1
|
Paulsson NI, Taborsky M. Norway rats help social partners in need in response to ultrasonic begging signals. Ethology 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.13333] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Niklas Ingvar Paulsson
- Division of Behavioural Ecology Institute of Ecology and Evolution, University of Bern Switzerland
| | - Michael Taborsky
- Division of Behavioural Ecology Institute of Ecology and Evolution, University of Bern Switzerland
- Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior Konstanz Germany
- Institute for Advanced Study (Wissenschaftskolleg) Berlin Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Paulsson NI, Taborsky M. Reaching Out for Inaccessible Food Is a Potential Begging Signal in Cooperating Wild-Type Norway Rats, Rattus norvegicus. Front Psychol 2021; 12:712333. [PMID: 34526936 PMCID: PMC8435742 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.712333] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2021] [Accepted: 07/30/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Begging is widespread in juvenile animals. It typically induces helpful behaviours in parents and brood care helpers. However, begging is sometimes also shown by adults towards unrelated social partners. Adult Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus) display a sequence of different behaviours in a reciprocal food provisioning task that have been interpreted as such signals of need. The first behaviour in this sequence represents reaching out for a food item the animal cannot obtain independently. This may reflect either an attempt to grasp the food object by itself, or a signal to the social partner communicating the need for help. To distinguish between these two possibilities, we tested in female wild-type Norway rats if the amount of reaching performed by a food-deprived rat changes with the presence/absence of food and a social partner. Focal rats displayed significantly more reaching behaviour, both in terms of number and total duration of events, when food and a potentially helpful partner were present compared to when either was missing. Our findings hence support the hypothesis that rats use reaching behaviour to signal need to social partners that can help them to obtain food.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niklas I Paulsson
- Division of Behavioural Ecology, Institute of Ecology and Evolution, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Michael Taborsky
- Division of Behavioural Ecology, Institute of Ecology and Evolution, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Dantzer B, Dubuc C, Goncalves IB, Cram DL, Bennett NC, Ganswindt A, Heistermann M, Duncan C, Gaynor D, Clutton-Brock TH. The development of individual differences in cooperative behaviour: maternal glucocorticoid hormones alter helping behaviour of offspring in wild meerkats. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2019; 374:20180117. [PMID: 30966876 PMCID: PMC6460081 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2018.0117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/08/2018] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
The phenotype of parents can have long-lasting effects on the development of offspring as well as on their behaviour, physiology and morphology as adults. In some cases, these changes may increase offspring fitness but, in others, they can elevate parental fitness at a cost to the fitness of their offspring. We show that in Kalahari meerkats ( Suricata suricatta), the circulating glucocorticoid (GC) hormones of pregnant females affect the growth and cooperative behaviour of their offspring. We performed a 3-year experiment in wild meerkats to test the hypothesis that GC-mediated maternal effects reduce the potential for offspring to reproduce directly and therefore cause them to exhibit more cooperative behaviour. Daughters (but not sons) born to mothers treated with cortisol during pregnancy grew more slowly early in life and exhibited significantly more of two types of cooperative behaviour (pup rearing and feeding) once they were adults compared to offspring from control mothers. They also had lower measures of GCs as they aged, which could explain the observed increases in cooperative behaviour. Because early life growth is a crucial determinant of fitness in female meerkats, our results indicate that GC-mediated maternal effects may reduce the fitness of offspring, but may elevate parental fitness as a consequence of increasing the cooperative behaviour of their daughters. This article is part of the theme issue 'Developing differences: early-life effects and evolutionary medicine'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ben Dantzer
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ, UK
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| | - Constance Dubuc
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ, UK
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
| | - Ines Braga Goncalves
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Dominic L. Cram
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ, UK
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
| | - Nigel C. Bennett
- Department of Zoology and Entomology, Mammal Research Institute, University of Pretoria, 0002 Pretoria, South Africa
| | - Andre Ganswindt
- Department of Zoology and Entomology, Mammal Research Institute, University of Pretoria, 0002 Pretoria, South Africa
- Endocrine Research Laboratory, Department of Anatomy and Physiology, University of Pretoria, Onderstepoort 0110, South Africa
| | - Michael Heistermann
- Endocrinology Laboratory, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
| | - Chris Duncan
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ, UK
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
| | - David Gaynor
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
- Department of Zoology and Entomology, Mammal Research Institute, University of Pretoria, 0002 Pretoria, South Africa
| | - Tim H. Clutton-Brock
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ, UK
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
- Department of Zoology and Entomology, Mammal Research Institute, University of Pretoria, 0002 Pretoria, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Dantzer B, Goncalves IB, Spence-Jones HC, Bennett NC, Heistermann M, Ganswindt A, Dubuc C, Gaynor D, Manser MB, Clutton-Brock TH. The influence of stress hormones and aggression on cooperative behaviour in subordinate meerkats. Proc Biol Sci 2018; 284:rspb.2017.1248. [PMID: 28931736 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2017.1248] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2017] [Accepted: 08/22/2017] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
In cooperative breeders, aggression from dominant breeders directed at subordinates may raise subordinate stress hormone (glucocorticoid) concentrations. This may benefit dominants by suppressing subordinate reproduction but it is uncertain whether aggression from dominants can elevate subordinate cooperative behaviour, or how resulting changes in subordinate glucocorticoid concentrations affect their cooperative behaviour. We show here that the effects of manipulating glucocorticoid concentrations in wild meerkats (Suricata suricatta) on cooperative behaviour varied between cooperative activities as well as between the sexes. Subordinates of both sexes treated with a glucocorticoid receptor antagonist (mifepristone) exhibited significantly more pup protection behaviour (babysitting) compared to those treated with glucocorticoids (cortisol) or controls. Females treated with mifepristone had a higher probability of exhibiting pup food provisioning (pup-feeding) compared to those treated with cortisol. In males, there were no treatment effects on the probability of pup-feeding, but those treated with cortisol gave a higher proportion of the food they found to pups than those treated with mifepristone. Using 19 years of behavioural data, we also show that dominant females did not increase the frequency with which they directed aggression at subordinates at times when the need for assistance was highest. Our results suggest that it is unlikely that dominant females manipulate the cooperative behaviour of subordinates through the effects of aggression on their glucocorticoid levels and that the function of aggression directed at subordinates is probably to reduce the probability they will breed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ben Dantzer
