1
|
Zhang Z, Feng Z, Gerow KG, Wallhead T, Zhu Q. Seeing the unseen boundary behind you: Predicting the out-of-bounds of flick serves in playing badminton doubles. PSYCHOLOGY OF SPORT AND EXERCISE 2024; 75:102702. [PMID: 39009099 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2024.102702] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/04/2024] [Revised: 06/09/2024] [Accepted: 07/10/2024] [Indexed: 07/17/2024]
Abstract
Previous research on affordance perception has shown that elite athletes can relate the environmental features to their motor abilities to detect the opportunities for action. In playing badminton doubles, experienced players can often anticipate whether a flick serve will go out-of-bounds or not. A field study was performed to examine if such an ability was associated with a developed affordance perception. Twenty-two players with and without badminton playing experience were asked to play a receiver on court to judge without looking back whether a flick serve was out-of-bounds in both standing and retreating conditions. Forty flick serves were randomly delivered to either inner or outer rear corner of the receiver's box and land adjacent to the back service line. Each player's ability to reach high and retreat backward with a racquet was assessed on court. The results showed that predictions were better in the inner direction than the outer direction, but experienced players were more sensitive and conservative overall. Playing experience appeared to be a consistent predictor for judgment accuracy. In identifying the affordance information that could have been utilized by players for accurate predictions, only experienced players demonstrated the ability to utilize both body-scaled and action-scaled affordances effectively and reliably for better predictions in both standing and retreating conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zuoqi Zhang
- Division of Kinesiology and Health, University of Wyoming, USA
| | - Zhichen Feng
- Division of Kinesiology and Health, University of Wyoming, USA
| | - Kenneth G Gerow
- Department of Mathematics and Statistics, University of Wyoming, USA
| | | | - Qin Zhu
- Division of Kinesiology and Health, University of Wyoming, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Harrison RE, Giesel M, Hesse C. No evidence for top-down expertise effects on action perception in sprinters using static images. Neuropsychologia 2024; 202:108945. [PMID: 38944412 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108945] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2023] [Revised: 06/07/2024] [Accepted: 06/27/2024] [Indexed: 07/01/2024]
Abstract
Athletes have been found to demonstrate a superior ability to detect subtle variations in dynamic displays (e.g., point-light displays and videos) depicting expert actions compared to non-athletes. The current study aimed to determine whether this advantage also exists when dynamic information is unavailable (i.e., using static images). Using a staircase procedure, two frames from a video depicting an athlete either walking (everyday action) or performing a sprint start (expert action) were presented, and athletes (sprinters) and non-athletes were asked to indicate whether the images were identical or different. We examined whether presenting the images sequentially (temporal task) or simultaneously (spatial task) influenced participants' discrimination performance. We predicted that the sprinters would outperform the non-sprinters in the spatial task as body postures could be compared directly but not in the temporal task due to larger representational momentum effects for athletes. Contrary to our hypotheses, the sprinters and non-sprinters performed similarly in all tasks and conditions. In line with the prediction that representational momentum may impair performance, participants' thresholds were lower for the spatial than the temporal task. However, post-hoc analysis suggested that this effect is likely to be better explained by a task order effect whereby participants who completed the temporal task first exhibited an advantage in the spatial task, while there were no performance differences for participants who completed the opposite task order. In sum, our results provide no evidence for the idea that motor expertise affects action perception (i.e., perceptual resonance) in a simple psychophysical task employing static images.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Róisín Elaine Harrison
- School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, Scotland, UK; School of Applied Social Studies, The Robert Gordon University, Aberdeen, Scotland, UK.
| | - Martin Giesel
- School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, Scotland, UK
| | - Constanze Hesse
- School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, Scotland, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Hansen MK. Perceiving affordances and the problem of visually indiscernible kinds. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1388852. [PMID: 39295750 PMCID: PMC11409843 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1388852] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2024] [Accepted: 08/16/2024] [Indexed: 09/21/2024] Open
Abstract
In this study, I defend the claim that we can perceptually experience what objects afford when we engage with objects belonging to natural or artificial categorical high-level kinds. Experiencing affordances perceptually positions us to act in specific ways. The main aim of this study was to argue that this view has explanatory advantages over alternative views. An increasingly popular view within the philosophy of perception, most famously defended by Susanna Siegel, claims that we sometimes visually experience natural and artificial objects as belonging to categorical high-level kinds. When visually experiencing a lemon, one does not only experience its low-level properties such as shape and color, sometimes one also experiences the object as a lemon. A challenge arises when attempting to explain what happens when one experiences an object that is experientially indistinguishable from another object, yet these objects belong to different high-level categorical kinds. For instance, if someone perceptually experiences a lemon as a lemon, her experience can be considered as accurately representing or presenting a lemon. However, if the subject perceptually experiences a lemon-shaped soap bar, which cannot be discriminated from a real lemon by sight alone, the experience is deemed inaccurate because there is no real lemon present. The problem is that such a judgment seems counterintuitive; unlike with hallucinations and illusions, there seems to be nothing wrong with how the object appears. Therefore, it is difficult to understand how the mistake could be a perceptual mistake. I will first present arguments supporting the claim that when we visually encounter objects such as lemons, we sometimes also perceive the affordances of these objects-what they provide or offer us. I will further argue that this perspective on affordances offers a more compelling explanation than other alternative accounts when it comes to our perception of visually indistinguishable objects that nonetheless belong to categorically distinct high-level kinds.
Collapse
|
4
|
de Arruda DG, Barp F, Felisberto G, Tkak C, Wagman JB, Stoffregen TA. Perception of Affordances in Female Volleyball Players: Serving Short versus Serving to the Sideline. RESEARCH QUARTERLY FOR EXERCISE AND SPORT 2024; 95:573-580. [PMID: 38100607 DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2023.2279989] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/27/2023] [Accepted: 10/31/2023] [Indexed: 12/17/2023]
Abstract
Purpose: We investigated youth volleyball players' perception of affordances for different types of serves. Methods: Participants were separated into Less Experienced (n = 13, maximum of 3 years of competitive volleyball experience) and More Experienced (n = 11, minimum of 4 years of competitive volleyball experience) groups. In the Sideline Condition, participants were asked to report the narrowest gap, relative to either the left or right sideline, into which they could serve the ball. In the Short Condition, participants were asked to report the narrowest gap, relative to the net, into which they could serve the ball. Participants then attempted to serve into their reported gaps. Results: The proportion of successful serves was greater for the More Experienced group than for the Less Experienced group, but a statistically significant interaction revealed that this was true only when performing sideline serves. As expected, reported servable gaps were larger for the Short Serve condition (M = 3.66 m, SD = 0.67 m) than for the Sideline Serve condition (M = 1.93 m, SD = 0.71 m), F(1, 22) = 80.45, p < .001, partial η2 = 0.79. Conclusions: Our study extends previous work to a different sport (volleyball) and to different sport-specific actions (serving the ball). The finding that perceived minimum servable gap sizes were larger for short serves than for sideline serves is consistent with differences in the constraints operating on the two types of serves in the context of game play.
