1
|
Meyer NH, Gauthier B, Stampacchia S, Boscheron J, Babo-Rebelo M, Potheegadoo J, Herbelin B, Lance F, Alvarez V, Franc E, Esposito F, Morais Lacerda M, Blanke O. Embodiment in episodic memory through premotor-hippocampal coupling. Commun Biol 2024; 7:1111. [PMID: 39256570 PMCID: PMC11387647 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-024-06757-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2024] [Accepted: 08/20/2024] [Indexed: 09/12/2024] Open
Abstract
Episodic memory (EM) allows us to remember and relive past events and experiences and has been linked to cortical-hippocampal reinstatement of encoding activity. While EM is fundamental to establish a sense of self across time, this claim and its link to the sense of agency (SoA), based on bodily signals, has not been tested experimentally. Using real-time sensorimotor stimulation, immersive virtual reality, and fMRI we manipulated the SoA and report stronger hippocampal reinstatement for scenes encoded under preserved SoA, reflecting recall performance in a recognition task. We link SoA to EM showing that hippocampal reinstatement is coupled with reinstatement in premotor cortex, a key SoA region. We extend these findings in a severe amnesic patient whose memory lacked the normal dependency on the SoA. Premotor-hippocampal coupling in EM describes how a key aspect of the bodily self at encoding is neurally reinstated during the retrieval of past episodes, enabling a sense of self across time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nathalie Heidi Meyer
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Baptiste Gauthier
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
- Clinical Research Unit, Neuchâtel Hospital Network, 2000, Neuchâtel, Switzerland
| | - Sara Stampacchia
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Juliette Boscheron
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Mariana Babo-Rebelo
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Jevita Potheegadoo
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Bruno Herbelin
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Florian Lance
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Vincent Alvarez
- Hopital du Valais, Avenue Grand Champsec 80, 1950, Sion, Switzerland
| | - Elizabeth Franc
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Fabienne Esposito
- Clinique Romande de Réadaptation, SUVA, Avenue Grand Champsec 90, 1950, Sion, Switzerland
| | - Marilia Morais Lacerda
- Clinique Romande de Réadaptation, SUVA, Avenue Grand Champsec 90, 1950, Sion, Switzerland
| | - Olaf Blanke
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuro-X Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland.
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University Hospital Geneva, Rue Micheli-du-Crest 24, 1205, Geneva, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Dewey JA. Feelings of responsibility and temporal binding: A comparison of two measures of the sense of agency. Conscious Cogn 2024; 117:103606. [PMID: 37995434 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2023.103606] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2023] [Revised: 11/07/2023] [Accepted: 11/13/2023] [Indexed: 11/25/2023]
Abstract
Temporal binding refers to a subjective shortening of the interval between an action and its perceptual consequences. Temporal binding has often been used by researchers to indirectly measure participants' sense of agency (SoA), or the subjective sense of causing something to happen. Other studies have proposed links between temporal binding and feelings of moral responsibility. The present study compared subjective interval estimates to feelings of responsibility in a between-subjects design (Exp 1) and a within-subjects design (Exp 2). Participants either estimated the interval between two events (two tones in the passive condition, or a keypress followed by a tone in active conditions) or rated their feeling of responsibility for the tone(s). Manipulations of participant involvement and choice impacted feelings of responsibility more than temporal estimates. Overall, the two dependent variables followed different patterns, suggesting subjective interval estimates may not be a reliable proxy for feelings of responsibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John A Dewey
- Department of Psychological Science, University of North Georgia, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Reader AT, Coppi S, Trifonova VS, Ehrsson HH. No reduction in motor-evoked potential amplitude during the rubber hand illusion. Brain Behav 2023; 13:e3211. [PMID: 37548563 PMCID: PMC10570491 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.3211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2023] [Revised: 07/03/2023] [Accepted: 07/26/2023] [Indexed: 08/08/2023] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION In the rubber hand illusion (RHI), touches are applied to a fake hand at the same time as touches are applied to a participant's real hand that is hidden in a congruent position. Synchronous (but not asynchronous) tactile stimulation of the two hands may induce the sensation that the fake hand is the participant's own. As such, the illusion is commonly used to examine the sense of body ownership. Some studies indicate that in addition to the subjective experience of limb ownership reported by participants, the RHI can also reduce corticospinal excitability (e.g., as reflected in motor-evoked potential [MEP] amplitude) and alter parietal-motor cortical connectivity in passive participants. These findings have been taken to support a link between motor cortical processing and the subjective experience of body ownership. METHODS In this study, we tried to replicate the reduction in MEP amplitude associated with the RHI and uncover the components of the illusion that might explain these changes. As such, we used single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation to probe the excitability of the corticospinal motor system as participants experienced the RHI. RESULTS Despite participants reporting the presence of the illusion and showing shifts in perceived real hand position towards the fake limb supporting its elicitation, we did not observe any associated reduction in MEP amplitude. CONCLUSION We conclude that a reduction in MEP amplitude is not a reliable outcome of the RHI and argue that if such effects do occur, they are unlikely to be large or functionally relevant.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arran T. Reader
- Department of PsychologyFaculty of Natural SciencesUniversity of StirlingStirlingUK
- Department of NeuroscienceKarolinska InstitutetStockholmSweden
| | - Sara Coppi
- Department of NeuroscienceKarolinska InstitutetStockholmSweden
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Spapé MM, Serrien DJ, Ravaja N. 3-2-1, action! A combined motor control-temporal reproduction task shows intentions, motions, and consequences alter time perception. Heliyon 2023; 9:e19728. [PMID: 37809398 PMCID: PMC10559010 DOI: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e19728] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2023] [Revised: 08/28/2023] [Accepted: 08/31/2023] [Indexed: 10/10/2023] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Michiel M. Spapé
- University of Helsinki, Faculty of Medicine, Department of Psychology & Logopedics, Finland
| | - Deborah J. Serrien
- University of Nottingham, School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, United Kingdom
| | - Niklas Ravaja
- University of Helsinki, Faculty of Medicine, Department of Psychology & Logopedics, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Ma K, Yang L, Hommel B. The impact of interoceptive accuracy and stimulation type on the out-of-body experience. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2023; 76:1940-1952. [PMID: 36113171 DOI: 10.1177/17470218221128285] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/20/2023]
Abstract
People tend to perceive a virtual body standing in front of them as their own if it is either stroked or moving synchronously with their own real body-the out-of-body experience (OBE). We combined synchrony manipulation with two other factors of theoretical interest: the kind of stimulation, visuotactile stimuli or visuomotor correlations, being synchronised and the interoceptive accuracy (IA) of participants, assessed by means of the heartbeat counting task. Results showed that explicit measures of embodiment were systematically affected by synchrony, and this synchrony effect was more pronounced for visuomotor than for visuotactile conditions. The walking drift was affected by IA: In visuotactile conditions, the synchrony effect was pronounced in individuals with low IA, presumably reflecting a stronger impact of the visual information. In visuomotor conditions, however, the synchrony effect was stronger in individuals with high IA, presumably reflecting a stronger impact of re-afferent information generated by the participants' own movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ke Ma
- Key Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, Faculty of Psychological Science, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Liping Yang
- Key Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, Faculty of Psychological Science, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Bernhard Hommel
- University Neuropsychology Center and Cognitive Neurophysiology, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
- Faculty of Psychology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, Jinan, China
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Moullec Y, Cogne M, Saint-Aubert J, Lecuyer A. Assisted walking-in-place: Introducing assisted motion to walking-by-cycling in embodied virtual reality. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS 2023; 29:2796-2805. [PMID: 37015135 DOI: 10.1109/tvcg.2023.3247070] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/04/2023]
Abstract
In this paper, we investigate the use of a motorized bike to support the walk of a self-avatar in virtual reality (VR). While existing walking-in-place (WIP) techniques render compelling walking experiences, they can be judged repetitive and strenuous. Our approach consists in assisting a WIP technique so that the user does not have to actively move in order to reduce effort and fatigue. We chose to assist a technique called walking-by-cycling, which consists in mapping the cycling motion of a bike onto the walking of the user's self-avatar, by using a motorized bike. We expected that our approach could provide participants with a compelling walking experience while reducing the effort required to navigate. We conducted a within-subjects study where we compared "assisted walking-by-cycling" to a traditional active walking-by-cycling implementation, and to a standard condition where the user is static. In the study, we measured embodiment, including ownership and agency, walking sensation, perceived effort and fatigue. Results showed that assisted walking-by-cycling induced more ownership, agency, and walking sensation than the static simulation. Additionally, assisted walking-by-cycling induced levels of ownership and walking sensation similar to that of active walking-by-cycling, but it induced less perceived effort. Taken together, this work promotes the use of assisted walking-by-cycling in situations where users cannot or do not want to exert much effort while walking in embodied VR such as for injured or disabled users, for prolonged uses, medical rehabilitation, or virtual visits.
