1
|
Yates TS, Sherman BE, Yousif SR. More than a moment: What does it mean to call something an 'event'? Psychon Bull Rev 2023; 30:2067-2082. [PMID: 37407794 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02311-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/14/2023] [Indexed: 07/07/2023]
Abstract
Experiences are stored in the mind as discrete mental units, or 'events,' which influence-and are influenced by-attention, learning, and memory. In this way, the notion of an 'event' is foundational to cognitive science. However, despite tremendous progress in understanding the behavioral and neural signatures of events, there is no agreed-upon definition of an event. Here, we discuss different theoretical frameworks of event perception and memory, noting what they can and cannot account for in the literature. We then highlight key aspects of events that we believe should be accounted for in theories of event processing--in particular, we argue that the structure and substance of events should be better reflected in our theories and paradigms. Finally, we discuss empirical gaps in the event cognition literature and what the future of event cognition research may look like.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tristan S Yates
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA.
| | - Brynn E Sherman
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
| | - Sami R Yousif
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Ongchoco JDK, Walter-Terrill R, Scholl BJ. Visual event boundaries restrict anchoring effects in decision-making. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2023; 120:e2303883120. [PMID: 37874857 PMCID: PMC10623015 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2303883120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2023] [Accepted: 06/29/2023] [Indexed: 10/26/2023] Open
Abstract
Research on higher-level thought has revealed many principles of reasoning and decision-making but has rarely made contact with how we perceive the world in the first place. Here we show how a lower-level property of perception-the spontaneous and task-irrelevant segmentation of continuous visual stimulation into discrete events-can restrict one of the most notorious biases in decision-making: numerical anchoring. Subjects walked down a long room in an immersive three dimensional (3D) animation and then made a numerical judgment (e.g., of how much a suitcase is worth, or of how many hours of community service a minor crime deserved). Critically, some subjects passed through a doorway (a visual event boundary) during their virtual walk, while others did not-equating time, distance traveled, and visual complexity. The anchoring manipulation was especially innocuous, not appearing to be part of the experiment at all. Before the online trial began, subjects reported the two-digit numerical value from a visually distorted "CAPTCHA" ("to verify that you are human")-where this task-irrelevant anchor was either low (e.g., 29) or high (e.g., 92). With no doorway, we observed reliable anchoring effects: Higher CAPTCHA values produced higher estimates. With the doorway, however, such effects were attenuated or even eliminated. This generalized across tasks involving item valuations, factual questions, and legal judgments and in tests of both incidental and explicit anchoring. This demonstrates how spontaneous visual event segmentation can have profound consequences for higher-level thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Brian J. Scholl
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT06520-8047
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bower IS, Clark GM, Tucker R, Hill AT, Lum JAG, Mortimer MA, Enticott PG. Built environment color modulates autonomic and EEG indices of emotional response. Psychophysiology 2022; 59:e14121. [PMID: 35723272 PMCID: PMC9786701 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2021] [Revised: 02/25/2022] [Accepted: 05/19/2022] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Understanding built environment exposure as a component of environmental enrichment has significant implications for mental health, but little is known about the effects design characteristics have on our emotions and associated neurophysiology. Using a Cave Automatic Virtual Environment while monitoring indoor environmental quality (IEQ), 18 participants were exposed to a resting state (black), and two room scenes, control (white) and condition (blue), to understand if the color of the virtual walls affected self-report, autonomic nervous system, and central nervous system correlates of emotion. Our findings showed that exposure to the chromatic color condition (blue) compared to the achromatic control (white) and resting-state (black, no built environment) significantly increased the range in respiration and skin conductance response. We also detected a significant increase in alpha frontal midline power and frontal hemispheric lateralization relative to blue condition, and increased power spectral density across all electrodes in the blue condition for theta, alpha, and beta bandwidths. The ability for built environment design to modulate emotional response has the potential to deliver significant public health, economic, and social benefits to the entire community. The findings show that blue coloring of the built environment increases autonomic range and is associated with modulations of brain activity linked to emotional processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Isabella S. Bower
- Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Faculty of HealthDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia,School of Architecture and Built Environment, Faculty of Science, Engineering and Built EnvironmentDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia
| | - Gillian M. Clark
- Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Faculty of HealthDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia
| | - Richard Tucker
- School of Architecture and Built Environment, Faculty of Science, Engineering and Built EnvironmentDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia
| | - Aron T. Hill
- Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Faculty of HealthDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia
| | - Jarrad A. G. Lum
- Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Faculty of HealthDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia
| | - Michael A. Mortimer
- CADET Virtual Reality Training and Simulation Research Lab, School of Engineering, Faculty of Science, Engineering and Built EnvironmentDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia
| | - Peter G. Enticott
- Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Faculty of HealthDeakin UniversityGeelongVictoriaAustralia
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Cohn-Sheehy BI, Delarazan AI, Crivelli-Decker JE, Reagh ZM, Mundada NS, Yonelinas AP, Zacks JM, Ranganath C. Narratives bridge the divide between distant events in episodic memory. Mem Cognit 2022; 50:478-494. [PMID: 33904017 PMCID: PMC8546012 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-021-01178-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/31/2021] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Many studies suggest that information about past experience, or episodic memory, is divided into discrete units called "events." Yet we can often remember experiences that span multiple events. Events that occur in close succession might simply be linked because of their proximity to one another, but we can also build links between events that occur farther apart in time. Intuitively, some kind of organizing principle should enable temporally distant events to become bridged in memory. We tested the hypothesis that episodic memory exhibits a narrative-level organization, enabling temporally distant events to be better remembered if they form a coherent narrative. Furthermore, we tested whether post-encoding memory consolidation is necessary to integrate temporally distant events. In three experiments, participants learned and subsequently recalled events from fictional stories, in which pairs of temporally distant events involving side characters ("sideplots") either formed one coherent narrative or two unrelated narratives. Across participants, we varied whether recall was assessed immediately after learning, or after a delay: 24 hours, 12 hours between morning and evening ("wake"), or 12 hours between evening and morning ("sleep"). Participants recalled more information about coherent than unrelated narrative events, in most delay conditions, including immediate recall and wake conditions, suggesting that post-encoding consolidation was not necessary to integrate temporally distant events into a larger narrative. Furthermore, post hoc modeling across experiments suggested that narrative coherence facilitated recall over and above any effects of sentence-level semantic similarity. This reliable memory benefit for coherent narrative events supports theoretical accounts which propose that narratives provide a high-level architecture for episodic memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brendan I Cohn-Sheehy
- M.D./Ph.D. Program, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA.
- Neuroscience Graduate Group, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA.
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA.
| | - Angelique I Delarazan
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University, 1 Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Jordan E Crivelli-Decker
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Zachariah M Reagh
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University, 1 Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Nidhi S Mundada
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Andrew P Yonelinas
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Jeffrey M Zacks
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University, 1 Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Charan Ranganath
- Neuroscience Graduate Group, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Jafarpour A, Buffalo EA, Knight RT, Collins AG. Event segmentation reveals working memory forgetting rate. iScience 2022; 25:103902. [PMID: 35252809 PMCID: PMC8891967 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.103902] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2020] [Revised: 07/30/2021] [Accepted: 02/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We encounter the world as a continuous flow and effortlessly segment sequences of events into episodes. This process of event segmentation engages working memory (WM) for tracking the flow of events and impacts subsequent memory accuracy. WM is limited in how much information (i.e., WM capacity) and for how long the information is retained (i.e., forgetting rate). In this study, across multiple tasks, we estimated participants’ WM capacity and forgetting rate in a dynamic context and evaluated their relationship to event segmentation. A U-shaped relationship across tasks shows that individuals who segmented the movie more finely or coarsely than the average have a faster WM forgetting rate. A separate task assessing long-term memory retrieval revealed that the coarse-segmenters have better recognition of temporal order of events compared to the fine-segmenters. These findings show that event segmentation employs dissociable memory strategies and correlates with how long information is retained in WM The event segmentation grain is variable across individuals The event segmentation grain has a U-shaped relationship with the WM forgetting rate The temporal order memory accuracy decreases with the increasing event segmentation The number of recalled events increases with the increasing event segmentation
Collapse
|
6
|
Semantic knowledge attenuates age-related differences in event segmentation and episodic memory. Mem Cognit 2021; 50:586-600. [PMID: 34553341 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-021-01220-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/24/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