Collapse
|
5
|
Benguigui N, Rioult F, Kauffmann F, Miller-Dicks M, Murphy CP. Henri Cochet's theory of angles in tennis (1933) reveals a new facet of anticipation. Sci Rep 2024; 14:3364. [PMID: 38337002 PMCID: PMC10858201 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-53136-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2023] [Accepted: 01/29/2024] [Indexed: 02/12/2024] Open
Abstract
In this study, we tested the theory of angles that was proposed almost a century ago by the tennis player Henri Cochet. This theory proposes that expert tennis players should position themselves on the bisector of the angle of the opponent's possibilities in order to optimize shot return, suggesting a geometric occupation of the court relative to the opponent's affordances; namely what he/she is capable of doing. We tested this hypothesis by analysing player and ball positioning data from professional tennis matches recorded with a Hawk-Eye system. We compared this hypothesis with two alternative computational and probabilistic hypotheses which would consist in positioning oneself on the average or the median of the shots usually played from a given location. The results show that expert tennis players apply the principles of the theory of angles and thus confirm Henri Cochet's intuition. That is, for lateral court positioning, a geometric strategy is deemed optimal by expert players. It also appears that the more experienced the players are, the more precise their application of this strategy becomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Matt Miller-Dicks
- School of Sport, Health and Exercise Science, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK
| | - Colm P Murphy
- Cardiff School of Sport and Health Sciences, Cardiff Metropolitan University, Cardiff, UK
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Oku K, Tanaka S, Kida N. Dissociation of perception and motor execution of lower limb in multi-directional movements. Sci Rep 2023; 13:17237. [PMID: 37821576 PMCID: PMC10567789 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-44544-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2023] [Accepted: 10/10/2023] [Indexed: 10/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Estimating the action capability is vital for humans to move their bodies successfully. Researchers have proposed reachability as an overestimation of motor abilities by judging unreachable distances as reachable. The existing literature has mainly investigated the sagittal direction, but multi-directional reachability is unexplored. This study examined the relationship between perception and motor using the reaching of the lower limbs in multiple directions. We asked 16 adults to reach targets projected onto the floor at 21 locations (seven directions and three distances) to estimate the reaching time. We found that the reaching time slowed as the direction increased toward the contralateral side, but the subjective reaching time did not change with direction. Multiple regression analysis showed that the subjective reaching time could be calculated accurately, mainly using the duration from the toe leaving the ground to movement completion. These results suggest that changes in direction may not be perceived precisely by the motor system of the lower limbs and that the subjective reaching time was strongly affected by the time after the toe left the ground. Our findings provide novel insights into the relationship between motor and perception in multiple directions, which may provide a new strategy for the maximal performance of lower-limb movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyosuke Oku
- Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
| | - Shinsuke Tanaka
- Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
- Institute for Liberal Arts and Sciences, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
| | - Noriyuki Kida
- Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Kyoto Institute of Technology, Kyoto, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Jones KS, Garcia NA, Wilder DL. Investigating What Variables People Pick Up When Perceiving Other People’s Maximum Vertical One Degree-of-Freedom Reach Heights to Inform the Design of Assistive Robots. ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2023. [DOI: 10.1080/10407413.2023.2192200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/05/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Keith S. Jones
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
| | - Nicholas A. Garcia
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
| | - Dana L. Wilder
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Does optic flow provide information about actions? Atten Percept Psychophys 2023; 85:1287-1303. [PMID: 36918506 PMCID: PMC10013980 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02674-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/09/2023] [Indexed: 03/16/2023]
Abstract
Optic flow, the pattern of light generated in the visual field by motion of objects and the observer's body, serves as information that underwrites perception of events, actions, and affordances. This visual pattern informs the observer about their own actions in relation to their surroundings, as well as those of others. This study explored the limits of action detection for others as well as the role of optic flow. First-person videos were created using camera recordings of the actor's perspective as they performed various movements (jumping jacks, jumping, squatting, sitting, etc.). In three experiments participants attempted to detect the action from first-person video footage using open ended responses (Experiment 1), forced-choice responses (Experiment 2), and a match-to-sample paradigm (Experiment 3). It was discovered that some actions are more difficult to detect than others. When the task was challenging (Experiment 1) athletes were more accurate, but this was not the case in Experiments 2 and 3. All actions were identified above chance level across viewpoints, suggesting that invariant information was detected and used to perform the task.
Collapse
|
9
|
Peker AT, Böge V, Bailey G, Wagman JB, Stoffregen TA. Perception of Affordances in Soccer: Kicking for Power Versus Kicking for Precision. RESEARCH QUARTERLY FOR EXERCISE AND SPORT 2022; 93:144-152. [PMID: 32924810 DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2020.1812494] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2020] [Accepted: 08/11/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Purpose: We investigated youth soccer players' perception of affordances for different types of kicks. Method: In the Power task, players judged the maximum distance they could kick the ball. In the Precision task, players judged how close to a designated target line they could kick the ball. Following judgments, players performed each task. Both judgments and performance were assessed immediately before and immediately after players competed in a regulation soccer match, thereby permitting us to assess possible effects of long-term experience on perceptual sensitivity to short-term changes in ability. We compared players from two league groups: U16 (mean age = 15.45 years, SD = 0.52 years) versus U18 (mean age = 17.55 years, SD = 0.52 years). Results: As expected, for the Power task actual kicking ability was greater for the U18 group (p < .05). In statistically significant interactions, we found that judgments of Power kicking ability differed before versus after match play, but only for the U16 group. We found no statistically significant effects for the Precision task. Conclusions: We identified interactions between long-term and short-term soccer experience which revealed that the effects of long-term experience on affordance perception were not general. Two additional years of playing experience (in the U18 group, relative to the U16 group) did not lead to an overall improvement in the perception of kicking-related affordances. Rather, variation in long-term experience was associated with changes in affordance perception which were situation-specific, being manifested only after playing a soccer match, and not before.
Collapse
|
10
|
Sakamoto M, Ifuku H. Experience-Dependent Modulation of Rubber Hand Illusion in Badminton Players. JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 44:14-22. [PMID: 34861653 DOI: 10.1123/jsep.2020-0178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2020] [Revised: 06/30/2021] [Accepted: 09/13/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Badminton players have a plastic modification of their arm representation in the brain due to the prolonged use of their racket. However, it is not known whether their arm representation can be altered through short-term visuotactile integration. The neural representation of the body is easily altered when multiple sensory signals are integrated in the brain. One of the most popular experimental paradigms for investigating this phenomenon is the "rubber hand illusion." This study was designed to investigate the effect of prolonged use of a racket on the modulation of arm representation during the rubber hand illusion in badminton players. When badminton players hold the racket, their badminton experience in years is negatively correlated with the magnitude of the rubber hand illusion. This finding suggests that tool embodiment obtained by the prolonged use of the badminton racket is less likely to be disturbed when holding the racket.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Masanori Sakamoto
- Department of Physical Education, Faculty of Education, Kumamoto University, Kumamoto,Japan
| | - Hirotoshi Ifuku
- Department of Physical Education, Faculty of Education, Kumamoto University, Kumamoto,Japan
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Peker AT, Erkmen N, Kocaoglu Y, Bayraktar Y, Arguz A, Wagman JB, Stoffregen TA. Perception of Affordances for Vertical and Horizontal Jumping in Children: Gymnasts Versus Non-Athletes. RESEARCH QUARTERLY FOR EXERCISE AND SPORT 2021; 92:770-778. [PMID: 32853118 DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2020.1775768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2020] [Accepted: 05/16/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Purpose: We investigated the perception of affordances for vertical jumping-and-reaching and horizontal jumping by children. Method: In the horizontal task, children were asked to judge their ability in the standing long jump. In the vertical task, children were asked to judge the height of a ball that they could run to, jump up, and reach with their fingertips. Following judgments, children performed both types of jumps. We compared gymnasts (children with at least 2 years of gymnastics training; 7.92 ± 0.91 years) versus children with no competitive athletic experience (7.74 ± 0.86 years). Results: As expected, actual ability was greater in gymnasts than in non-athletes, for both types of jump (each p < .001). We separately analyzed Constant Error and Absolute Error of judgments (relative to actual performance). Results revealed that gymnasts tended toward underestimation, while non-athletes tended toward overestimation. Absolute error differed between tasks for the non-athletes (p < .001), but for the gymnasts the difference between conditions was not significant (p = .25). Absolute error differed between groups for vertical jump-and-reach (p < .01) but not for horizonal jump (p = .17). Conclusions: Gymnastics experience was associated with a generalized tendency for children to underestimate their jumping ability. In addition, gymnastics experience was associated with judgment accuracy that was consistent across tasks. The results reveal that gymnastics training is associated with changes in athletic ability, but also with changes in the perception of affordances.