Collapse
|
7
|
Raoul L, Grosbras MH. Relating different Dimensions of Bodily Experiences: Review and proposition of an integrative model relying on phenomenology, predictive brain and neuroscience of the self. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2023; 148:105141. [PMID: 36965863 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2022] [Revised: 03/18/2023] [Accepted: 03/20/2023] [Indexed: 03/27/2023]
Abstract
How we mentally experience our body has been studied in a variety research domains. Each of these domains focuses in its own ways on different aspects of the body, namely the neurophysiological, perceptual, affective or social components, and proposes different conceptual taxonomies. It is therefore difficult to find one's way through this vast literature and to grasp the relationships between the different dimensions of bodily experiences. In this narrative review, we summarize the existing research directions and present their limits. We propose an integrative framework, grounded in studies on phenomenal consciousness, self-consciousness and bodily self-consciousness, that can provide a common basis for evaluating findings on different dimensions of bodily experiences. We review the putative mechanisms, relying on predictive processes, and neural substrates that support this model. We discuss how this model enables a conceptual assessment of the interrelationships between multiple dimensions of bodily experiences and potentiate interdisciplinary approaches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Raoul
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LNC, Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, Marseille, France.
| | - Marie-Hélène Grosbras
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LNC, Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, Marseille, France.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Pathak A, Jovanov K, Nitsche M, Mazalek A, Welsh TN. Do Changes in the Body-Part Compatibility Effect Index Tool-Embodiment? J Mot Behav 2023; 55:135-151. [PMID: 36642420 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2022.2132201] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
Tool-embodiment is said to occur when the representation of the body extends to incorporate the representation of a tool following goal-directed tool-use. The present study was designed to determine if tool-embodiment-like phenomenon emerges following different interventions. Participants completed body-part compatibility task in which they responded with foot or hand presses to colored targets presented on the foot or hand of a model, or on a rake held by the model. This response time (RT) task was performed before and after one of four interventions. In the Virtual-Tangible and the Virtual-Keyboard interventions, participants used customized controllers or keyboards, respectively, to move a virtual rake and ball around a course. Participants in the Tool-Perception intervention manually pointed to targets presented on static images of the virtual tool-use task. Participants in the Tool-Absent group completed math problems and were not exposed to a tool task. Results revealed that all four interventions lead to a pattern of pre-/post-intervention changes in RT thought to indicate the emergence of tool-embodiment. Overall, the study indicated that tool-embodiment can occur through repeated exposure to the body-part compatibility paradigm in the absence of any active tool-use, and that the paradigm may tap into more than just body schema.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aarohi Pathak
- Centre for Motor Control, Faculty of Kinesiology & Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Kimberley Jovanov
- Centre for Motor Control, Faculty of Kinesiology & Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Michael Nitsche
- School of Literature, Media, and Communication, Georgia Tech, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Ali Mazalek
- Synaesthetic Media Lab, University of Ryerson, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Timothy N Welsh
- Centre for Motor Control, Faculty of Kinesiology & Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Dewe H, Gottwald JM, Bird LA, Brenton H, Gillies M, Cowie D. My Virtual Self: The Role of Movement in Children's Sense of Embodiment. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS 2022; 28:4061-4072. [PMID: 33872150 DOI: 10.1109/tvcg.2021.3073906] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
There are vast potential applications for children's entertainment and education with modern virtual reality (VR) experiences, yet we know very little about how the movement or form of such a virtual body can influence children's feelings of control (agency) or the sensation that they own the virtual body (ownership). In two experiments, we gave a total of 197 children aged 4-14 years a virtual hand which moved synchronously or asynchronously with their own movements and had them interact with a VR environment. We found that movement synchrony influenced feelings of control and ownership at all ages. In Experiment 1 only, participants additionally felt haptic feedback either congruently, delayed or not at all - this did not influence feelings of control or ownership. In Experiment 2 only, participants used either a virtual hand or non-human virtual block. Participants embodied both forms to some degree, provided visuomotor signals were synchronous (as indicated by ownership, agency, and location ratings). Yet, only the hand in the synchronous movement condition was described as feeling like part of the body, rather than like a tool (e.g., a mouse or controller). Collectively, these findings highlight the overall dominance of visuomotor synchrony for children's own-body representation; that children can embody non-human forms to some degree; and that embodiment is also somewhat constrained by prior expectations of body form.
Collapse
|
10
|
Eckstein KN, Rosenbaum D, Zehender N, Pleiss S, Platzbecker S, Martinelli A, Herrmann ML, Wildgruber D. Induced feelings of external influence during instructed imaginations in healthy subjects. Front Psychol 2022; 13:1005479. [PMID: 36389532 PMCID: PMC9664387 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1005479] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2022] [Accepted: 10/14/2022] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
The psychopathological phenomenon of delusions of influence comprises variable disturbances of the self-environment-border leading to the feeling of external influence on thoughts, feelings, impulses or behaviors. Delusions of influence are a hallmark in psychotic illness, but nevertheless, attenuated forms can also appear in healthy individuals. Here we present a newly developed paradigm to induce and assess feelings of external influence during instructed imaginations in healthy individuals. In the current study, we asked 60 healthy individuals to visually imagine different objects. To induce feelings of external influence, we applied one of three different physical setups (low-amplitude transcranial direct current stimulation, eye contact, or skin-to-skin hand touch), and informed the participants whether or not an external influence was attempted during the respective trial. The physical setup (setup vs. no setup, Z = -3.847, p < 0.001, r = 0.497) as well as the information given to the participants (confirmation vs. negation, Z = -5.218, p < 0.001, r = 0.674) alone were able to modulate the feeling of external influence in all three interventions. The impact of information (whether influence was attempted or not attempted) significantly exceeded the impact of the physical setup on the ratings of experienced external influence (Z = -2.394, p = 0.016, r = 0.310). Moreover, the response latency correlated with the estimated feeling of external influence (r S = 0.392, p = 0.002). Additional analyses addressed the influence of the emotional content of imagined objects and examined the intensity and emotional valence of the imaginations. Further supplemental analyses correlated external influence estimation of the participants with other psychopathological measures (trait markers for supernatural beliefs, proneness to hallucinations, and delusions and attributional style). In conclusion, this study endorses a quantitative model of psychopathological characteristics, in this case feelings of external influence that can be induced by external cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathrin N. Eckstein
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany,*Correspondence: Kathrin N. Eckstein,
| | - David Rosenbaum
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Nadine Zehender
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany,Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Sonja Pleiss
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Sharon Platzbecker
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Anne Martinelli
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany,School of Psychology, Fresenius University of Applied Sciences, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Matthias L. Herrmann
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany,Department of Neurology and Neuroscience, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Dirk Wildgruber
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Social, affective, and non-motoric bodily cues to the Sense of Agency: A systematic review of the experience of control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2022; 142:104900. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104900] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2022] [Revised: 09/15/2022] [Accepted: 09/29/2022] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
|
12
|
Suzuishi Y, Hidaka S. Noninformative Vision of Body Movements can Enhance Tactile Discrimination. Iperception 2022; 13:20416695211059203. [PMID: 35047162 PMCID: PMC8761889 DOI: 10.1177/20416695211059203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2021] [Accepted: 10/12/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Vision of the body without task cues enhances tactile discrimination performance. This effect has been investigated only with static visual information, although our body usually moves, and dynamic visual and bodily information provides ownership (SoO) and agency (SoA) sensations to body parts. We investigated whether vision of body movements could enhance tactile discrimination performance. Participants observed white dots without any textural information showing lateral hand movements (dynamic condition) or static hands (static condition). For participants experiencing the dynamic condition first, it induced a lower tactile discrimination threshold, as well as a stronger SoO and SoA, compared to the static condition. For participants observing the static condition first, the magnitudes of the enhancement effect in the dynamic condition were positively correlated between the tactile discrimination and SoO/SoA. The enhancement of the dynamic visual information was not observed when the hand shape was not maintained in the scrambled white dot images. Our results suggest that dynamic visual information without task cues can enhance tactile discrimination performance by feeling SoO and SoA only when it maintains bodily information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yosuke Suzuishi
- Department of Psychology, Rikkyo University
- Japan Society for the Promotion of Science