While semantic and episodic memory may be distinct memory systems, their interdependence is substantial. For instance, decades of work have shown that semantic knowledge facilitates episodic memory. Here, we aim to clarify this interactive relationship by determining whether semantic knowledge facilitates the acquisition of new episodic memories, in part, by influencing an encoding mechanism, event segmentation. In the current study, we evaluated the extent to which semantic knowledge shapes how people segment ongoing activity and how such knowledge-related benefits in segmentation affect episodic memory performance. To investigate these effects, we combined data across three studies that had young and older adults segment and remember videos of everyday activities that were either familiar or unfamiliar to their age group. We found age-related differences in event-segmentation ability and memory performance, but only when older adults lacked semantic knowledge. Most importantly, when they had access to relevant semantic knowledge, older adults segmented and remembered information similar to young adults. Our findings indicate that older adults can use semantic knowledge to effectively encode and retrieve everyday information. These effects suggest that future interventions can leverage older adults' intact semantic knowledge to attenuate age-related deficits in event segmentation and episodic long-term memory.
Collapse
|
7
|
McFadyen J, Nolan C, Pinocy E, Buteri D, Baumann O. Doorways do not always cause forgetting: a multimodal investigation. BMC Psychol 2021; 9:41. [PMID: 33685514 PMCID: PMC7938580 DOI: 10.1186/s40359-021-00536-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2020] [Accepted: 02/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The 'doorway effect', or 'location updating effect', claims that we tend to forget items of recent significance immediately after crossing a boundary. Previous research suggests that such a forgetting effect occurs both at physical boundaries (e.g., moving from one room to another via a door) and metaphysical boundaries (e.g., imagining traversing a doorway, or even when moving from one desktop window to another on a computer). Here, we aimed to conceptually replicate this effect using virtual and physical environments. METHODS Across four experiments, we measured participants' hit and false alarm rates to memory probes for items recently encountered either in the same or previous room. Experiments 1 and 2 used highly immersive virtual reality without and with working memory load (Experiments 1 and 2, respectively). Experiment 3 used passive video watching and Experiment 4 used active real-life movement. Data analysis was conducted using frequentist as well as Bayesian inference statistics. RESULTS Across this series of experiments, we observed no significant effect of doorways on forgetting. In Experiment 2, however, signal detection was impaired when participants responded to probes after moving through doorways, such that false alarm rates were increased for mismatched recognition probes. Thus, under working memory load, memory was more susceptible to interference after moving through doorways. CONCLUSIONS This study presents evidence that is inconsistent with the location updating effect as it has previously been reported. Our findings call into question the generalisability and robustness of this effect to slight paradigm alterations and, indeed, what factors contributed to the effect observed in previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica McFadyen
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia.,Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, University College London, London, UK
| | - Christopher Nolan
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia
| | - Ellen Pinocy
- School of Psychology and Interdisciplinary Centre for the Artificial Mind, Bond University, Robina, QLD, 4226, Australia
| | - David Buteri
- School of Psychology and Interdisciplinary Centre for the Artificial Mind, Bond University, Robina, QLD, 4226, Australia
| | - Oliver Baumann
- School of Psychology and Interdisciplinary Centre for the Artificial Mind, Bond University, Robina, QLD, 4226, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Access to prior spatial information. Mem Cognit 2020; 48:1234-1248. [DOI: 10.3758/s13421-019-00996-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
9
|
For humans navigating without vision, navigation depends upon the layout of mechanically contacted ground surfaces. Exp Brain Res 2020; 238:917-930. [PMID: 32172353 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05767-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2019] [Accepted: 03/03/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Navigation can be haptically guided. In specific, tissue deformations arising from both limb motions during locomotion (i.e., gait patterns) and mechanical interactions between the limbs and the environment can convey information, detected by the haptic perceptual system, about how the body is moving relative to the environment. Here, we test hypotheses concerning the properties of mechanically contacted environments relevant to navigation of this kind. We studied blindfolded participants implicitly learning to perceive their location within environments that were physically encountered via walking on, stepping on, and probing ground surfaces with a cane. Environments were straight-line paths with elevated sections where the path either narrowed or remained the same width. We formed hypotheses concerning how these two environments would affect spatial updating and reorientation processes. In the constant pathwidth environment, homing task accuracy was higher and a manipulation of the elevated surface, to be either unchanged or (unbeknown to participants) shortened, biased the performance. This was consistent with our hypothesis of a metric recalibration scaled to elevated surface extent. In the narrowing pathwidth environment, elevated surface shortening did not bias performance. This supported our hypothesis of positional recalibration resulting from contact with the leading edge of the elevated surface. We discuss why certain environmental properties, such as path-narrowing, have significance for how one becomes implicitly oriented the surrounding environment.