Collapse
|
12
|
The influence of skill and task complexity on perception of nested affordances. Atten Percept Psychophys 2021; 83:3240-3249. [PMID: 34414530 PMCID: PMC8550654 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-021-02355-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
This study investigated how skill level and task complexity influence the calibration of perception–action and particularly how close an individual acts relative to his or her maximal action capabilities. Complexity was manipulated between two (Touch, Grasp) and more than two (Removing, Moving Up) nested affordance conditions. For all conditions, we examined whether advanced climbers had greater maximal action capabilities than intermediate climbers or whether they better scaled their action (i.e., acted nearer to their maximal action capabilities) or both. Eleven intermediate and 11 advanced male climbers were first asked to estimate the maximum distance that they could reach a climbing hold. The hold was moved along a slide and fixed once requested by the participant; subsequently, the distance to the starting hold was measured. After each estimation, the participant was required to execute the climbing action. After four estimation-action trials in each of the four conditions, the maximal action capability (i.e., actual maximal reaching distance) was determined. Advanced climbers demonstrated greater actual maximal reaching distances than intermediate climbers for all conditions, but they only estimated greater maximal reaching distances for the more complex conditions, which featured more than two nested affordances. When estimated maximal reaching distances were scaled to actual maximal reaching distances, advanced climbers did not differ from intermediate climbers for any condition, and there were no differences between conditions. Our findings indicate that expertise was a function of greater action capabilities, but not due to the accuracy of calibration.
Collapse
|
13
|
Jones KS, Garcia NA. How Do People Perceive Other People’s Affordances, and How Might That Help Us Design Robots That Can Do So? ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/10407413.2021.1965478] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Keith S. Jones
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Texas Tech University
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
Experts' cognitive abilities adapt in response to the challenges they face in order to produce elite-level performance. Expert athletes, in particular, must integrate their motor capabilities with their cognitive and perceptual processes. Indoor rock climbers are particularly unique athletes in that much of the challenge they face is to accurately perceive and consolidate multiple movements into manageable action plans. In the current study, we investigated how climbers' level of expertise influenced their perception of action capabilities, visual memory of holds, and memory of planned and performed motor sequences. In Experiment 1, climbers judged their perceived capability to perform single climbing moves and then attempted each movement. Skilled climbers were less confident, but perceived their action capabilities more accurately than less skilled climbers. In Experiment 2, climbers recalled holds on a route, as well as predicted and recalled move sequences before and after climbing, respectively. Expertise was positively associated with visual memory performance as well as planned and recalled motor sequence accuracy. Together, these findings contribute to our knowledge of motor expertise and suggest that motor expert's ability to accurately estimate their action capabilities may underlie complex cognitive processes in their domain of expertise.
Collapse
|
15
|
Randerath J, Finkel L, Shigaki C, Burris J, Nanda A, Hwang P, Frey SH. Is This Within Reach? Left but Not Right Brain Damage Affects Affordance Judgment Tendencies. Front Hum Neurosci 2021; 14:531893. [PMID: 33584218 PMCID: PMC7873490 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.531893] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2020] [Accepted: 11/23/2020] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to judge accurately whether or not an action can be accomplished successfully is critical for selecting appropriate response options that enable adaptive behaviors. Such affordance judgments are thought to rely on the perceived fit between environmental properties and knowledge of one's current physical capabilities. Little, however, is currently known about the ability of individuals to judge their own affordances following a stroke, or about the underlying neural mechanisms involved. To address these issues, we employed a signal detection approach to investigate the impact of left or right hemisphere injuries on judgments of whether a visual object was located within reach while remaining still (i.e., reachability). Regarding perceptual sensitivity and accuracy in judging reachability, there were no significant group differences between healthy controls (N = 29), right brain damaged (RBD, N = 17) and left brain damaged stroke patients (LBD, N = 17). However, while healthy controls and RBD patients demonstrated a negative response criterion and thus overestimated their reach capability, LBD patients' average response criterion converged to zero, indicating no judgment tendency. Critically, the LBD group's judgment tendency pattern is consistent with previous findings in this same sample on an affordance judgment task that required estimating whether the hand can fit through apertures (Randerath et al., 2018). Lesion analysis suggests that this loss of judgment tendency may be associated with damage to the left insula, the left parietal and middle temporal lobe. Based on these results, we propose that damage to the left ventro-dorsal stream disrupts the retrieval and processing of a stable criterion, leading to stronger reliance on intact on-line body-perceptive processes computed within the preserved bilateral dorsal network.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Randerath
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany.,Lurija Institute for Rehabilitation Science and Health Research, Kliniken Schmieder, Allensbach, Germany.,Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, United States
| | - Lisa Finkel
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany.,Lurija Institute for Rehabilitation Science and Health Research, Kliniken Schmieder, Allensbach, Germany
| | - Cheryl Shigaki
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, United States
| | - Joe Burris
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, United States
| | - Ashish Nanda
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital, Columbia, MO, United States.,Stroke and Neurointerventional SSM Neurosciences, St. Clare Hospital, Fenton, MO, United States.,Department of Neurology, Saint Louis University Hospital, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Peter Hwang
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, United States
| | - Scott H Frey
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, United States.,Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, United States
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Lopez A, Germani A, Tinella L, Caffò AO, Postma A, Bosco A. The Road More Travelled: The Differential Effects of Spatial Experience in Young and Elderly Participants. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2021; 18:E709. [PMID: 33467572 PMCID: PMC7830856 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph18020709] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2020] [Revised: 01/07/2021] [Accepted: 01/12/2021] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
Our spatial mental representations allow us to give refined descriptions of the environment in terms of the relative locations and distances between objects and landmarks. In this study, we investigated the effects of familiarity with the everyday environment, in terms of frequency of exploration and mode of transportation, on categorical and coordinate spatial relations, on young and elderly participants, controlling for socio-demographic factors. Participants were tested with a general anamnesis, a neuropsychological assessment, measures of explorations and the Landmark Positioning on a Map task. The results showed: (a) a modest difference in performance with categorical spatial relations; (b) a larger difference in coordinate spatial relations; (c) a significant moderating effect of age on the relationship between familiarity and spatial relations, with a stronger relation among the elderly than the young. Ceteris paribus, the role of direct experience with exploring their hometown on spatial mental representations appeared to be more important in the elderly than in the young. This advantage appears to make the elderly wiser and likely protects them from the detrimental effects of aging on spatial mental representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antonella Lopez
- Department of Educational Sciences, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari, 70121 Bari, Italy; (L.T.); (A.O.C.); (A.B.)
| | - Alessandro Germani
- Department of Philosophy, Social Sciences and Education, University of Perugia, 06123 Perugia, Italy;
| | - Luigi Tinella
- Department of Educational Sciences, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari, 70121 Bari, Italy; (L.T.); (A.O.C.); (A.B.)
| | - Alessandro Oronzo Caffò
- Department of Educational Sciences, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari, 70121 Bari, Italy; (L.T.); (A.O.C.); (A.B.)
| | - Albert Postma
- Helmholtz Institute, Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands;
| | - Andrea Bosco
- Department of Educational Sciences, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari, 70121 Bari, Italy; (L.T.); (A.O.C.); (A.B.)
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Is a letterbox always a letterbox? The role of affordances in guiding perceptual categorization. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2020; 85:1673-1684. [PMID: 32279095 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-020-01328-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/12/2019] [Accepted: 03/24/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Classically investigated in the context of judgment tasks about achievable actions, affordances have also been investigated in the context of the stimulus-response compatibility paradigm. Earlier work showed that perceptual categorization performance is significantly faster and more accurate when the orientation of the graspable part of a presented object, and the orientation of the participant's response are compatible, suggesting that the main function of affordances is restricted to action preparation. Here, we investigate the potential role of affordances in the categorization of ambiguous stimuli through a stimulus-response compatibility paradigm. In other words, we investigate if in ambiguous situations, such as ones in which a stimulus may give rise to two percepts, affordances would stabilize perception on one of these two and, therefore, helps in the subsequent categorizations. Two experiments were run, based on the forced-choice stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) paradigm, with a progressive series of ambiguous (bistable) lateral-graspable objects. In Experiment 1, subjects responded by pressing horizontally opposite keyboard keys, while in Experiment 2, the keyboard keys were vertically separated. Experiment 1 found that subjects perceived the initial object in a bistable series for longer, and exhibited greater response stability in compatible than incompatible situations. In Experiment 2, none of these modulations were significant. Overall, our results show that affordances operationalized through a SRC paradigm modulated how subjects categorized ambiguous stimuli. We argue that affordances may play a substantial role in ambiguous contexts by reducing the uncertainty of such situations.