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Reader AT, Trifonova VS, Ehrsson HH. Little evidence for an effect of the rubber hand illusion on basic movement. Eur J Neurosci 2021; 54:6463-6486. [PMID: 34486767 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.15444] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2021] [Revised: 08/12/2021] [Accepted: 08/28/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Body ownership refers to the distinct sensation that our observed body belongs to us, which is believed to stem from multisensory integration. This is commonly shown through the rubber hand illusion (RHI), which induces a sense of ownership over a false limb. Whilst the RHI may interfere with object-directed action and alter motor cortical activity, it is not yet clear whether a sense of ownership over an artificial hand has functional consequences for movement production per se. As such, we performed two motion-tracking experiments (n = 117) to examine the effects of the RHI on the reaction time, acceleration, and velocity of rapid index finger abduction. We observed little convincing evidence that the induction of the RHI altered these kinematic variables. Moreover, the subjective sensations of rubber hand ownership, referral of touch, and agency did not convincingly correlate with kinematic variables, and nor did proprioceptive drift, suggesting that changes in body representation elicited by the RHI may not influence basic movement. Whilst experiment 1 suggested that individuals reporting a greater sensation of the real hand disappearing performed movements with smaller acceleration and velocity following illusion induction, we did not replicate this effect in a second experiment, suggesting that these effects may be small or not particularly robust. Overall, these results indicate that manipulating the conscious experience of body ownership has little impact on basic motor control, at least in the RHI with healthy participants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arran T Reader
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, UK.,Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | | | - H Henrik Ehrsson
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Burin D, Kawashima R. Repeated Exposure to Illusory Sense of Body Ownership and Agency Over a Moving Virtual Body Improves Executive Functioning and Increases Prefrontal Cortex Activity in the Elderly. Front Hum Neurosci 2021; 15:674326. [PMID: 34135743 PMCID: PMC8200494 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.674326] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2021] [Accepted: 04/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We previously showed that the illusory sense of ownership and agency over a moving body in immersive virtual reality (displayed in a first-person perspective) can trigger subjective and physiological reactions on the real subject’s body and, therefore, an acute improvement of cognitive functions after a single session of high-intensity intermittent exercise performed exclusively by one’s own virtual body, similar to what happens when we actually do physical activity. As well as confirming previous results, here, we aimed at finding in the elderly an increased improvement after a longer virtual training with similar characteristics. Forty-two healthy older subjects (28 females, average age = 71.71 years) completed a parallel-group randomized controlled trial (RCT; UMIN000039843, umin.ac.jp) including an adapted version of the virtual training previously used: while sitting, participants observed the virtual body in a first-person perspective (1PP) or a third-person perspective (3PP) performing 20 min of virtual high-intensity intermittent exercise (vHIE; the avatar switched between fast and slow walking every 2 min). This was repeated twice a week for 6 weeks. During the vHIE, we measured the heart rate and administered questionnaires to evaluate illusory body ownership and agency. Before the beginning of the intervention, immediately after the first session of vHIE, and at the end of the entire intervention, we evaluated the cognitive performance at the Stroop task with online recording of the hemodynamic activity over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. While we confirm previous results regarding the virtual illusion and its physiological effects, we did not find significant cognitive or neural improvement immediately after the first vHIE session. As a novelty, in the 1PP group only, we detected a significant decrease in the response time of the Stroop task in the post-intervention assessment compared to its baseline; coherently, we found an increased activation on left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) after the entire intervention. While the current results strengthen the impact of the virtual full-body illusion and its physiological consequences on the elderly as well, they might have stronger and more established body representations. Perhaps, a longer and increased exposure to those illusions is necessary to initiate the cascade of events that culminates to an improved cognitive performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dalila Burin
- Department of Advanced Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.,Smart Aging International Research Center, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Ryuta Kawashima
- Department of Advanced Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.,Smart Aging International Research Center, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Hidaka S, Sasaki K, Kawagoe T, Asai N, Teramoto W. Bodily ownership and agency sensations in a natural state. Sci Rep 2021; 11:8651. [PMID: 33883582 PMCID: PMC8060257 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-87843-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2020] [Accepted: 04/06/2021] [Indexed: 02/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Our bodily sensation is a fundamental cue for our self-consciousness. Whereas experimental studies have uncovered characteristics of bodily sensation, these studies investigated bodily sensations through manipulating bodily sensations to be apart from one's own body and to be assigned to external, body-like objects. In order to capture our bodily sensation as it is, this questionnaire survey study explored the characteristics of bodily sensation using a large population-based sample (N = 580, comprising 20s to 70s age groups) without experimental manipulations. We focused on the sensations of ownership, the feeling of having a body part as one's own, and agency, the feeling of controlling a body part by oneself, in multiple body parts (the eyes, ears, hands, legs, nose, and mouth). The ownership and agency sensations were positively related to each other in each body part. Interestingly, the agency sensation of the hands and legs had a positive relationship with the ownership sensations of the other body parts. We also found the 60s age group had a unique internal configuration, assessed by the similarity of rating scores, of the body parts for each bodily sensation. Our findings revealed the existence of unique characteristics for bodily sensations in a natural state.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Souta Hidaka
- Department of Psychology, Rikkyo University, 1-2-26, Kitano, Niiza-shi, Saitama, 352-8558, Japan.
| | - Kyoshiro Sasaki
- Faculty of Informatics, Kansai University, 2-1-1, Ryozenji-cho, Takatsuki, Osaka, 569-1095, Japan
| | - Toshikazu Kawagoe
- Department of Psychology, Rikkyo University, 1-2-26, Kitano, Niiza-shi, Saitama, 352-8558, Japan
| | - Nobuko Asai
- Department of Social Relations, Kyoto-Bunkyo University, 80 Senzoku, Makishima-cho, Uji, Kyoto, 611-0041, Japan
| | - Wataru Teramoto
- Department of Psychology, Kumamoto University, 2-40-1 Kurokami, Chuo-ku, Kumamoto, 860-8555, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Herbst J, Polanski-Schräder LM, Vogt J, Schürmann T, Beckerle P. Validation and revision of the questionnaire to explore human factors and their technical potential for lower limb prosthetics. Prosthet Orthot Int 2021; 45:6-11. [PMID: 33834739 DOI: 10.1177/0309364620931991] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND A variety of instruments exist to measure human factors for lower limb amputation and prosthesis research. Yet, there is no valid or reliable tool available that focuses on technical potentials. OBJECTIVE This study aimed to validate and revise the Questionnaire to Explore Human Factors and their Technical Potential. STUDY DESIGN Cross-sectional study METHODS:: A total of 150 persons with lower limb amputation from Germany participated in the study. Statistical properties, including Cronbach's alpha, item difficulty, item-total correlation, and distribution of missing values were calculated. Thresholds for acceptable psychometric properties were defined, unsuitable items were removed, and problematic items were reviewed regarding formulation. RESULTS The Cronbach's alpha for subscales within the Questionnaire to Explore Human Factors and their Technical Potential were between 0.72 and 0.89. A total of 56 items showed acceptability, and 14 items had problematic item property values. Four of those items were reformulated, five were excluded, six were left in the scale, and an additional one was added to the scale. CONCLUSION Evaluation of the Questionnaire to Explore Human Factors and their Technical Potential indicates it exhibits good internal consistency and acceptable psychometric properties. The scale was revised and is recommended to explore aspects of technical prosthesis development. CLINICAL RELEVANCE Our results show that the revised Questionnaire to Explore Human Factors and their Technical Potential may serve as a reliable and valid means, when designing prostheses, both during development and clinical evaluations and fittings, to assess the technical potential of lower limb prostheses directly according to the needs of users with lower-limb amputations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan Herbst
- Work and Engineering Psychology Research Group, Department of Human Sciences, Technische Universität Darmstadt, Darmstadt, Germany
| | - Lucas Mt Polanski-Schräder
- Work and Engineering Psychology Research Group, Department of Human Sciences, Technische Universität Darmstadt, Darmstadt, Germany
| | - Joachim Vogt
- Work and Engineering Psychology Research Group, Department of Human Sciences, Technische Universität Darmstadt, Darmstadt, Germany
| | - Tim Schürmann
- Work and Engineering Psychology Research Group, Department of Human Sciences, Technische Universität Darmstadt, Darmstadt, Germany
| | - Philipp Beckerle
- Elastic Lightweight Robotics, Department of Electrical Engineering and Information Technology, Robotics Research Institute, Technische Universität Dortmund, Dortmund, Germany
- Institute for Mechatronic Systems, Department of Mechanical Engineering, Technische Universität Darmstadt, Darmstadt, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Cibrian FL, Madrigal M, Avelais M, Tentori M. Supporting coordination of children with ASD using neurological music therapy: A pilot randomized control trial comparing an elastic touch-display with tambourines. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2020; 106:103741. [PMID: 32950853 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103741] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2020] [Revised: 06/26/2020] [Accepted: 07/13/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
AIM To evaluate the efficacy of Neurologic Music Therapy (NMT) using a traditional and a technological intervention (elastic touch-display) in improving the coordination of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), as a primary outcome, and the timing and strength control of their movements as secondary outcomes. METHODS Twenty-two children with ASD completed 8 NMT sessions, as a part of a 2-month intervention. Participants were randomly assigned to either use an elastic touch-display (experimental group) or tambourines (control group). We conducted pre- and post- assessment evaluations, including the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ) and motor assessments related to the control of strength and timing of movements. OUTCOMES AND RESULTS All participants improved their coordination, according to the DCDQ scores, and exhibited better control of their movements according to the strength and timing assessments after the intervention. Participants who used the elastic touch-display scored higher on the DCDQ. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS NMT is an efficacious treatment to improve the coordination skills of children with ASD. Elastic touch-displays provide more benefits than the use of tambourines.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Franceli L Cibrian
- Fowler School of Engineering, Chapman University, Orange California, USA.