Collapse
|
10
|
Abstract
Events make up much of our lived experience, and the perceptual mechanisms that represent events in experience have pervasive effects on action control, language use, and remembering. Event representations in both perception and memory have rich internal structure and connections one to another, and both are heavily informed by knowledge accumulated from previous experiences. Event perception and memory have been identified with specific computational and neural mechanisms, which show protracted development in childhood and are affected by language use, expertise, and brain disorders and injuries. Current theoretical approaches focus on the mechanisms by which events are segmented from ongoing experience, and emphasize the common coding of events for perception, action, and memory. Abetted by developments in eye-tracking, neuroimaging, and computer science, research on event perception and memory is moving from small-scale laboratory analogs to the complexity of events in the wild.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey M Zacks
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Abstract
Knowledge benefits episodic memory, particularly when provided before encoding (Anderson & Pichert in Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(1), 1-12, 1978; Bransford & Johnson in Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 717-726, 1972). These benefits can occur through several encoding mechanisms, one of which may be event segmentation. Event segmentation is one's ability to parse information into meaningful units as an activity unfolds. The current experiment evaluated whether two top-down manipulations-providing context or perspective taking-influence the segmentation and memory of text. For the ambiguous texts in Experiment 1, half the participants received context in the form of a title, whereas the other half received no context. For the text in Experiment 2, half the participants read from the perspective of a burglar and the other half read from the perspective of a home buyer. In both experiments, participants read the passages, recalled the information, and then segmented the passages into meaningful units. Consistent with previous findings, participants who received context recalled more information compared with those who received no context, and participants in one perspective were more likely to recall information relevant to their perspective. Most importantly, we found that context and perspective facilitated more normative segmentation; however, the differences were small and suggest that effects of top-down processing on the segmentation of text may be modest at best. Thus, event segmentation processes that operate during text comprehension are influenced by semantic knowledge but may be more heavily driven by other factors (e.g., perceptual cues).
Collapse
|
12
|
Ongchoco JDK, Scholl BJ. Did that just happen? Event segmentation influences enumeration and working memory for simple overlapping visual events. Cognition 2019; 187:188-197. [PMID: 30897509 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2018] [Revised: 01/02/2019] [Accepted: 01/03/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
For working memory to be efficient, it is important not only to remember, but also to forget-thus freeing up memory for additional information. But what triggers forgetting? Beyond continuous temporal decay, memory is thought to be effectively 'flushed' to some degree at discrete event boundaries-i.e. when one event ends and another begins. But this framework does not readily apply to real-world visual experience, where events are constantly and asynchronously beginning, unfolding, and ending all around us. In this rush of things always happening, when might memory be flushed? In a series of experiments, we explored this using maximally simple visual events. A number of dots appeared, a subset moved at random speeds in random directions, and observers simply had to estimate the number of dots that moved. Critically, however, these motions could begin and end asynchronously. In general, asynchronous motions led to underestimation, but further experiments demonstrated that this was driven only by endings: regardless of whether dots started moving together or separately, animations with asynchronous endings led to underestimation-even while carefully controlling for both the overall amount of motion and average starting and ending times. (In contrast, no such effect occurred for asynchronous beginnings.) Thus, the ends of events seem to have an outsize influence on working memory-but only in the context of other ongoing events: once a motion ends amidst other unfinished motions, it seems more difficult to recall that particular motion as having occurred as a distinct event.