Collapse
|
18
|
|
19
|
Ji H, Pan JS. Can I Choose a Throwable Object for You? Perceiving Affordances for Other Individuals. Front Psychol 2019; 10:2205. [PMID: 31611833 PMCID: PMC6776595 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2019] [Accepted: 09/13/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Throwing is an important motor skill for human survival and societal development. It has been shown that throwers could select throwable balls for themselves and ball throwability was determined by its size and weight. In this study, we investigated whether throwers could perceive ball throwability for other throwers (experimental confederates) and whether the perceived throwability for others also followed a size-weight relation. Like other types of affordances, throwability entails a scaling between the thrower and the throwing object. This requires knowledge about the thrower and the object. In this study, knowledge about the objects was gained by hefting balls of various sizes and weights; knowledge about the throwers was gained by interacting with throwers in person (Experiment 1) and by viewing videos of confederates throwing (containing kinematic and anthropometric information) or photographs of the confederates standing (containing anthropometric information; Experiment 2). By comparing observers' perceived throwability for others using various materials, we attempted to uncover whether scaling of throwability was based on kinematic or anthropometric information. In this study, participants ranked throwability of balls of various sizes and weights for confederates of different sexes and fitness levels. In all experimental conditions, observers' ranking and confederates' actual throwing performances yielded linear relationships with slopes close to 1 and moderate to high r 2 values. These suggested that participants were able to accurately perceive throwability and choose throwable balls for the confederates. The throwable balls followed a size-weight relation, where bigger balls had to weigh more to be perceived as throwable as smaller balls. Furthermore, there was no difference between throwability perception based on in-person interaction, watching videos of confederates throwing and seeing pictures of the confederates standing. This suggested that the scaling of throwability was likely to be based on anthropometric information. These results enriched our understanding of whether one could perceive the action opportunities for others, and extended the canonical Gibsonian concept of affordance to a social setting and thus could be important for understanding team coordination in sports and interpersonal action collaboration in general.
Collapse
|
20
|
Vázquez-Guerrero J, Fernández-Valdés B, Gonçalves B, Sampaio JE. Changes in Locomotor Ratio During Basketball Game Quarters From Elite Under-18 Teams. Front Psychol 2019; 10:2163. [PMID: 31616351 PMCID: PMC6763949 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2019] [Accepted: 09/09/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Quantifying game and training demands in basketball allows to determine player’s readiness and optimizes preparation to perform and reduce injury risks. Available research is using tracking technology to perform general descriptions of the game activities at professional levels, but somehow, is not exploring the possibilities of gathering data from new variables that can contribute with complementary information for the coaching staffs. The aim of this study was to identify changes in locomotor ratio, at higher and lower speeds, during the game quarters from elite under-18 basketball teams. Ninety-four male players participated in the study (age: 17.4 ± 0.74 years; height: 199.0 ± 0.1 cm; body mass: 87.1 ± 13.1 kg) from different playing positions, Guards (n = 35), Forwards (n = 42), and Centers (n = 17). Data were gathered from an international tournament and players’ movements were measured using a portable ultra-wide band position-tracking system (WIMU PRO®, Realtrack Systems, Almeria, Spain). The following variables were measured: (1) relative distance in different speed zones: walking (<6.0 km·h−1), jogging (6.0–12.0 km·h−1), running (12.1–18.0 km·h−1), high-intensity running (18.1–24.0 km·h−1), and sprinting (>24.1 km·h−1); and (2) player load, vector magnitude expressed as the square root of the sum of the squared instantaneous rates of change in acceleration in each of the three planes divided by 100. Afterward, these variables were used to calculate players’ locomotor ratio (player load per meter covered) at higher (running, high-intensity running, and sprinting) and lower speeds (walking and jogging). Results from the locomotor ratio at both lower and higher speeds presented a significant effect for the quarter (F = 7.3, p < 0.001 and F = 7.1, p < 0.001, respectively) and player position (F = 3.1, p = 0.04, F = 9.2, p < 0.001, respectively). There was an increase in the locomotor ratio from game quarter (Q) Q1 to Q4 at lower speeds, but contrary trends at higher speeds, i.e., the values have decreased from Q1 to Q4. Also, forwards and centers of the best teams presented lower values at higher speeds. Altogether, the findings may be used by coaching staffs as a first baseline to elaborate normative behavior models from the players’ performance and also to induce variability and adaptation in specific practice planning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Bruno Gonçalves
- Research Center in Sports Sciences, Health Sciences and Human Development, CIDESD, CreativeLab Research Community, Vila Real, Portugal
| | - Jaime E Sampaio
- Research Center in Sports Sciences, Health Sciences and Human Development, CIDESD, CreativeLab Research Community, Vila Real, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Weast-Knapp JA, Shockley K, Riley MA, Cummins-Sebree S, Richardson MJ, Wirth TD, Haibach PC. Perception of another person's maximum reach-with-jump height from walking kinematics. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2019; 72:2018-2031. [PMID: 30681043 DOI: 10.1177/1747021818821935] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Humans can perceive affordances (possibilities for action) for themselves and others, including the maximum overhead height reachable by jumping (reach-with-jump height, RWJ). While observers can accurately perceive maximum RWJ for another person without previously seeing the person jump, estimates improve after viewing the person walk, suggesting there is structure in walking kinematics that is informative about the ability to produce vertical force for jumping. We used principal component analysis (PCA) to identify patterns in human walking kinematics that specify another person's maximum RWJ ability, and to determine whether athletes are more sensitive than non-athletes to these patterns. Kinematic data during treadmill walking were collected and submitted to PCA to obtain loading values for the kinematic time series variables on the first principal component. Kinematic data were also used to create point-light (PL) displays, in which the movement kinematics of PL walkers were manipulated using the obtained PCA loading values to determine how changes in body-segment movements impacted perception of maximum RWJ height. While manipulating individual segmental loadings in the PL displays did not substantially affect RWJ estimates, PL displays created by replacing the PCA loadings of a high-jumper with those of a low-jumper, and vice versa, resulted in corresponding reversals of participants' RWJ estimates, suggesting that the global structure of walking kinematics carries information about another's maximum RWJ height. Athletes exhibited greater sensitivity than controls to the kinematic manipulations, indicating that they are better attuned to useful kinematic information as a result of their sport experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie A Weast-Knapp
- 1 Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Kevin Shockley
- 1 Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Michael A Riley
- 1 Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Sarah Cummins-Sebree
- 1 Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA.,2 Behavioral Science Department, University of Cincinnati, Blue Ash, OH, USA
| | | | - Trenton D Wirth
- 1 Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA.,4 Department of Cognitive, Linguistic & Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI, USA
| | - Philip C Haibach
- 1 Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Wagman JB, Thomas BJ, McBride DM. Perceiving and Remembering Affordances for Others Are Continuous Processes. Exp Psychol 2019; 65:385-392. [PMID: 30638168 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000424] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
In information-based approaches, affordances are perceived by detecting information that specifies an animal-environment fit, not by combining perceptions of constituent lower-order properties. Given that detection of such information necessarily occurs over space and time, there is no clear distinction between perception and memory. Rather, perceiving and remembering are continuous processes. Whereas previous research has investigated the continuity of perceived and remembered affordances for the self, we did so with respect to perceived and remembered affordances for others. Participants reported remembered maximum reaching height and remembered anthropometric properties of another person. Remembered maximum reaching height was not reducible to a combination of remembered anthropometric properties. Moreover, remembered maximum reaching height scaled to the reaching ability of the other person and not to that of the perceiver. Both results are consistent with an information-based perspective on perceiving and remembering affordances and demonstrate a continuity between perceiving and remembering affordances for others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey B Wagman
- 1 Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA
| | - Brandon J Thomas
- 2 Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
| | - Dawn M McBride
- 1 Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Den Hartigh RJR, Van der Sluis JK, Zaal FTJM. Perceiving affordances in sports through a momentum lens. Hum Mov Sci 2018; 62:124-133. [PMID: 30384180 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2018.10.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2018] [Revised: 08/23/2018] [Accepted: 10/20/2018] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
In this experimental study, we tested whether athletes' judgments of affordances and of environmental features vary with psychological momentum (PM). We recruited golf, hockey, and tennis players, who were assigned to a positive or negative momentum condition. We designed a golf course on which participants made practice putts, after which they were asked to place the ball at their maximum "puttable" distance and to judge the hole size. Next, participants played a golf match against an opponent, in which the first to take a lead of 5 points would win the match. Participants were told that they could win a point by making the putt or by being closest to the hole. They wore visual occlusion goggles to prevent them from seeing the actual result, and the experimenter manipulated the scoring pattern to induce positive or negative PM. Participants in the positive momentum condition came back from a four-point lag to a four-point lead, whereas those in the negative momentum condition underwent the opposite scenario. We then asked the participants again to indicate their maximum puttable distance from the hole and to judge the hole size. After the manipulation, participants judged the maximum puttable distance to be longer in the positive momentum condition and shorter in the negative momentum condition. For the hole-size judgments, there were no significant effects. These results provide first indications for the idea that athletes' affordances change when they experience positive PM compared to negative PM. This sheds a new light on the dynamics of perception-action processes and PM in sports.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruud J R Den Hartigh