| | | | | | - Monica Tentori
- Computer Science Department, Center for Scientific Research and Higher Education of Ensenada (CICESE), Mexico.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Jung M, Kim J, Kim K. Measuring recognition of body changes over time: A human-computer interaction tool using dynamic morphing and body ownership illusion. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0239322. [PMID: 32946504 PMCID: PMC7500668 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0239322] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2020] [Accepted: 09/04/2020] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Measuring body image is crucial at both personal and social levels. Previous studies have attempted to quantitatively measure body image but methods for measuring body change recognition over time have not yet been established. The present study proposes a novel human-computer interaction technique using dynamic morphing and body ownership illusion, and we conducted a user study to investigate how body ownership illusion and gender would affect to body change recognition. The results showed that a participant's body change recognition was weak when the body ownership illusion was strong. In addition, female participants were less sensitive than male participants. With our proposed technique, we demonstrated that we were able to quantitatively measure body change recognition and our empirical data indicated that body change recognition varied depending on body ownership illusion and gender, suggesting that our methodology could not only be used in future body image studies but also in eating disorder treatments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Myeongul Jung
- Department of Computer Science, Hanyang University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Jejoong Kim
- Department of Psychology, Duksung Women’s University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Kwanguk Kim
- Department of Computer Science, Hanyang University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Nataraj R, Hollinger D, Liu M, Shah A. Disproportionate positive feedback facilitates sense of agency and performance for a reaching movement task with a virtual hand. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0233175. [PMID: 32433665 PMCID: PMC7239468 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0233175] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2019] [Accepted: 04/29/2020] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
This study investigated the generalized effects of positive feedback (PF) versus negative feedback (NF) during training on performance and sense of agency for a reach-to-touch task with a virtual hand. Virtual reality (VR) is increasingly employed for rehabilitation after neuromuscular traumas such as stroke and spinal cord injury. However, VR methods still need to be optimized for greater effectiveness and engagement to increase rates of clinical retention. In this study, we observed that training with disproportionate PF subsequently produced greater reaching performance (minimizing path length) and greater agency (perception of control) than with disproportionate NF. During PF training, there was also progressive increase in agency, but conversely a decrease in performance. Thus, the increase in performance after training may not be due to positively bolstered learning, but rather priming higher confidence reflected in greater agency. Agency was positively measured as compression in perceived time-intervals between the action of touch to a sound consequence, as standard with intentional binding paradigms. Positive feedback desirably increased agency (~180 msec) and reduced path length (1.8 cm) compared to negative feedback, which itself showed insignificant, or neutral, effects. Future investigations into optimizing virtual reality paradigms for neuromotor rehabilitation should consider agency as a driving factor for performance. These studies may serve to optimize how feedback is better presented with performance results for complex motor learning. Investigators should also ponder how personal characteristics, both cognitive and physical, may further affect sensitivity to feedback and the rate of neuromotor rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raviraj Nataraj
- Movement Control Rehabilitation (MOCORE) Laboratory, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
| | - David Hollinger
- Movement Control Rehabilitation (MOCORE) Laboratory, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
| | - Mingxiao Liu
- Movement Control Rehabilitation (MOCORE) Laboratory, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
| | - Aniket Shah
- Movement Control Rehabilitation (MOCORE) Laboratory, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Roel Lesur M, Weijs ML, Simon C, Kannape OA, Lenggenhager B. Psychometrics of Disembodiment and Its Differential Modulation by Visuomotor and Visuotactile Mismatches. iScience 2020; 23:100901. [PMID: 32109678 PMCID: PMC7044746 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.100901] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2019] [Revised: 11/12/2019] [Accepted: 01/20/2020] [Indexed: 10/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Altered states of embodiment are fundamental to the scientific understanding of bodily self consciousness. The feeling of disembodiment during everyday activities is common to clinical conditions; however, the direct study of disembodiment in experimental setups is rare compared to the extensive investigation of illusory embodiment of an external object. Using mixed reality to modulate embodiment through temporally mismatching sensory signals from the own body, we assessed how such mismatches affect phenomenal and physiological aspects of embodiment and measured perceptual thresholds for these across multimodal signals. The results of a principal component analysis suggest that multimodal mismatches generally induce disembodiment by increasing the sense of disownership and deafference and decreasing embodiment; however, this was not generally reflected in physiological changes. Although visual delay decreased embodiment both during active movement and passive touch, the effect was stronger for the former. We discuss the relevance of these findings for understanding bodily self plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marte Roel Lesur
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.
| | - Marieke Lieve Weijs
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Colin Simon
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Oliver Alan Kannape
- School of Psychology, University of Central Lancashire, Preston PR1 2HE, UK; Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Center for Neuroprosthetics and Brain Mind Institute, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Bigna Lenggenhager
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Reader AT, Ehrsson HH. Weakening the subjective sensation of own hand ownership does not interfere with rapid finger movements. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0223580. [PMID: 31585001 PMCID: PMC6777829 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223580] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2019] [Accepted: 09/24/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
When we perform a movement we generally have a clear distinction between which parts of the world constitute our body and which parts do not. However, how the sense of ownership over our body supports movement is not yet fully understood. We aimed to see whether a sense of ownership over the hand supports the performance of rapid hand movements. In three experiments (n = 48, n = 30, n = 24), we presented participants with congruent and incongruent visuotactile and visuoproprioceptive information regarding their own hand. In keeping with previous experiments, multisensory disintegration resulted in a reduction in the subjective sensation of ownership over the hand, as reflected in questionnaire responses. Following sensory stimulation, participants were required to rapidly abduct their index finger whilst the movement was tracked. We examined the hypothesis that, should a sense of ownership over the limb be necessary for generating rapid movements with that limb, reaction time would increase when hand ownership was reduced, whilst the acceleration and velocity of the movement would decrease. We observed that reductions in own hand ownership did not interfere with rapid index finger abduction, suggesting that the motor system may not be reliant on a subjective sense of ownership over the body in order to generate movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arran T. Reader
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
- * E-mail:
| | - H. Henrik Ehrsson
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Riemer M, Trojan J, Beauchamp M, Fuchs X. The rubber hand universe: On the impact of methodological differences in the rubber hand illusion. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 104:268-280. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2019] [Revised: 07/04/2019] [Accepted: 07/15/2019] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
|
23
|
Hülsmann F, Frank C, Senna I, Ernst MO, Schack T, Botsch M. Superimposed Skilled Performance in a Virtual Mirror Improves Motor Performance and Cognitive Representation of a Full Body Motor Action. Front Robot AI 2019; 6:43. [PMID: 33501059 PMCID: PMC7805859 DOI: 10.3389/frobt.2019.00043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2018] [Accepted: 05/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Feedback is essential for skill acquisition as it helps identifying and correcting performance errors. Nowadays, Virtual Reality can be used as a tool to guide motor learning, and to provide innovative types of augmented feedback that exceed real world opportunities. Concurrent feedback has shown to be especially beneficial for novices. Moreover, watching skilled performances helps novices to acquire a motor skill, and this effect depends on the perspective taken by the observer. To date, however, the impact of watching one's own performance together with full body superimposition of a skilled performance, either from the front or from the side, remains to be explored. Here we used an immersive, state-of-the-art, low-latency cave automatic virtual environment (CAVE), and we asked novices to perform squat movements in front of a virtual mirror. Participants were assigned to one of three concurrent visual feedback groups: participants either watched their own avatar performing full body movements or were presented with the movement of a skilled individual superimposed on their own performance during movement execution, either from a frontal or from a side view. Motor performance and cognitive representation were measured in order to track changes in movement quality as well as motor memory across time. Consistent with our hypotheses, results showed an advantage of the groups that observed their own avatar performing the squat together with the superimposed skilled performance for some of the investigated parameters, depending on perspective. Specifically, for the deepest point of the squat, participants watching the squat from the front adapted their height, while those watching from the side adapted their backward movement. In a control experiment, we ruled out the possibility that the observed improvements were due to the mere fact of performing the squat movements—irrespective of the type of visual feedback. The present findings indicate that it can be beneficial for novices to watch themselves together with a skilled performance during execution, and that improvement depends on the perspective chosen.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Felix Hülsmann
- Computer Graphics and Geometry Processing, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany.,Social Cognitive Systems, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Cornelia Frank
- Neurocognition and Action, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Irene Senna
- Applied Cognitive Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
| | - Marc O Ernst
- Applied Cognitive Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
| | - Thomas Schack
- Neurocognition and Action, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Mario Botsch
- Computer Graphics and Geometry Processing, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Bansal S, Murthy KG, Fitzgerald J, Schwartz BL, Joiner WM. Reduced transfer of visuomotor adaptation is associated with aberrant sense of agency in schizophrenia. Neuroscience 2019; 413:108-122. [PMID: 31228588 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.06.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2018] [Revised: 06/10/2019] [Accepted: 06/10/2019] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