Collapse
|
13
|
Pettijohn KA, Radvansky GA. Walking through doorways causes forgetting: active and passive interaction. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2018.1540492] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Kyle A. Pettijohn
- The Henry M. Jackson Foundation for the Advancement of Military Medicine (HJF), Bethesda, MD, USA
- Naval Medical Research Unit – Dayton, Dayton, OH, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
The aim of the current study was to explore how the location updating effect is affected when people are tested using recall rather than recognition, which is what has been done in prior work. Differences in the memory processes involved with these two tasks lead to predictions for two different patterns of data. In Experiment 1, memory was tested by having participants recall the single object they were carrying or had just put down, whereas in Experiment 2, people sometimes needed to recall both objects. It was found that, unlike recognition test performance, a similar location updating effect was found for both Associated (what was currently being carried) and Dissociated (what was recently set down) objects. Moreover, when both objects were correctly recalled, there was a bias to remember them in the order that they were encountered. Finally, if only one object was correctly recalled, it was the Associated object that was currently being carried. Overall, these results are consistent with the Event Horizon Model of event cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyle A Pettijohn
- a Department of Psychology , University of Notre Dame , Notre Dame , IN , USA.,b The Henry M. Jackson Foundation for the Advancement of Military Medicine (HJF) , Bethesda , MD , USA.,c Naval Medical Research Unit - Dayton , Dayton , OH , USA
| | - Gabriel A Radvansky
- a Department of Psychology , University of Notre Dame , Notre Dame , IN , USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
This study explores location-based prospective memory. People often have to remember to do things when in a particular location, such as buying tissues the next time they are in the supermarket. For event cognition theory, location is important for structuring events. However, because event cognition has not been used to examine prospective memory, the question remains of how multiple events will influence prospective memory performance. In our experiments, people delivered messages from store to store in a virtual shopping mall as an ongoing task. The prospective tasks were to do certain activities in certain stores. For Experiment 1, each trial involved one prospective memory task to be done in a single location at one of three delays. The virtual environment and location cues were effective for prospective memory, and performance was unaffected by delay. For Experiment 2, each trial involved two prospective memory tasks, given in either one or two instruction locations, and to be done in either one or two store locations. There was improved performance when people received instructions from two locations and did both tasks in one location relative to other combinations. This demonstrates that location-based event structure influences how well people perform on prospective memory tasks.
Collapse
|
16
|
Abstract
PURPOSE OF REVIEW The aim of this review is to report on the specialized neuronal systems mediating spatial orientation and navigation discovered in animal experiments. These findings have important implications for the clinical management of patients with vestibular disorders or dementia and for translational research in these fields. RECENT FINDINGS The following anatomically and functionally separate, but nevertheless cooperative cell types have been characterized: angular head velocity cells and head direction cells, which depend on vestibular input and interact with place cells and grid cells, which represent position and distance. The entire system is thought to encode internal cognitive maps whose spatial data can be utilized for navigation and orientation. Flying and swimming species use spatial orientation and navigation isotropically, i.e., in the earth-horizontal and vertical directions, whereas ground-based species, including humans, perform better in the earth-horizontal plane (anisotropically). Examples of clinical disorders with deficits of spatial orientation and navigation are bilateral peripheral vestibulopathy, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia. SUMMARY Testing spatial orientation and navigation should become an integral part of routine neurological examinations, especially in the elderly. Also desirable are the further development and standardization of simple and reliable smart phone-based bedside tests to measure these functions in patients.
Collapse
|
17
|
Abstract
Research on event cognition is rapidly developing and is revealing fundamental aspects of human cognition. In this paper, we review recent and current work that is driving this field forward. We first outline the Event Horizon Model, which broadly describes the impact of event boundaries on cognition and memory. Then, we address recent work on event segmentation, the role of event cognition in working memory and long-term memory, including event model updating, and long term retention. Throughout we also consider how event cognition varies across individuals and groups of people and consider the neural mechanisms involved.
Collapse
|