- Department of Psychology, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | - Joske K Van der Sluis
- Department of Psychology, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | - Frank T J M Zaal
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University of Groningen and University Medical Center Groningen, Antonius Deusinglaan 1, 9713 AV Groningen, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
van Opstal AAMD, Benerink NH, Zaal FTJM, Casanova R, Bootsma RJ. Information-Based Social Coordination Between Players of Different Skill in Doubles Pong. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1731. [PMID: 30283383 PMCID: PMC6156536 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01731] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2018] [Accepted: 08/27/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We studied how teams of two players of different skill level intercepted approaching balls in the doubles-pong task. In this task, the two players moved their on-screen paddles along a shared interception axis, so that the approaching ball was intercepted by one of the paddles and that the paddles did not collide. Earlier work revealed the presence of a fuzzy division of interception space, with a boundary between interception domains located in the space between the two initial paddle positions. In the present study, using the performance of the players in their individual training sessions, we formed teams of players of varying skill level. We considered two accounts of how this boundary should be understood. In a first account, the players have shared knowledge of this boundary. Based on the side of the boundary at which the approaching ball will cross the interception axis, the players would decide whose paddle is to make the interception. Under this account, we expected that a better-skilled player would take responsibility for a larger interception domain, leading to a boundary closer to the lesser-skilled player. However, our analyses did not reveal any systematic effect of skill difference on the location (or degree of fuzziness) of the boundary: location of boundaries and overlap of interception domains varied over teams but were not systematically related to skill differences between team members. We did find effects of ball speed and approach angle. In a second account, the boundary emerges from (information-driven) player–player–ball interactions. An action-based model consistent with this account was able to capture all the patterns in boundary positions and overlaps that we observed. We conclude that the interception patterns that players demonstrate in the doubles-pong task are best understood as emerging from the unfolding of the dynamics of the system of the two players and the ball, coupled through information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A A M Daphne van Opstal
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands.,Institut des Sciences du Mouvement, Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Marseille, France
| | - Niek H Benerink
- Institut des Sciences du Mouvement, Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Marseille, France
| | - Frank T J M Zaal
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
| | - Remy Casanova
- Institut des Sciences du Mouvement, Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Marseille, France
| | - Reinoud J Bootsma
- Institut des Sciences du Mouvement, Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Marseille, France
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Croft JL, Pepping GJ, Button C, Chow JY. Children's perception of action boundaries and how it affects their climbing behavior. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 166:134-146. [PMID: 28888193 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2017] [Revised: 07/17/2017] [Accepted: 07/19/2017] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
There are some concerns that children today may be less calibrated to their action capabilities because of the "risk-free" culture that has proliferated during recent decades. This study investigated the extent to which judgments of reaching affordances presented in different directions (i.e., overhead, diagonal, and horizontal) are related to children's climbing behavior on a climbing wall. A sample of 30 schoolchildren from 6 to 11years old (20 boys and 10 girls) estimated maximum reach and grasp distances and subsequently attempted to climb across an indoor climbing wall. Children who perceived the extents of their reach more accurately completed the climb more often and more quickly. Judgments in the primary directions of climbing locomotion (horizontal and diagonal) were better predictors of success than vertical judgments. Judgments about whether objects are reachable and graspable are complex and influenced by various dynamic factors (including perceptual-motor calibration), and as such different levels of accuracy are likely in different reaching directions. It appears that young children are relatively sensitive to their action boundaries for climbing and, therefore, may be able to make informed decisions themselves about whether a surface is climbable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James L Croft
- Centre of Exercise and Sports Science Research, School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, Western Australia 6027, Australia.
| | - Gert-Jan Pepping
- School of Exercise Science, Australian Catholic University, Banyo, Queensland 4014, Australia
| | - Chris Button
- School of Physical Education, Sport and Exercise Science, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
| | - Jia-Yi Chow
- Physical Education and Sports Science, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 637616, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Paulo A, Zaal FTJM, Seifert L, Fonseca S, Araújo D. Predicting volleyball serve-reception at group level. J Sports Sci 2018; 36:2621-2630. [PMID: 29745752 DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2018.1473098] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/16/2022]
Abstract
In a group-serve-reception task, how does serve-reception become effective? We addressed "who" receives/passes the ball, what task-related variables predict action mode selection and whether the action mode selected was associated with reception efficacy. In 182 serve-receptions we tracked the ball and the receivers' heads with two video-cameras to generate 3D world-coordinates reconstructions. We defined receivers' reception-areas based on Voronoi diagrams (VD). Our analyses of the data showed that this approach was accurate in describing "who" receives the serve in 95.05% of the times. To predict action mode selection, we used variables related to: serve kinematics, receiver's movement and on-court positioning, the relation between receiver and his closest partner, and interactions between receiver-ball and receiver-target. Serve's higher initial velocities together with higher maximum height, as well as smaller longitudinal distances between receiver and target increased the chances for the use of the overhand pass. Conversely, decreasing alignment of the receiver with the ball and the target increased the chances of using the underhand-lateral pass. Finally, the use of the underhand-lateral pass was associated with lower quality receptions. Behavioural variability's relevance for serve-reception training is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ana Paulo
- a CIPER, Faculdade de Motricidade Humana , Universidade de Lisboa , Cruz Quebrada , Portugal
| | - Frank T J M Zaal
- b Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen , University of Groningen , Groningen , The Netherlands
| | - Ludovic Seifert
- c Faculty of Sports Sciences, Centre d'Etude des Transformations des Activités Physiques et Sportives (CETAPS) , University of Rouen , Mont Saint Aignan , France
| | - Sofia Fonseca
- d Faculdade de Educação Física e Desporto , Universidade Lusófona de Humanidades e Tecnologias , Lisboa , Portugal
| | - Duarte Araújo
- a CIPER, Faculdade de Motricidade Humana , Universidade de Lisboa , Cruz Quebrada , Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Benerink NH, Zaal FTJM, Casanova R, Bonnardel N, Bootsma RJ. Division of labor as an emergent phenomenon of social coordination: The example of playing doubles-pong. Hum Mov Sci 2017; 57:134-148. [PMID: 29207283 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.11.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2017] [Revised: 11/25/2017] [Accepted: 11/27/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
In many daily situations, our behavior is coordinated with that of others. This study investigated this coordination in a doubles-pong task. In this task, two participants each controlled a paddle that could move laterally near the bottom of a shared computer screen. With their paddles, the players needed to block balls that moved down under an angle. In doing so, they needed to make sure that their paddles did not collide. A successful interception led to the ball bouncing back upwards. Importantly, all communication other than through vision of the shared screen was excluded. In the experiment, the initial position of the paddle of the right player was varied across trials. This allowed testing hypotheses regarding the use of a tacitly understood boundary to divide interception space. This boundary could be halfway the screen, or in the middle between the initial positions of the two paddles. These two hypotheses did not hold. As an alternative to planned division of labor, the behavioral patterns might emerge from continuous visual couplings of paddles and ball. This was tested with an action-based decision model that considered the rates of change of each player's angle between the interception axis and the line connecting the ball and inner edge of the paddle. The model accounted for the observed patterns of behavior to a very large extent. This led to the conclusion that decisions of who would take the ball emerged from ongoing social coordination. Implications for social coordination in general are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niek H Benerink
- Aix-Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Instit Movement Sci, Marseille, France; Aix-Marseille Univ, PsyCLE, Aix-en-Provence, France
| | - Frank T J M Zaal
- University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Center for Human Movement Sciences, The Netherlands
| | - Remy Casanova
- Aix-Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Instit Movement Sci, Marseille, France
| | | | - Reinoud J Bootsma
- Aix-Marseille Univ, CNRS, ISM, Instit Movement Sci, Marseille, France.