One deficit associated with schizophrenia (SZ) is the reduced ability to distinguish self-caused sensations from those due to external sources. This reduced sense of agency (SoA, subjective awareness of control over one's actions) is hypothesized to result from a diminished utilization of internal monitoring signals of self-movement (i.e., efference copy) which subsequently impairs forming and utilizing sensory prediction errors (differences between the predicted and actual sensory consequences resulting from movement). Another important function of these internal monitoring signals is the facilitation of higher-order mechanisms related to motor learning and control. Current predictive-coding models of adaptation postulate that the sensory consequences of motor commands are predicted based on internal action-related information, and that ownership and control of motor behavior is modified in various contexts based on predictive processing. Here, we investigated the connections between SoA and motor adaptation. Schizophrenia patients (SZP, N=30) and non-psychiatric control subjects (HC, N=31) adapted to altered movement visual feedback and applied the motor recalibration to untested contexts (i.e., the spatial generalization). Although adaptation was similar for SZP and controls, the extent of generalization was significantly less for SZP; movement trajectories made by patients to the furthest untrained target (135o) before and after adaptation were largely indistinguishable. Interestingly, deficits in generalization were correlated with positive symptoms of psychosis in SZP (e.g., hallucinations). Generalization was also associated with measures of SoA across both SZP and HC, emphasizing the role action awareness plays in motor behavior, and suggesting that misattributing agency, even in HC, manifests in abnormal motor performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonia Bansal
- Department of Neuroscience, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia; Mental Health Service Line, Washington DC Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Washington, DC; Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21224
| | - Karthik G Murthy
- Department of Bioengineering, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia
| | - Justin Fitzgerald
- Department of Bioengineering, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia
| | - Barbara L Schwartz
- Mental Health Service Line, Washington DC Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Washington, DC; Department of Psychiatry, Georgetown University School of Medicine, Washington, DC
| | - Wilsaan M Joiner
- Department of Neuroscience, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia; Department of Bioengineering, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia; Krasnow Institute for Advanced Study, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia; Department of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Zopf R, Schweinberger SR, Rich AN. Limits on visual awareness of object targets in the context of other object category masks: Investigating bottlenecks in the continuous flash suppression paradigm with hand and tool stimuli. J Vis 2019; 19:17. [PMID: 31100133 DOI: 10.1167/19.5.17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The continuous flash suppression (CFS) task can be used to investigate what limits our capacity to become aware of visual stimuli. In this task, a stream of rapidly changing mask images to one eye initially suppresses awareness for a static target image presented to the other eye. Several factors may determine the breakthrough time from mask suppression, one of which is the overlap in representation of the target/mask categories in higher visual cortex. This hypothesis is based on certain object categories (e.g., faces) being more effective in blocking awareness of other categories (e.g., buildings) than other combinations (e.g., cars/chairs). Previous work found mask effectiveness to be correlated with category-pair high-level representational similarity. As the cortical representations of hands and tools overlap, these categories are ideal to test this further as well as to examine alternative explanations. For our CFS experiments, we predicted longer breakthrough times for hands/tools compared to other pairs due to the reported cortical overlap. In contrast, across three experiments, participants were generally faster at detecting targets masked by hands or tools compared to other mask categories. Exploring low-level explanations, we found that the category average for edges (e.g., hands have less detail compared to cars) was the best predictor for the data. This low-level bottleneck could not completely account for the specific category patterns and the hand/tool effects, suggesting there are several levels at which object category-specific limits occur. Given these findings, it is important that low-level bottlenecks for visual awareness are considered when testing higher-level hypotheses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Regine Zopf
- Perception in Action Research Centre & Department of Cognitive Science, Faculty of Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australia
| | - Stefan R Schweinberger
- ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australia.,Department of Psychology, Friedrich Schiller University, Jena, Germany
| | - Anina N Rich
- Perception in Action Research Centre & Department of Cognitive Science, Faculty of Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australia.,Centre for Elite Performance, Training & Expertise, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Reddish P, Tong EMW, Jong J, Whitehouse H. Interpersonal synchrony affects performers’ sense of agency. SELF AND IDENTITY 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/15298868.2019.1604427] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Reddish
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore
| | - Eddie M. W. Tong
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore
| | - Jonathan Jong
- Centre for Research in Psychology, Behavior, and Achievement, Coventry University, Coventry, UK
| | - Harvey Whitehouse
- Institute of Cognitive and Evolutionary Anthropology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Active control as evidence in favor of sense of ownership in the moving Virtual Hand Illusion. Conscious Cogn 2019; 71:123-135. [PMID: 31005748 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2018] [Revised: 04/10/2019] [Accepted: 04/10/2019] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The sense of ownership, the feeling that our body belongs to ourselves, relies on multiple sources of sensory information. Among these sources, the contribution of visuomotor information is still debated. We tested the effect of active control in the sense of ownership in the moving Virtual Hand Illusion. Participants reported sense of ownership and sense of agency over a virtual arm in which we manipulated the morphological congruence of the hand and the visuomotor information. We found that congruent active control enhanced and maintained the reported sense of ownership over a hand that appeared detached from the body, but not in a morphological congruent limb. Also, incongruent active control, achieved by adding noise to the trajectory of the movement, decreased both reported sense of agency and ownership. Overall, our results are consistent with a framework in which active control acts as evidence for eliciting a sense of ownership.
Collapse
|
28
|
Hamasaki S, An Q, Wen W, Tamura Y, Yamakawa H, Unenaka S, Shibuya S, Ohki Y, Yamashita A, Asama H. Changes in Body Representation of the Human Upper Limb as a Function of Movement and Visual Hand Position. JOURNAL OF ADVANCED COMPUTATIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND INTELLIGENT INFORMATICS 2019. [DOI: 10.20965/jaciii.2019.p0196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Several disease presentations are linked to a mismatch between the real body and the body’s internal representation of itself. In order to develop effective rehabilitation therapies, it is necessary to understand the mechanisms underlying changes in body representation. In this study, we focused on changes in body representation of the upper limb as a large part of the body and investigated the conditions under which such changes occur. Participants were presented four conditions which differentially affected their sense of ownership and agency, including a movement condition related to their sense of agency, and a visual hand information condition related to the sense of ownership. In the experiment, participants were asked to move their upper limb forward and backward on a manipulandum. Results of the study showed that visual hand position affected changes in body representation relevant to both conscious and unconscious body parts. In addition, changes in the representation of the unconscious body part occurred with, and were dependent on, active movement.
Collapse
|
29
|
Grechuta K, Ulysse L, Rubio Ballester B, Verschure PFMJ. Self Beyond the Body: Action-Driven and Task-Relevant Purely Distal Cues Modulate Performance and Body Ownership. Front Hum Neurosci 2019; 13:91. [PMID: 30949038 PMCID: PMC6435571 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00091] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2018] [Accepted: 02/26/2019] [Indexed: 01/24/2023] Open
Abstract
Our understanding of body ownership largely relies on the so-called Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI). In this paradigm, synchronous stroking of the real and the rubber hands leads to an illusion of ownership of the rubber hand provided that it is physically, anatomically, and spatially plausible. Self-attribution of an artificial hand also occurs during visuomotor synchrony. In particular, participants experience ownership over a virtual or a rubber hand when the visual feedback of self-initiated movements follows the trajectory of the instantiated motor commands, such as in the Virtual Hand Illusion (VHI) or the moving Rubber Hand Illusion (mRHI). Evidence yields that both when the cues are triggered externally (RHI) and when they result from voluntary actions (VHI and mRHI), the experience of ownership is established through bottom-up integration and top-down prediction of proximodistal cues (visuotactile or visuomotor) within the peripersonal space. It seems, however, that depending on whether the sensory signals are externally (RHI) or self-generated (VHI and mRHI), the top-down expectation signals are qualitatively different. On the one hand, in the RHI the sensory correlations are modulated by top-down influences which constitute empirically induced priors related to the internal (generative) model of the body. On the other hand, in the VHI and mRHI body ownership is actively shaped by processes which allow for continuous comparison between the expected and the actual sensory consequences of the actions. Ample research demonstrates that the differential processing of the predicted and the reafferent information is addressed by the central nervous system via an internal (forward) model or corollary discharge. Indeed, results from the VHI and mRHI suggest that, in action-contexts, the mechanism underlying body ownership could be similar to the forward model. Crucially, forward models integrate across all self-generated sensory signals including not only proximodistal (i.e., visuotactile or visuomotor) but also purely distal sensory cues (i.e., visuoauditory). Thus, if body ownership results from a consistency of a forward model, it will be affected by the (in)congruency of purely distal cues provided that they inform about action-consequences and are relevant to a goal-oriented task. Specifically, they constitute a corrective error signal. Here, we explicitly addressed this question. To test our hypothesis, we devised an embodied virtual reality-based motor task where action outcomes were signaled by distinct auditory cues. By manipulating the cues with respect to their spatial, temporal and semantic congruency, we show that purely distal (visuoauditory) feedback which violates predictions about action outcomes compromises both performance and body ownership. These results demonstrate, for the first time, that body ownership is influenced by not only externally and self-generated cues which pertain to the body within the peripersonal space but also those arising outside of the body. Hence, during goal-oriented tasks body ownership may result from the consistency of forward models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Klaudia Grechuta
- Department of Information and Communication Technologies, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain.,Institute for Bioengineering of Catalonia (IBEC), The Barcelona Institute of Science and Technology (BIST), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Laura Ulysse
