1
|
Soffer M, Butters MA, Herrmann N, Black SE, Kumar S, Pugh B, Rajji TK, Tartaglia MC, Tang-Wai DF, Freedman M. About time: neurocognitive correlates of stimulus-bound and other time setting errors in the Clock Drawing Test. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2024; 30:471-478. [PMID: 38088261 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617723011396] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/09/2024]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Previous findings suggest that time setting errors (TSEs) in the Clock Drawing Test (CDT) may be related mainly to impairments in semantic and executive function. Recent attempts to dissociate the classic stimulus-bound error (setting the time to "10 to 11" instead of "10 past 11") from other TSEs, did not support hypotheses regarding this error being primarily executive in nature or different from other time setting errors in terms of neurocognitive correlates. This study aimed to further investigate the cognitive correlates of stimulus-bound errors and other TSEs, in order to trace possible underlying cognitive deficits. METHODS We examined cognitive test performance of participants with preliminary diagnoses associated with mild cognitive impairment. Among 490 participants, we identified clocks with stimulus-bound errors (n = 78), other TSEs (n = 41), other errors not related to time settings (n = 176), or errorless clocks (n = 195). RESULTS No differences were found on any dependent measure between the stimulus-bound and the other TSErs groups. Group comparisons suggested TSEs in general, to be associated with lower performance on various cognitive measures, especially on semantic and working memory measures. Regression analysis further highlighted semantic and verbal working memory difficulties as being the most prominent deficits associated with these errors. CONCLUSION TSEs in the CDT may indicate underlying deficits in semantic function and working memory. In addition, results support previous findings related to the diagnostic value of TSEs in detecting cognitive impairment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matan Soffer
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Canada
| | - Meryl A Butters
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Nathan Herrmann
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
- Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Toronto, Canada
| | - Sandra E Black
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), Unviversity of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Sanjeev Kumar
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Canada
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Bradley Pugh
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences Centre, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Tarek K Rajji
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Canada
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Maria Carmela Tartaglia
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), Unviversity of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
- University Health Network Memory Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - David F Tang-Wai
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), Unviversity of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
- University Health Network Memory Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Morris Freedman
- Toronto Dementia Research Alliance, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Medicine (Neurology), Unviversity of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences Centre, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Frank B, Bandyopadhyay S, Dion C, Formanski E, Matusz E, Penney D, Davis R, O'Connor MK, Au R, Amini S, Rashidi P, Tighe P, Libon DJ, Price CC. A Network Analysis of Digital Clock Drawing for Command and Copy Conditions. Assessment 2024:10731911241236336. [PMID: 38494894 PMCID: PMC11408704 DOI: 10.1177/10731911241236336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/19/2024]
Abstract
Graphomotor and time-based variables from the digital Clock Drawing Test (dCDT) characterize cognitive functions. However, no prior publications have quantified the strength of the associations between digital clock variables as they are produced. We hypothesized that analysis of the production of clock features and their interrelationships, as suggested, will differ between the command and copy test conditions. Older adults aged 65+ completed a digital clock drawing to command and copy conditions. Using a Bayesian hill-climbing algorithm and bootstrapping (10,000 samples), we derived directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) to examine network structure for command and copy dCDT variables. Although the command condition showed moderate associations between variables (μ | β z | = 0.34) relative to the copy condition (μ | β z | = 0.25), the copy condition network had more connections (18/18 versus 15/18 command). Network connectivity across command and copy was most influenced by five of the 18 variables. The direction of dependencies followed the order of instructions better in the command condition network. Digitally acquired clock variables relate to one another but differ in network structure when derived from command or copy conditions. Continued analyses of clock drawing production should improve understanding of quintessential normal features to aid in early neurodegenerative disease detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brandon Frank
- University of Florida, Gainesville, USA
- Boston University, MA, USA
| | | | | | | | | | - Dana Penney
- Lahey Clinic Medical Center, Burlington, MA, USA
| | - Randall Davis
- Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Guimarães V, Sousa I, de Bruin ED, Pais J, Correia MV. Using shoe-mounted inertial sensors and stepping exergames to assess the motor-cognitive status of older adults: A correlational study. Digit Health 2023; 9:20552076231167001. [PMID: 37009304 PMCID: PMC10061638 DOI: 10.1177/20552076231167001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2022] [Accepted: 03/15/2023] [Indexed: 03/31/2023] Open
Abstract
Objective Stepping exergames designed to stimulate physical and cognitive skills can provide important information concerning individuals’ performance. In this study, we investigated the potential of stepping and gameplay metrics to assess the motor-cognitive status of older adults. Methods Stepping and gameplay metrics were recorded in a longitudinal study involving 13 older adults with mobility limitations. Game parameters included games’ scores and reaction times. Stepping parameters included length, height, speed, and duration, measured by inertial sensors placed on the shoes while interacting with the exergames. Parameters measured on the first gameplay were correlated against standard cognitive and mobility assessments, including the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), gait speed, and the Short Physical Performance Battery. Based on MoCA scores, patients were then stratified into two groups: cognitively impaired and healthy controls. The differences between the two groups were visually inspected, considering their within-game progression over the training period. Results Stepping and gameplay metrics had moderate-to-strong correlations with cognitive and mobility performance indicators: faster, longer, and higher steps were associated with better mobility scores; better cognitive games’ scores and reaction times, and longer and faster steps were associated with better cognitive performance. The preliminary visual analysis revealed that the group with cognitive impairment required more time to advance to the next difficulty level, also presenting slower reaction times and stepping speeds when compared to the healthy control group. Conclusion Stepping exergames may be useful for assessing the cognitive and motor status of older adults, potentially allowing assessments to be more frequent, affordable, and enjoyable. Further research is required to confirm results in the long term using a larger and more diverse sample.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vânia Guimarães
- Fraunhofer Portugal AICOS, Porto, Portugal
- Faculty of Engineering, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- Vânia Guimarães, Fraunhofer Portugal AICOS, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal.
| | - Inês Sousa
- Fraunhofer Portugal AICOS, Porto, Portugal
| | - Eling D. de Bruin
- Institute of Human Movement Sciences and Sport, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Division of Physiotherapy, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
- Department of Health, OST - Eastern Swiss University of Applied Sciences, St. Gallen, Switzerland
| | - Joana Pais
- Neuroinova, Lda., Vila Nova de Gaia, Portugal
- EPIUnit - Institute of Public Health, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- Laboratory for Integrative and Translational Research in Population Health (ITR), University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - Miguel Velhote Correia
- Faculty of Engineering, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- INESC TEC (Institute for Systems and Computer Engineering, Technology and Science), Porto, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|