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Thomas BJ, Hawkins MM, Nalepka P. Perceiver as polar planimeter: Direct perception of jumping, reaching, and jump-reaching affordances for the self and others. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2017; 82:665-674. [PMID: 28361471 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-017-0858-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2017] [Accepted: 03/16/2017] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Brandon J Thomas
- Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 380 S 1530 E Beh S 502, Salt Lake City, UT, 84112, USA.
| | - Matthew M Hawkins
- Department of Psychology, Center for Cognition, Action, and Perception, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, USA
| | - Patrick Nalepka
- Department of Psychology, Center for Cognition, Action, and Perception, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Wagman JB, Stoffregen TA, Bai J, Schloesser DS. Perceiving nested affordances for another person's actions. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2017; 71:790-799. [PMID: 28056648 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1277249] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Affordances are available behaviors that emerge out of relations between properties of animals and properties of their environment. Affordances are nested within one another. One way to conceptualize this nesting is through a mean-ends hierarchy. Previous research has shown that perceivers are sensitive to hierarchical means-ends relationships when perceiving affordances for their own actions. Affordances are also nested in a social context. We investigated perception of hierarchical mean-ends nesting of affordances for another person's actions. We asked participants to judge the maximum reaching height of another person (the "actor"). Judgments of the actor's maximum reaching height reflected manipulated constraints on the reaching task, suggesting that participants were sensitive (prospectively) to hierarchical relations between lower order affordances and higher order affordances. In addition, the results revealed that judgments scaled to the reaching ability of the actor and not that of the perceiver. We argue that perceivers were sensitive to hierarchical means-ends nesting of affordances for another person across two-levels of this hierarchy, and that perceivers' judgments were based upon perceptual information about the actor's action capabilities, rather than being based upon simulation of perceivers' own abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey B Wagman
- 1 Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA
| | | | - Jiuyang Bai
- 1 Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Thomas BJ, Wagman JB, Hawkins M, Havens M, Riley MA. The Independent Perceptual Calibration of Action-Neutral and -Referential Environmental Properties. Perception 2016; 46:586-604. [PMID: 27864556 DOI: 10.1177/0301006616679172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Two experiments were conducted to explore how the calibration of perception of environmental properties taken with reference to an animal and their action capabilities (e.g., affordances) and those that are independent of action capabilities (e.g., metric properties) relate. In both experiments, participants provided reports of the maximum height they could reach above their head with a number of different stick(s) (reach-with-stick height) and the length of those stick(s), a property that is a constituent of reach-with-stick height. In Experiment 1 reach-with-stick height reports improved over trials whereas stick length reports remained constant. In Experiment 2, feedback about maximum reach-with-stick height improved perception of this affordance, but such improvements did not transfer to perception of stick length in a pretest/practice task/posttest design. The results suggest that the perceptual calibration with practice perceiving or feedback about actual dimensions of action-referential and action-neutral properties do not necessarily depend on one another.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Matthew Hawkins
- Department of Psychology, Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, University of Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Mark Havens
- Department of Psychology, Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, University of Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Michael A Riley
- Department of Psychology, Center for Cognition, Action, & Perception, University of Cincinnati, OH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Linkenauger SA. The Relative Nature of Perception: A Response to Cañal-Bruland and van der Kamp (2015). Iperception 2016; 6:2041669515599898. [PMID: 27648215 PMCID: PMC5016820 DOI: 10.1177/2041669515599898] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Cañal-Bruland and van der Kamp present an argument about the incommensurate relationship between affordance perception and spatial perception in a criticism of Proffitt and Linkenauger’s phenotypic approach to perception. Many of their criticisms are based on a difference in the interpretation of the core ideas underlying the phenotypic approach. The most important of these differences in interpretations concern fundamental assumptions about the nature of the perceptions of size and distance themselves. Extent perception must be relative to the organism; therefore, there can be no veridical perception of space. Also, we argue in the phenotypic approach that space perception is an emergent property of affordance perception; they are not different types of perceptions as Cañal-Bruland and van der Kamp presume. Third, affordance perception need not be perfectly accurate, just good enough. Additionally, affordance perception need not be dichotomous; this presumption likely originates in the methodology typically employed to study affordance perception. Finally, I agree with Cañal-Bruland and van der Kamp that joint research efforts will clarify and improve our understanding of these issues.
Collapse
|
32
|
Randerath J, Frey SH. Diagnostics and Training of Affordance Perception in Healthy Young Adults-Implications for Post-Stroke Neurorehabilitation. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 9:674. [PMID: 26778995 PMCID: PMC4701931 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00674] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2015] [Accepted: 11/30/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Affordance perception is critical to adaptive behavior. It comprises the ability to evaluate whether the environment and the actor's capabilities enable particular actions. It remains unclear how brain damage and its behavioral sequela impact this ability. Two affordance based judgment tasks were applied in healthy young adults that were adapted for prospective diagnostic purposes in patients. In addition to the commonly analyzed error-rate we included response times and accuracy measures based on a detection theory approach. Moreover, a manipulation was added intended to determine the effectiveness of feedback-based learning. We further applied control tasks that consider whether errors in affordance perception can be explained by errors in perception. Participants responded yes or no to decide prospectively if a given setting would afford a particular action. In study1, 27 participants judged whether their hand would fit through a given aperture (adapted from Ishak et al., 2008). In study2, 19 participants judged whether objects are reachable [adapted from Gabbard et al. (2005)]. For both studies two sessions were administered. In the first session all participants solved the judgment-task without executing the action. In the second session (feedback manipulation), half of the participants were allowed to first judge and then perform the task for each trial (reach forward and touch the object, or fitting the hand into the aperture). Judgments were slowest and errors most frequent for openings or distances close to the individual's actual physical limits. With more extreme settings accuracy increased and responses became faster. Importantly, we found an advantageous effect of feedback on performance in both tasks suggesting that affordance perception is rapidly trainable. Further, the aperture task demonstrated that feedback experienced with one hand can transfer to the other. This may have important implications for rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Randerath
- Department of Psychology, University of KonstanzKonstanz, Germany
- Lurija Institute for Rehabilitation Science and Health Research, Kliniken SchmiederAllensbach, Germany
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of MissouriColumbia, MO, USA
| | - Scott H. Frey
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of MissouriColumbia, MO, USA
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Department of Neurology and Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University School of MedicineSt. Louis, MO, USA