- Computational Neuroscience Group, Department of Information and Communication Technologies, Center for Brain and Cognition, Pompeu Fabra University, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Belén Rubio Ballester
- Institute for Bioengineering of Catalonia (IBEC), The Barcelona Institute of Science and Technology (BIST), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Paul F M J Verschure
- Institute for Bioengineering of Catalonia (IBEC), The Barcelona Institute of Science and Technology (BIST), Barcelona, Spain.,Catalan Institution for Research and Advanced Studies (ICREA), Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Tamè L, Azañón E, Longo MR. A Conceptual Model of Tactile Processing across Body Features of Size, Shape, Side, and Spatial Location. Front Psychol 2019; 10:291. [PMID: 30863333 PMCID: PMC6399380 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00291] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2018] [Accepted: 01/29/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
The processing of touch depends of multiple factors, such as the properties of the skin and type of receptors stimulated, as well as features related to the actual configuration and shape of the body itself. A large body of research has focused on the effect that the nature of the stimuli has on tactile processing. Less research, however, has focused on features beyond the nature of the touch. In this review, we focus on some features related to the body that have been investigated for less time and in a more fragmented way. These include the symmetrical quality of the two sides of the body, the postural configuration of the body, as well as the size and shape of different body parts. We will describe what we consider three key aspects: (1) how and at which stages tactile information is integrated between different parts and sides of the body; (2) how tactile signals are integrated with online and stored postural configurations of the body, regarded as priors; (3) and how tactile signals are integrated with representations of body size and shape. Here, we describe how these different body dimensions affect integration of tactile information as well as guide motor behavior by integrating them in a single model of tactile processing. We review a wide range of neuropsychological, neuroimaging, and neurophysiological data and suggest a revised model of tactile integration on the basis of the one proposed previously by Longo et al.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luigi Tamè
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, United Kingdom.,School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, United Kingdom
| | - Elena Azañón
- Institute of Psychology, Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany.,Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences, Magdeburg, Germany.,Department of Behavioral Neurology, Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Matthew R Longo
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Apparent speed of motion concomitant with action alters with delay. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0212105. [PMID: 30768623 PMCID: PMC6377110 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2018] [Accepted: 01/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Multiple studies have shown action to affect perception of motion. The speed intended in the generation of a motion by action affects the apparent speed of the motion. However, it was unclear whether action with no intention of speed affects the apparent speed of a motion. In Experiment 1, we investigated the apparent speed of a motion following a key press action. We manipulated the delay from the action to the consequent motion for shifting the timing of efference copy and found the apparent speed decreasing with increases in the delay. This could be because it is known that speed irrelevant action caused expansion of perceived duration of the consequent stimulus and it might have influenced the result in Experiment 1, we investigated the apparent duration of the action consequent static (Ex. 2-1) and motion (Ex. 2-2) stimulus. We found that the apparent duration was not changed with delay. Moreover, the apparent speed and duration had different characteristics on delay. These results were discussed in terms of the sense of agency.
Collapse
|
32
|
Do movements contribute to sense of body ownership? Rubber hand illusion in expert pianists. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 83:185-195. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1137-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2018] [Accepted: 12/10/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
33
|
Christensen MS, Grünbaum T. Sense of agency for movements. Conscious Cogn 2018; 65:27-47. [PMID: 30007133 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2018] [Revised: 04/30/2018] [Accepted: 07/02/2018] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
In this paper, we argue that the comparator model is not a satisfactory model of sense of agency (SoA). We present a theoretical argument and experimental studies. We show (1) most studies of SoA neglect a distinction between SoA associated with movements (narrow SoA) and SoA associated with environmental events (broad SoA); (2) the comparator model emerges from experimental studies of sensory consequences narrowly associated with movements; (3) narrow SoA can be explained by a comparator model, but a motor signal model is simpler and explain narrow SoA equally well; and (4) standard experimental paradigms study only broad SoA. Finally, we present results from two experiments, where we have failed to induce illusory narrow SoA in healthy participants. We believe our experimental approaches should have led to illusory SoA, if the comparator model of SoA was correct. The results challenge proponents of the comparator model of narrow SoA.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Schram Christensen
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Copenhagen, Denmark; DTU Compute, Department of Applied Mathematics and Computer Science, Technical University of Denmark, Denmark.
| | - Thor Grünbaum
- Section of Philosophy, Department of Media, Cognition, and Communication, University of Copenhagen, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Thieme H, Morkisch N, Mehrholz J, Pohl M, Behrens J, Borgetto B, Dohle C. Mirror therapy for improving motor function after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2018; 7:CD008449. [PMID: 29993119 PMCID: PMC6513639 DOI: 10.1002/14651858.cd008449.pub3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 112] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Mirror therapy is used to improve motor function after stroke. During mirror therapy, a mirror is placed in the person's midsagittal plane, thus reflecting movements of the non-paretic side as if it were the affected side. OBJECTIVES To summarise the effectiveness of mirror therapy compared with no treatment, placebo or sham therapy, or other treatments for improving motor function and motor impairment after stroke. We also aimed to assess the effects of mirror therapy on activities of daily living, pain, and visuospatial neglect. SEARCH METHODS We searched the Cochrane Stroke Group's Trials Register, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), MEDLINE, Embase, CINAHL, AMED, PsycINFO and PEDro (last searched 16 August 2017). We also handsearched relevant conference proceedings, trials and research registers, checked reference lists, and contacted trialists, researchers and experts in our field of study. SELECTION CRITERIA We included randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and randomised cross-over trials comparing mirror therapy with any control intervention for people after stroke. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS Two review authors independently selected trials based on the inclusion criteria, documented the methodological quality, assessed risks of bias in the included studies, and extracted data. We assessed the quality of the evidence using the GRADE approach. We analysed the results as standardised mean differences (SMDs) or mean differences (MDs) for continuous variables, and as odds ratios (ORs) for dichotomous variables. MAIN RESULTS We included 62 studies with a total of 1982 participants that compared mirror therapy with other interventions. Of these, 57 were randomised controlled trials and five randomised cross-over trials. Participants had a mean age of 59 years (30 to 73 years). Mirror therapy was provided three to seven times a week, between 15 and 60 minutes for each session for two to eight weeks (on average five times a week, 30 minutes a session for four weeks).When compared with all other interventions, we found moderate-quality evidence that mirror therapy has a significant positive effect on motor function (SMD 0.47, 95% CI 0.27 to 0.67; 1173 participants; 36 studies) and motor impairment (SMD 0.49, 95% CI 0.32 to 0.66; 1292 participants; 39 studies). However, effects on motor function are influenced by the type of control intervention. Additionally, based on moderate-quality evidence, mirror therapy may improve activities of daily living (SMD 0.48, 95% CI 0.30 to 0.65; 622 participants; 19 studies). We found low-quality evidence for a significant positive effect on pain (SMD -0.89, 95% CI -1.67 to -0.11; 248 participants; 6 studies) and no clear effect for improving visuospatial neglect (SMD 1.06, 95% CI -0.10 to 2.23; 175 participants; 5 studies). No adverse effects were reported. AUTHORS' CONCLUSIONS The results indicate evidence for the effectiveness of mirror therapy for improving upper extremity motor function, motor impairment, activities of daily living, and pain, at least as an adjunct to conventional rehabilitation for people after stroke. Major limitations are small sample sizes and lack of reporting of methodological details, resulting in uncertain evidence quality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Holm Thieme
- Erste Europäische Schule für Physiotherapie, Ergotherapie und Logopädie, Klinik Bavaria KreischaKreischa, SachsenGermany
- Martin Luther University Halle‐WittenbergInstitute for Health and Nursing Science, German Center for Evidence‐based NursingHalle/SaaleGermany
- Fakultät Soziale Arbeit und GesundheitHAWK Hochschule für angewandte Wissenschaft und KunstHildesheimGermany31134
| | - Nadine Morkisch
- Charité ‐ University Medicine BerlinCenter for Stroke Research BerlinCharitéplatz 1BerlinGermany0117
- MEDIAN Klinik Berlin‐KladowKladower Damm 223BerlinGermany14089
| | - Jan Mehrholz
- Technical University DresdenDepartment of Public Health, Dresden Medical SchoolFetscherstr. 74DresdenGermany01307
| | - Marcus Pohl
- Helios Klinik Schloss PulsnitzNeurological RehabilitationWittgensteiner Str. 1PulsnitzSaxonyGermany01896
| | - Johann Behrens
- Martin Luther University Halle‐WittenbergInstitute for Health and Nursing Science, German Center for Evidence‐based NursingHalle/SaaleGermany
| | - Bernhard Borgetto
- Fakultät Soziale Arbeit und GesundheitHAWK Hochschule für angewandte Wissenschaft und KunstHildesheimGermany31134
| | - Christian Dohle
- Charité ‐ University Medicine BerlinCenter for Stroke Research BerlinCharitéplatz 1BerlinGermany0117
- MEDIAN Klinik Berlin‐KladowKladower Damm 223BerlinGermany14089
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
On the relation between body ownership and sense of agency: A link at the level of sensory-related signals. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2018. [PMID: 29533775 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.03.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023] Open
Abstract
The relation between sense of body ownership and sense of agency is still highly debated. Here we investigated in a large sample of healthy participants the associations between several implicit and explicit indexes of the two senses. Specifically, we examined the correlations between proprioceptive shift (implicit measure) and questionnaire on the subjective experience of ownership (explicit measure) within the rubber hand illusion paradigm (body ownership), and intentional binding (implicit measure), attenuation of the intensity of auditory outcomes of actions (implicit measure) and questionnaire on the subjective experience of authorship (explicit measure) within the Libet's clock paradigm (sense of agency). Our results showed that proprioceptive shift was positively correlated with the attenuation of auditory outcomes. No significant correlations were found between the explicit measures of the two senses. We argue that the individual spatiotemporal constraints subserving the integration of sensory-related signals (implicit signature) would be common to both senses, whereas their subjective experience (explicit signature) would rely on additional processes specific for any given sense.
