot be readily captured at the individual level. In this article, we examine these macrolevel affective phenomena, which are termed collective emotions. We open with a general review of research on collective psychological processes. We then define collective emotions and discuss their key features. Next, we focus our attention on the emergent properties of collective emotions and map them using three dimensions: quality, magnitude, and time course. Finally, we discuss pressing open questions and future directions for research on collective emotions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - David Garcia
- Complexity Science Hub Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Medical University of Vienna
| | - Eran Halperin
- Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Asymmetric dynamic interaction shifts synchronized frequency of coupled oscillators. Sci Rep 2020; 10:2516. [PMID: 32054877 PMCID: PMC7018743 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-58854-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2019] [Accepted: 11/18/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Interacting dynamic agents can often exhibit synchronization. It has been reported that the rhythm all agents agree on in the synchronized state could be different from the average of intrinsic rhythms of individual agents. Hinted by such a real-world behavior of the interaction-driven change of the average phase velocity, we propose a modified version of the Kuramoto model, in which the ith oscillator of the phase ϕi interacts with other oscillator j only when the phase difference \documentclass[12pt]{minimal}
\usepackage{amsmath}
\usepackage{wasysym}
\usepackage{amsfonts}
\usepackage{amssymb}
\usepackage{amsbsy}
\usepackage{mathrsfs}
\usepackage{upgreek}
\setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt}
\begin{document}$${{\phi }}_{{j}}$$\end{document}ϕj − \documentclass[12pt]{minimal}
\usepackage{amsmath}
\usepackage{wasysym}
\usepackage{amsfonts}
\usepackage{amssymb}
\usepackage{amsbsy}
\usepackage{mathrsfs}
\usepackage{upgreek}
\setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt}
\begin{document}$${{\phi }}_{i}$$\end{document}ϕi is in a limited range [−βπ, απ]. From extensive numerical investigations, we conclude that the asymmetric dynamic interaction characterized by β ≠ α leads to the shift of the synchronized frequency with respect to the original distribution of the intrinsic frequency. We also perform and report our computer-based synchronization experiment, which exhibits the expected shift of the synchronized frequency of human participants. In analogy to interacting runners, our result roughly suggests that agents tend to run faster if they are more influenced by runners ahead than behind. We verify the observation by using a simple model of interacting runners.
Collapse
|
15
|
Heggli OA, Cabral J, Konvalinka I, Vuust P, Kringelbach ML. A Kuramoto model of self-other integration across interpersonal synchronization strategies. PLoS Comput Biol 2019; 15:e1007422. [PMID: 31618261 PMCID: PMC6816575 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007422] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2019] [Revised: 10/28/2019] [Accepted: 09/19/2019] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Human social behaviour is complex, and the biological and neural mechanisms underpinning it remain debated. A particularly interesting social phenomenon is our ability and tendency to fall into synchronization with other humans. Our ability to coordinate actions and goals relies on the ability to distinguish between and integrate self and other, which when impaired can lead to devastating consequences. Interpersonal synchronization has been a widely used framework for studying action coordination and self-other integration, showing that even in simple interactions, such as joint finger tapping, complex interpersonal dynamics emerge. Here we propose a computational model of self-other integration via within- and between-person action-perception links, implemented as a simple Kuramoto model with four oscillators. The model abstracts each member of a dyad as a unit consisting of two connected oscillators, representing intrinsic processes of perception and action. By fitting this model to data from two separate experiments we show that interpersonal synchronization strategies rely on the relationship between within- and between-unit coupling. Specifically, mutual adaptation exhibits a higher between-unit coupling than within-unit coupling; leading-following requires that the follower unit has a low within-unit coupling; and leading-leading occurs when two units jointly exhibit a low between-unit coupling. These findings are consistent with the theory of interpersonal synchronization emerging through self-other integration mediated by processes of action-perception coupling. Hence, our results show that chaotic human behaviour occurring on a millisecond scale may be modelled using coupled oscillators.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ole Adrian Heggli
- Center for Music in the Brain, Aarhus University & The Royal Academy of Music Aarhus/Aalborg, Aarhus, Denmark
| | - Joana Cabral
- Life and Health Sciences Research Institute (ICVS), School of Medicine, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Ivana Konvalinka
- SINe Lab, Section for Cognitive Systems, DTU Compute, Technical University of Denmark, Kongens Lyngby, Denmark
| | - Peter Vuust
- Center for Music in the Brain, Aarhus University & The Royal Academy of Music Aarhus/Aalborg, Aarhus, Denmark
| | - Morten L. Kringelbach
- Center for Music in the Brain, Aarhus University & The Royal Academy of Music Aarhus/Aalborg, Aarhus, Denmark
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Combining Phase Advancement and Period Correction Explains Rushing during Joint Rhythmic Activities. Sci Rep 2019; 9:9350. [PMID: 31249346 PMCID: PMC6597726 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-45601-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2018] [Accepted: 06/06/2019] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
When people engage in rhythmic joint actions, from simple clapping games to elaborate joint music making, they tend to increase their tempo unconsciously. Despite the rich literature on rhythmic performance in humans, the mechanisms underlying joint rushing are still unknown. We propose that joint rushing arises from the concurrent activity of two separate mechanisms. The phase advance mechanism was first proposed in research on synchronously flashing fireflies and chorusing insects. When this mechanism is combined with a human-specific period correction mechanism, the shortened periods of individual intervals are translated into a tempo increase. In three experiments, we investigated whether joint rushing can be reliably observed in a joint synchronization-continuation drumming task. Furthermore, we asked whether perceptual similarities produced by the actions of different individuals modulate the joint rushing effect. The results showed that joint rushing is a robust phenomenon occurring in groups of different sizes. Joint rushing was more pronounced when the action effects produced by different individuals were perceptually similar, supporting the assumption that a phase advance mechanism contributed to rushing. Further control conditions ruled out the alternative hypothesis that rushing during rhythmic interactions can be explained by social facilitation or action mirroring effects.
Collapse
|
17
|
van de Rijt A. All-sense-all networks are suboptimal for sensorimotor synchronization. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0202056. [PMID: 30157192 PMCID: PMC6114297 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2018] [Accepted: 07/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
In human groups that seek to synchronize to a common steady beat, every member can typically perceive every other member. We question whether this naturally occurring all-sense-all condition is optimal for temporal coordination. We consider alternative configurations represented by directed graphs, in which individuals can only hear or see a subset of others. We identify a trade-off in the topology of such networks: While denser graphs provide stronger coupling, improving synchrony, density increases sensitivity to early taps, which produces rushing. Results from an experimental study with music conservatory students show that networks that combine short path length with low density match all-sense-all networks in synchrony while yielding a steadier beat. These findings suggest that professional teams in arts, sports, industry, and the military may improve temporal coordination by employing technology that strategically configures who can track whom.
Collapse
|