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Sampaio J, McGarry T, Calleja-González J, Jiménez Sáiz S, Schelling i del Alcázar X, Balciunas M. Exploring Game Performance in the National Basketball Association Using Player Tracking Data. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0132894. [PMID: 26171606 PMCID: PMC4501835 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132894] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2015] [Accepted: 06/22/2015] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent player tracking technology provides new information about basketball game performance. The aim of this study was to (i) compare the game performances of all-star and non all-star basketball players from the National Basketball Association (NBA), and (ii) describe the different basketball game performance profiles based on the different game roles. Archival data were obtained from all 2013-2014 regular season games (n = 1230). The variables analyzed included the points per game, minutes played and the game actions recorded by the player tracking system. To accomplish the first aim, the performance per minute of play was analyzed using a descriptive discriminant analysis to identify which variables best predict the all-star and non all-star playing categories. The all-star players showed slower velocities in defense and performed better in elbow touches, defensive rebounds, close touches, close points and pull-up points, possibly due to optimized attention processes that are key for perceiving the required appropriate environmental information. The second aim was addressed using a k-means cluster analysis, with the aim of creating maximal different performance profile groupings. Afterwards, a descriptive discriminant analysis identified which variables best predict the different playing clusters. The results identified different playing profile of performers, particularly related to the game roles of scoring, passing, defensive and all-round game behavior. Coaching staffs may apply this information to different players, while accounting for individual differences and functional variability, to optimize practice planning and, consequently, the game performances of individuals and teams.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaime Sampaio
- Research Center in Sports Sciences, Health and Human Development, CIDESD, CreativeLab Research Community, University of Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, Vila Real, Portugal
- * E-mail:
| | - Tim McGarry
- Faculty of Kinesiology, University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, Canada
| | - Julio Calleja-González
- Faculty of Physical Activity Sport Sciences, University of the Basque Country, Vitoria, Spain
| | - Sergio Jiménez Sáiz
- Facultad de Ciencias de la Actividad Física y el Deporte, Universidad Europea de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Xavi Schelling i del Alcázar
- Complex Systems and Sport Research Group, National Institute of Physical Education of Catalonia (INEFC), Barcelona, Spain
| | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Day BM, Wagman JB, Smith PJK. Perception of maximum stepping and leaping distance: Stepping affordances as a special case of leaping affordances. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 158:26-35. [PMID: 25898112 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.03.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2014] [Revised: 01/23/2015] [Accepted: 03/27/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Successfully performing everyday behaviors requires perceiving affordances-possibilities for behavior that depend on the fit between environmental properties and action capabilities. Whereas affordances for some behaviors are primarily constrained by relatively static geometric properties of the perceiver (non-launching behaviors such as stepping), others are additionally constrained by dynamic force production capabilities of the perceiver (launching behaviors such as leaping). This experiment used a transfer of calibration paradigm to investigate whether visual perception of launching and non-launching behaviors represent independent perception-action tasks. In particular, we investigated whether calibration of visual perception of maximum leaping distance transferred to visual perception of maximum stepping distance, and/or vice versa. The results showed that calibration of perception of maximum leaping distance transferred to perception of maximum stepping distance, suggesting that perception of launching and non-launching are not independent. Rather, perception of stepping affordances may be a special case of perception of leaping affordances.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brian M Day
- Department of Psychology, Clemson University, Clemson, IL, USA
| | - Jeffrey B Wagman
- Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA.
| | - Peter J K Smith
- School of Kinesiology and Recreation, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Daviaux Y, Cremoux S, Tallet J, Amarantini D, Cornu C, Deschamps T. An enhanced experimental procedure to rationalize on the impairment of perception of action capabilities. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2015; 80:224-34. [PMID: 25702038 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-015-0653-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2015] [Accepted: 02/13/2015] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
It is well documented that changes in the physiological states of the perceiver-actor influence the perception of action capabilities. However, because experimental procedures of most studies involved a limitless availability for stimuli visual encoding and perceptual strategies, it remains difficult to adopt a single position among the large range of alternative interpretations for impaired perception. A reaching-to-grasp paradigm under breathing restriction was adapted from Graydon et al. (Cogn Emot 26:1301-1305, 2012) to standardize the time for encoding of stimuli information and narrowed the involvement of perceptual strategies. In the present study, we propose a highly controlled environment where the discrete information is presented during 300 ms, congruently with neurophysiological studies focused on visuomotor transformation. An underestimation of the perception of action capabilities is found under breath restriction, suggesting that 300 ms for stimuli encoding is sufficient to induce altered visuomotor brain transformations when limiting the involvement of perceptual strategies. This result suggests that such behavior could refer to an impaired brain potentiation of the perceptual occurrence, providing strong hypotheses on the brain dynamics of sensorimotor integration that underlie impaired perception of action capabilities in stressful situations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yannick Daviaux
- Laboratory "Motricité, Interactions, Performance" (UPRES EA 4334), University of Nantes, 25 bis boulevard Guy Mollet, BP 72206, 44322, Nantes Cedex 3, France.
| | - Sylvain Cremoux
- Laboratory "PRISSMH" (EA 4651), University of Paul Sabatier Toulouse 3, 31062, Toulouse, France.,LAMIH, UMR CNRS 8201, Université de Valenciennes et du Hainaut-Cambrésis, Valenciennes, France
| | - Jessica Tallet
- Laboratory "PRISSMH" (EA 4651), University of Paul Sabatier Toulouse 3, 31062, Toulouse, France
| | - David Amarantini
- Inserm, Imagerie cérébrale et handicaps neurologiques UMR 825, Toulouse, France.,UPS, Imagerie cérébrale et handicaps neurologiques UMR 825, Université de Toulouse, Toulouse, France
| | - Christophe Cornu
- Laboratory "Motricité, Interactions, Performance" (UPRES EA 4334), University of Nantes, 25 bis boulevard Guy Mollet, BP 72206, 44322, Nantes Cedex 3, France
| | - Thibault Deschamps
- Laboratory "Motricité, Interactions, Performance" (UPRES EA 4334), University of Nantes, 25 bis boulevard Guy Mollet, BP 72206, 44322, Nantes Cedex 3, France.
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Meerhoff LRA, De Poel HJ. Asymmetric interpersonal coupling in a cyclic sports-related movement task. Hum Mov Sci 2014; 35:66-79. [PMID: 24835161 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2013] [Revised: 03/27/2014] [Accepted: 04/07/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
In interactive sports, teammates and/or opponents mutually tune their behavior. Expert performance thus implies certain interactive abilities, which critically depend on perceptual coupling. To illustrate this assertion, we examined the coordination dynamics with asymmetric interaction of dyads performing a sports-related cyclical movement task. In pairs, basketball players performed lateral defensive slides in in-phase, until a cue prompted them to switch to antiphase coordination. We assessed how these switches were mediated by phase adaptations of each agent under bidirectional (i.e., agents facing one another) and unidirectional (i.e., one agent facing the back of the other) visual interaction conditions. This imposed asymmetry in visual coupling exemplified an imbalance in the interaction (or 'interact-ability') between two agents. The results concurred the asymmetric coupling: during the switch the agent facing the other adapted his phasing more than the other agent. Furthermore, also in the bidirectional condition the coupling revealed dyad-intrinsic asymmetries (e.g., related to implicit follower-leader strategies). Together, this illustrates that interpersonal coordination is characterized by asymmetric coupling between the agents, and highlights how mutual perception of pertinent information mediates interpersonal coordination. This study offered a first step towards analyzing interpersonal coordination dynamics in relation to 'interact-ability'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Rens A Meerhoff
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, P.O. Box 196, 9700 AD Groningen, The Netherlands; School of Physical Education, Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Otago, P.O. Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand.