Collapse
|
36
|
Spontaneous imitative movements induced by an illusory embodied fake hand. Neuropsychologia 2018; 111:77-84. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2017] [Revised: 01/06/2018] [Accepted: 01/16/2018] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
|
37
|
Burin D, Garbarini F, Bruno V, Fossataro C, Destefanis C, Berti A, Pia L. Movements and body ownership: Evidence from the rubber hand illusion after mechanical limb immobilization. Neuropsychologia 2017; 107:41-47. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2017] [Revised: 10/25/2017] [Accepted: 11/02/2017] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
|
38
|
Ratcliffe N, Newport R. The Effect of Visual, Spatial and Temporal Manipulations on Embodiment and Action. Front Hum Neurosci 2017; 11:227. [PMID: 28522968 PMCID: PMC5415570 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00227] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2016] [Accepted: 04/19/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The feeling of owning and controlling the body relies on the integration and interpretation of sensory input from multiple sources with respect to existing representations of the bodily self. Illusion paradigms involving multisensory manipulations have demonstrated that while the senses of ownership and agency are strongly related, these two components of bodily experience may be dissociable and differentially affected by alterations to sensory input. Importantly, however, much of the current literature has focused on the application of sensory manipulations to external objects or virtual representations of the self that are visually incongruent with the viewer’s own body and which are not part of the existing body representation. The current experiment used MIRAGE-mediated reality to investigate how manipulating the visual, spatial and temporal properties of the participant’s own hand (as opposed to a fake/virtual limb) affected embodiment and action. Participants viewed two representations of their right hand inside a MIRAGE multisensory illusions box with opposing visual (normal or grossly distorted), temporal (synchronous or asynchronous) and spatial (precise real location or false location) manipulations applied to each hand. Subjective experiences of ownership and agency towards each hand were measured alongside an objective measure of perceived hand location using a pointing task. The subjective sense of agency was always anchored to the synchronous hand, regardless of physical appearance and location. Subjective ownership also moved with the synchronous hand, except when both the location and appearance of the synchronous limb were incongruent with that of the real limb. Objective pointing measures displayed a similar pattern, however movement synchrony was not sufficient to drive a complete shift in perceived hand location, indicating a greater reliance on the spatial location of the real hand. The results suggest that while the congruence of self-generated movement is a sufficient driver for the sense of agency, the sense of ownership is additionally sensitive to cues about the visual appearance and spatial location of one’s own body.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Roger Newport
- School of Psychology, University of NottinghamNottingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
My action lasts longer: Potential link between subjective time and agency during voluntary action. Conscious Cogn 2017; 51:243-257. [PMID: 28412643 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.04.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2017] [Revised: 03/12/2017] [Accepted: 04/07/2017] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Time perception distorts across different phases of bodily movement. During motor execution, sensory feedback matching an internal sensorimotor prediction is perceived to last longer. The sensorimotor prediction also underlies sense of agency. We investigated association between subjective time and agency during voluntary action. Participants performed hand action while watching a video feedback of their hand with various delays to manipulate agency. The perceived duration and agency over the video feedback were judged. Minimal delay of the video feedback resulted in longer perceived duration than the actual duration and stronger agency, while substantial feedback delay resulted in shorter perceived duration and weaker agency. These fluctuations of perceived duration and agency were nullified by the feedback of other's hand instead of their own, but not by inverted feedback from a third-person perspective. Subjective time during action might be associated with agency stemming from sensorimotor prediction, and self-other distinction based on bodily appearance.
Collapse
|
40
|
Tamè L, Carr A, Longo MR. Vision of the body improves inter-hemispheric integration of tactile-motor responses. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2017; 175:21-27. [PMID: 28259727 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2016] [Revised: 02/01/2017] [Accepted: 02/28/2017] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Sensory input from and motor output to the two sides of the body needs to be continuously integrated between the two cerebral hemispheres. This integration can be measured through its cost in terms of processing speed. In simple detection tasks, reaction times (RTs) are faster when stimuli are presented to the side of the body ipsilateral to the body part used to respond. This advantage - the contralateral-ipsilateral difference (also known as the crossed-uncrossed difference: CUD) - is thought to reflect inter-hemispheric interactions needed for sensorimotor information to be integrated between the two hemispheres. Several studies have shown that non-informative vision of the body enhances performance in tactile tasks. However, it is unknown whether the CUD can be similarly affected by vision. Here, we investigated whether the CUD is modulated by vision of the body (i.e., the stimulated hand) by presenting tactile stimuli unpredictably on the middle fingers when one hand was visible (i.e., either the right or left hand). Participants detected the stimulus and responded as fast as possible using either their left or right foot. Consistent with previous results, a clear CUD (5.8ms) was apparent on the unseen hand. Critically, however, no such effect was found on the hand that was visible (-2.2ms). Thus, when touch is delivered to a seen hand, the usual cost in processing speed of responding with a contralateral effector is eliminated. This result suggests that vision of the body improves the interhemispheric integration of tactile-motor responses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luigi Tamè
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom.
| | - Alex Carr
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Matthew R Longo
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Affiliation(s)
- Roy Salomon
- Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Klaver M, Dijkerman HC. Bodily Experience in Schizophrenia: Factors Underlying a Disturbed Sense of Body Ownership. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:305. [PMID: 27378895 PMCID: PMC4911393 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00305] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2016] [Accepted: 06/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Emerging evidence is now challenging the view that patients diagnosed with schizophrenia experience a selective deficit in their sense of agency. Additional disturbances seem to exist in their sense of body ownership. However, the factors underlying this disturbance in body ownership remain elusive. Knowledge of these factors, and increased understanding of how body ownership is related to other abnormalities seen in schizophrenia, could ultimately advance development of new treatments. Research on body ownership in schizophrenia has mainly been investigated with the rubber hand illusion (RHI). Schizophrenia patients show higher susceptibility to the RHI, which may be explained by a stronger reliance on multisensory information over weaker stored body representations. This review shows that a coherent sense of body ownership arises from the integration of both bottom-up sensory processes and higher order, top-down bodily- and perceptual representations. Multisensory integration, temporal binding, anticipation, intention and efferent signals all partly modulate the complex experience of body ownership. Specifically, we propose that patients with schizophrenia have weaker stored body representations, and rely to a greater extent on external stimuli, such as visual information, due to imprecise or highly variable internal predictions. Moreover, the reduced sense of agency in schizophrenia may additionally contribute to the disturbed sense of body ownership, as evidence from healthy participants suggests that agency and body ownership are interrelated. Vice versa, a reduced sense of body ownership may also contribute to a reduced sense of agency. Future studies should explicitly target the precise relationship between the two in schizophrenia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maayke Klaver
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University CS Utrecht, Netherlands
| | - H Chris Dijkerman
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University CS Utrecht, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Desantis A, Waszak F, Gorea A. Agency alters perceptual decisions about action-outcomes. Exp Brain Res 2016; 234:2819-27. [PMID: 27278083 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4684-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2016] [Accepted: 05/24/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Humans experience themselves as agents, capable of controlling their actions and the outcomes they generate (i.e., the sense of agency). Inferences of agency are not infallible. Research shows that we often attribute outcomes to our agency even though they are caused by another agent. Moreover, agents report the sensory events they generate to be less intense compared to the events that are generated externally. These effects have been assessed using highly suprathreshold stimuli and subjective measurements. Consequently, it remains unclear whether experiencing oneself as an agent lead to a decision criterion change and/or a sensitivity change. Here, we investigate this issue. Participants were told that their key presses generated an upward dot motion but that on 30 % of the trials the computer would take over and display a downward motion. The upward/downward dot motion was presented at participant's discrimination threshold. Participants were asked to indicate whether they (upward motion) or the computer (downward motion) generated the motion. This group of participants was compared with a 'no-agency' group who performed the same task except that subjects did not execute any actions to generate the dot motion. We observed that the agency group reported seeing more frequently the motion they expected to generate (i.e., upward motion) than the no-agency group. This suggests that agency distorts our experience of (allegedly) caused events by altering perceptual decision processes, so that, in ambiguous contexts, externally generated events are experienced as the outcomes of one's actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Desantis
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes and CNRS, 45 rue des Saints Pères, 75006, Paris, France.
| | - Florian Waszak
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes and CNRS, 45 rue des Saints Pères, 75006, Paris, France
| | - Andrei Gorea
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes and CNRS, 45 rue des Saints Pères, 75006, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Brain Regions Associated to a Kinesthetic Illusion Evoked by Watching a Video of One's Own Moving Hand. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0131970. [PMID: 26287488 PMCID: PMC4544853 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2014] [Accepted: 02/12/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
It is well known that kinesthetic illusions can be induced by stimulation of several sensory systems (proprioception, touch, vision…). In this study we investigated the cerebral network underlying a kinesthetic illusion induced by visual stimulation by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans. Participants were instructed to keep their hand still while watching the video of their own moving hand (Self Hand) or that of someone else's moving hand (Other Hand). In the Self Hand condition they experienced an illusory sensation that their hand was moving whereas the Other Hand condition did not induce any kinesthetic illusion. The contrast between the Self Hand and Other Hand conditions showed significant activation in the left dorsal and ventral premotor cortices, in the left Superior and Inferior Parietal lobules, at the right Occipito-Temporal junction as well as in bilateral Insula and Putamen. Most strikingly, there was no activation in the primary motor and somatosensory cortices, whilst previous studies have reported significant activation in these regions for vibration-induced kinesthetic illusions. To our knowledge, this is the first study that indicates that humans can experience kinesthetic perception without activation in the primary motor and somatosensory areas. We conclude that under some conditions watching a video of one's own moving hand could lead to activation of a network that is usually involved in processing copies of efference, thus leading to the illusory perception that the real hand is indeed moving.
Collapse
|
45
|
Imaizumi S, Asai T. Dissociation of agency and body ownership following visuomotor temporal recalibration. Front Integr Neurosci 2015; 9:35. [PMID: 25999826 PMCID: PMC4423341 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2015] [Accepted: 04/22/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Bodily self-consciousness consists of one's sense of agency (I am causing an action) and body ownership (my body belongs to me). Both stem from the temporal congruence between different modalities, although some visuomotor temporal incongruence is acceptable for agency. To examine the association or dissociation between agency and body ownership in the context of different temporal sensitivities, we applied a temporal recalibration paradigm, in which subjective synchrony between asynchronous hand action and its visual feedback can be perceived after exposure to the asynchronous visuomotor stimulation. In the experiment, participants continuously clasped and unclasped their hand while watching an online video of their hand that was presented with delays of 50, 110, 170, 230, 290, and 350 ms. Then, they rated a video of their hand with a delay of 50 ms (test stimulus) with respect to the synchrony between hand action and hand video and the perceived agency over the video. Moreover, proprioceptive drift of participants' hand location toward the hand video during the exposure was measured as an index of illusory body ownership. Results indicated that perception of agency emerged over the delayed hand video as subjective visuomotor synchrony was recalibrated, but that body ownership did not emerge for the delayed video, even after the recalibration. We suggest that there is a dissociation between agency and body ownership following visuomotor temporal recalibration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shu Imaizumi
- Graduate School of Engineering, Chiba University Chiba, Japan ; NTT Communication Science Laboratories, NTT Corporation Kanagawa, Japan ; Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Tokyo, Japan
| | - Tomohisa Asai
- NTT Communication Science Laboratories, NTT Corporation Kanagawa, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Burin D, Livelli A, Garbarini F, Fossataro C, Folegatti A, Gindri P, Pia L. Are movements necessary for the sense of body ownership? Evidence from the rubber hand illusion in pure hemiplegic patients. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0117155. [PMID: 25775041 PMCID: PMC4361688 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117155] [Citation(s) in RCA: 78] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2014] [Accepted: 12/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
A question still debated within cognitive neuroscience is whether signals present during actions significantly contribute to the emergence of human’s body ownership. In the present study, we aimed at answer this question by means of a neuropsychological approach. We administered the classical rubber hand illusion paradigm to a group of healthy participants and to a group of neurological patients affected by a complete left upper limb hemiplegia, but without any propriceptive/tactile deficits. The illusion strength was measured both subjectively (i.e., by a self-report questionnaire) and behaviorally (i.e., the location of one’s own hand is shifted towards the rubber hand). We aimed at examining whether, and to which extent, an enduring absence of movements related signals affects body ownership. Our results showed that patients displayed, respect to healthy participants, stronger illusory effects when the left (affected) hand was stimulated and no effects when the right (unaffected) hand was stimulated. In other words, hemiplegics had a weaker/more flexible sense of body ownership for the affected hand, but an enhanced/more rigid one for the healthy hand. Possible interpretations of such asymmetrical distribution of body ownership, as well as limits of our results, are discussed. Broadly speaking, our findings suggest that the alteration of the normal flow of signals present during movements impacts on human’s body ownership. This in turn, means that movements have a role per se in developing and maintaining a coherent body ownership.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dalila Burin
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Alessandro Livelli
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Francesca Garbarini
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Carlotta Fossataro
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Alessia Folegatti
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Patrizia Gindri
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
- San Camillo Hospital, Turin, Italy
| | - Lorenzo Pia
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
- NIT Neuroscience Institute of Turin, Turin, Italy
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
The projected hand illusion: component structure in a community sample and association with demographics, cognition, and psychotic-like experiences. Atten Percept Psychophys 2014; 77:207-19. [DOI: 10.3758/s13414-014-0748-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
48
|
Piryankova IV, Wong HY, Linkenauger SA, Stinson C, Longo MR, Bülthoff HH, Mohler BJ. Owning an overweight or underweight body: distinguishing the physical, experienced and virtual body. PLoS One 2014; 9:e103428. [PMID: 25083784 PMCID: PMC4118886 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103428] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2014] [Accepted: 07/01/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Our bodies are the most intimately familiar objects we encounter in our perceptual environment. Virtual reality provides a unique method to allow us to experience having a very different body from our own, thereby providing a valuable method to explore the plasticity of body representation. In this paper, we show that women can experience ownership over a whole virtual body that is considerably smaller or larger than their physical body. In order to gain a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying body ownership, we use an embodiment questionnaire, and introduce two new behavioral response measures: an affordance estimation task (indirect measure of body size) and a body size estimation task (direct measure of body size). Interestingly, after viewing the virtual body from first person perspective, both the affordance and the body size estimation tasks indicate a change in the perception of the size of the participant's experienced body. The change is biased by the size of the virtual body (overweight or underweight). Another novel aspect of our study is that we distinguish between the physical, experienced and virtual bodies, by asking participants to provide affordance and body size estimations for each of the three bodies separately. This methodological point is important for virtual reality experiments investigating body ownership of a virtual body, because it offers a better understanding of which cues (e.g. visual, proprioceptive, memory, or a combination thereof) influence body perception, and whether the impact of these cues can vary between different setups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ivelina V. Piryankova
- Department of Human Perception, Cognition and Action, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
- Department of Philosophy of Neuroscience, Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen, Germany
- * E-mail: (IVP); (HHB); (BJM)
| | - Hong Yu Wong
- Department of Philosophy of Neuroscience, Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen, Germany
| | | | - Catherine Stinson
- Department of Human Perception, Cognition and Action, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
- Department of Philosophy of Neuroscience, Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Matthew R. Longo
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, England
| | - Heinrich H. Bülthoff
- Department of Human Perception, Cognition and Action, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Engineering, Korea University, Seoul, Korea
- * E-mail: (IVP); (HHB); (BJM)
| | - Betty J. Mohler
- Department of Human Perception, Cognition and Action, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
- * E-mail: (IVP); (HHB); (BJM)
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
The moving rubber hand illusion revisited: comparing movements and visuotactile stimulation to induce illusory ownership. Conscious Cogn 2014; 26:117-32. [PMID: 24705182 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 195] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2013] [Revised: 02/13/2014] [Accepted: 02/15/2014] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
The rubber hand illusion is a perceptual illusion in which a model hand is experienced as part of one's own body. In the present study we directly compared the classical illusion, based on visuotactile stimulation, with a rubber hand illusion based on active and passive movements. We examined the question of which combinations of sensory and motor cues are the most potent in inducing the illusion by subjective ratings and an objective measure (proprioceptive drift). In particular, we were interested in whether the combination of afferent and efferent signals in active movements results in the same illusion as in the purely passive modes. Our results show that the illusion is equally strong in all three cases. This demonstrates that different combinations of sensory input can lead to a very similar phenomenological experience and indicates that the illusion can be induced by any combination of multisensory information.
Collapse
|
50
|
Brown LE, Goodale MA. A brief review of the role of training in near-tool effects. Front Psychol 2013; 4:576. [PMID: 24027545 PMCID: PMC3759798 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00576] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2013] [Accepted: 08/11/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Research suggests that, like near-hand effects, visual targets appearing near the tip of a hand-held real or virtual tool are treated differently than other targets. This paper reviews neurological and behavioral evidence relevant to near-tool effects and describes how the effect varies with the functional properties of the tool and the knowledge of the participant. In particular, the paper proposes that motor knowledge plays a key role in the appearance of near-tool effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liana E Brown
- Department of Psychology, Trent University Peterborough, ON, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|