| | - Harjo J De Poel
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, P.O. Box 196, 9700 AD Groningen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Daviaux Y, Mignardot JB, Cornu C, Deschamps T. Effects of total sleep deprivation on the perception of action capabilities. Exp Brain Res 2014; 232:2243-53. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-014-3915-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2013] [Accepted: 03/11/2014] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
38
|
Wagman JB, Day BM. Changes in Context and Perception of Maximum Reaching Height. Perception 2014; 43:129-44. [PMID: 24919349 DOI: 10.1068/p7620] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Successfully performing a given behavior requires flexibility in both perception and behavior. In particular, doing so requires perceiving whether that behavior is possible across the variety of contexts in which it might be performed. Three experiments investigated how (changes in) context (ie point of observation and intended reaching task) influenced perception of maximum reaching height. The results of experiment 1 showed that perceived maximum reaching height more closely reflected actual reaching ability when perceivers occupied a point of observation that was compatible with that required for the reaching task. The results of experiments 2 and 3 showed that practice perceiving maximum reaching height from a given point of observation improved perception of maximum reaching height from a different point of observation, regardless of whether such practice occurred at a compatible or incompatible point of observation. In general, such findings show bounded flexibility in perception of affordances and are thus consistent with a description of perceptual systems as smart perceptual devices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey B Wagman
- Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Campus Box 4620, Normal, IL 61790-4620, USA
| | - Brian M Day
- Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Campus Box 4620, Normal, IL 61790-4620, USA
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Essential kinematic information, athletic experience, and affordance perception for others. Psychon Bull Rev 2013; 21:823-9. [DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0539-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
40
|
Creem-Regehr SH, Gagnon KT, Geuss MN, Stefanucci JK. Relating spatial perspective taking to the perception of other's affordances: providing a foundation for predicting the future behavior of others. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:596. [PMID: 24068992 PMCID: PMC3781345 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00596] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2013] [Accepted: 09/03/2013] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding what another agent can see relates functionally to the understanding of what they can do. We propose that spatial perspective taking and perceiving other's affordances, while two separate spatial processes, together share the common social function of predicting the behavior of others. Perceiving the action capabilities of others allows for a common understanding of how agents may act together. The ability to take another's perspective focuses an understanding of action goals so that more precise understanding of intentions may result. This review presents an analysis of these complementary abilities, both in terms of the frames of reference and the proposed sensorimotor mechanisms involved. Together, we argue for the importance of reconsidering the role of basic spatial processes to explain more complex behaviors.
Collapse
|
41
|
Perceiving children's affordances: recalibrating estimation following single-trial observation of three different tasks. Hum Mov Sci 2013; 32:270-8. [PMID: 23482301 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2013.01.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2012] [Revised: 01/03/2013] [Accepted: 01/03/2013] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The adults' ability to perceive affordances for children was investigated in three different tasks. Forty adults made two estimations of the maximum reachability of a 5-year-old boy from a standing position, during a reach-and-jump task and in making a maximum step. A laser light point was displayed on a wall for the estimations of the standing reach and reach-and-jump tasks, or on the floor for the estimations of the step length task. The participants in the experimental group observed the child performing the task between a first and a second estimation, but the participants in the control group did not. In general, the observers were less accurate in estimating the child's maximum step length than in the other tasks. The observation of a single trial was enough to adjust perceivers' estimations, reducing error magnitude to about 50% of the initial error, but only in tasks with a poor first estimation. An absolute error of 5 cm persisted after one-trial observation. The magnitude of the adjustment in the estimation of affordances for others is task-dependent, and is more pronounced in tasks that imply greater action scaling than in tasks that require direct body scaling.
Collapse
|
42
|
Affiliation(s)
- Ludovic Seifert
- Centre d'Etude des Transformations des Activités Physiques et Sportives (CETAPS)-EA 3832, Faculty of Sport Sciences, University of Rouen, Bld Siegfried, 76821 Mount Saint Aignan Cedex, Mont Saint Aignan, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Correia V, Araújo D, Vilar L, Davids K. From recording discrete actions to studying continuous goal-directed behaviours in team sports. J Sports Sci 2012; 31:546-53. [PMID: 23140581 DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2012.738926] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
This paper highlights the importance of examining interpersonal interactions in performance analysis of team sports, predicated on the relationship between perception and action, compared to the traditional cataloguing of actions by individual performers. We discuss how ecological dynamics may provide a potential unifying theoretical and empirical framework to achieve this re-emphasis in research. With reference to data from illustrative studies on performance analysis and sport expertise, we critically evaluate some of the main assumptions and methodological approaches with regard to understanding how information influences action and decision-making during team sports performance. Current data demonstrate how the understanding of performance behaviours in team sports by sport scientists and practitioners may be enhanced with a re-emphasis in research on the dynamics of emergent ongoing interactions. Ecological dynamics provides formal and theoretically grounded descriptions of player-environment interactions with respect to key performance goals and the unfolding information of competitive performance. Developing these formal descriptions and explanations of sport performance may provide a significant contribution to the field of performance analysis, supporting design and intervention in both research and practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vanda Correia
- School of Education and Communication, University of Algarve, Faro, Portugal.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Franchak JM, Celano EC, Adolph KE. Perception of passage through openings depends on the size of the body in motion. Exp Brain Res 2012; 223:301-10. [PMID: 22990292 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3261-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2012] [Accepted: 09/03/2012] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Walkers need to modify their ongoing actions to meet the demands of everyday environments. Navigating through openings requires gait modifications if the size of the opening is too small relative to the body. Here we ask whether the spatial requirements for navigating horizontal and vertical openings differ, and, if so, whether walkers are sensitive to those requirements. To test walkers' sensitivity to demands for gait modification, we asked participants to judge whether they could walk through horizontal openings without shoulder rotation and through vertical openings without ducking. Afterward, participants walked through the openings, so that we could determine which opening sizes elicited gait modifications. Participants turned their shoulders with more space available than the space they left themselves for ducking. Larger buffers for horizontal openings may reflect different spatial requirements created by lateral sway of the body during walking compared to vertical bounce. In addition, greater variability of turning from trial to trial compared with ducking may lead walkers to adopt a more conservative buffer to avoid errors. Verbal judgments accurately predicted whether openings required gait modifications. For horizontal openings, participants' judgments were best predicted by the body's dynamic abilities, not static shoulder width. The differences between horizontal and vertical openings illustrate that walkers account for the dynamic properties of walking in addition to scaling decisions to body dimensions.
Collapse
|
45
|
Attunement to haptic information helps skilled performers select implements for striking a ball in cricket. Atten Percept Psychophys 2012; 74:1782-91. [DOI: 10.3758/s13414-012-0368-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
46
|
Orth D, Davids K, Araújo D, Renshaw I, Passos P. Effects of a defender on run-up velocity and ball speed when crossing a football. Eur J Sport Sci 2012; 14 Suppl 1:S316-23. [PMID: 24444224 DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2012.696712] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
This study evaluated effects of defensive pressure on running velocity in footballers during the approach to kick a stationary football. Approach velocity and ball speed/accuracy data were recorded from eight football youth academy participants (15.25, SD=0.46 yrs). Participants were required to run to a football to cross it to a receiver to score against a goal-keeper. Defensive pressure was manipulated across three counterbalanced conditions: defender-absent (DA); defender-far (DF) and defender-near (DN). Pass accuracy (percentages of a total of 32 trials with 95% confidence limits in parenthesis) did not significantly reduce under changing defensive pressure: DA, 78% (55-100%); DF, 78% (61-96%); DN, 59% (40-79%). Ball speed (m · s(-1)) significantly reduced as defensive pressure was included and increased: DA, 23.10 (22.38-23.83); DF, 20.40 (19.69-21.11); DN, 19.22 (18.51-19.93). When defensive pressure was introduced, average running velocity of attackers did not change significantly: DA versus DF (m · s(-1)), 5.40 (5.30-5.51) versus 5.41 (5.34-5.48). Scaling defender starting positions closer to the start position of the attacker (DN) significantly increased average running velocity relative to the DA and DF conditions, 5.60 (5.50-5.71). In the final approach footfalls, all conditions significantly differed: DA, 5.69 (5.35-6.03); DF, 6 .22 (5.93-6.50); DN, 6.52 (6.23-6.80). Data suggested that approach velocity is constrained by both presence and initial distance of the defender during task performance. Implications are that the expression of kicking behaviour is specific to a performance context and some movement regulation features will not emerge unless a defender is present as a task constraint in practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dominic Orth
- a School of Human Movement Studies , Queensland University of Technology , Brisbane , Australia
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Wagman JB. Perception of maximum reaching height reflects impending changes in reaching ability and improvements transfer to unpracticed reaching tasks. Exp Brain Res 2012; 219:467-76. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3104-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/29/2012] [Accepted: 04/17/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
48
|
Suzuki M, Kirimoto H, Inamura A, Yagi M, Omori Y, Yamada S. The relationship between knee extension strength and lower extremity functions in nursing home residents with dementia. Disabil Rehabil 2011; 34:202-9. [DOI: 10.3109/09638288.2011.593678] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
|