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK .,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
| | - Ines Braga Goncalves
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa.,Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, Animal Behaviour, University of Zurich, Switzerland
| | | | - Nigel C Bennett
- Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
| | - Michael Heistermann
- Endocrinology Laboratory, German Primate Center, Kellnerweg 4, D-37077 Göttingen, Germany
| | - Andre Ganswindt
- Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa.,Endocrine Research Laboratory, Department of Anatomy and Physiology, University of Pretoria, 0110 Onderstepoort, South Africa
| | - Constance Dubuc
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa
| | - David Gaynor
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa.,Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
| | - Marta B Manser
- Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa.,Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, Animal Behaviour, University of Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Tim H Clutton-Brock
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, Northern Cape, South Africa.,Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Amici F, Mimó MC, von Borell C, Bueno-Guerra N. Meerkats (Suricata suricatta) fail to prosocially donate food in an experimental set-up. Anim Cogn 2017; 20:1059-1066. [PMID: 28766161 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-017-1122-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2017] [Revised: 07/26/2017] [Accepted: 07/28/2017] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Although humans are usually believed to be prosocial, the evolutionary origins of prosociality are largely debated. One hypothesis is that cooperative breeding has been one major precursor to the emergence of prosociality. In vertebrates, however, experimental evidence of prosociality has been mainly gathered in non-human primates. In this study, we tested the cooperative breeding hypothesis in cooperative breeding meerkats (Suricata suricatta). In particular, we tested whether meerkats take into account partners' benefits when distributing food rewards. Nine individuals were presented with two platforms baited with different food distributions (providing food to themselves, to a partner or both). In all conditions, the decision to operate the apparatus was based on the presence of food on the subject's side, and not on the possible benefits to partners. Despite being cooperative breeders, meerkats in this study failed to be prosocial, suggesting that prosociality in this species may be limited to specific contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Federica Amici
- Junior Research Group "Primate Kin Selection", Institute of Biology, Faculty of Bioscience, Pharmacy and Psychology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany.,Department of Primatology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.,Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Montserrat Colell Mimó
- Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Christoph von Borell
- Institute of Psychology, Biological Personality Psychology, University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Nereida Bueno-Guerra
- Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain. .,Department of Comparative and Developmental Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany. .,Department of Psychology, Comillas Pontifical University, Madrid, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Wyman MT, Rivers PR, Muller C, Toni P, Manser MB. Adult meerkats modify close call rate in the presence of pups. Curr Zool 2017; 63:349-355. [PMID: 29491994 PMCID: PMC5804182 DOI: 10.1093/cz/zox029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2017] [Accepted: 04/28/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
In animals, signaling behavior is often context-dependent, with variation in the probability of emitting certain signals dependent on fitness advantages. Senders may adjust signaling rate depending on receiver identity, presence of audiences, or noise masking the signal, all of which can affect the benefits and costs of signal production. In the cooperative breeding meerkat Suricata suricatta, group members emit soft contact calls, termed as “close calls”, while foraging in order to maintain group cohesion. Here, we investigated how the close calling rate during foraging was affected by the presence of pups, that produce continuous, noisy begging calls as they follow older group members. Adults decreased their overall close call rate substantially when pups were foraging with the group in comparison to periods when no pups were present. We suggest this decrease was likely due to a masking effect of the loud begging calls, which makes the close call function of maintaining group cohesion partly redundant as the centrally located begging calls can be used instead to maintain cohesion. There was some support that adults use close calls strategically to attract specific pups based on fitness advantages, that is, as the philopatric sex, females should call more than males and more to female pups than male pups. Dominant females called more than dominant males when a pup was in close proximity, while subordinates showed no sex-based differences. The sex of the nearest pup did not affect the calling rate of adults. The study shows that meerkats modify their close call production depending on benefits gained from calling and provides an example of the flexible use of one calling system in the presence of another, here contact calls versus begging calls, within the same species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan T Wyman
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zurich CH 8057, Switzerland.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, P.O. Box 64, Van Zylsrus, Northern Cape 8467, South Africa
| | - Pearl R Rivers
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zurich CH 8057, Switzerland.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, P.O. Box 64, Van Zylsrus, Northern Cape 8467, South Africa
| | - Coline Muller
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zurich CH 8057, Switzerland.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, P.O. Box 64, Van Zylsrus, Northern Cape 8467, South Africa
| | - Pauline Toni
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zurich CH 8057, Switzerland.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, P.O. Box 64, Van Zylsrus, Northern Cape 8467, South Africa
| | - Marta B Manser
- Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zurich CH 8057, Switzerland.,Kalahari Meerkat Project, Kuruman River Reserve, P.O. Box 64, Van Zylsrus, Northern Cape 8467, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
The vocal repertoire of adult and neonate giant otters (Pteronura brasiliensis). PLoS One 2014; 9:e112562. [PMID: 25391142 PMCID: PMC4229255 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112562] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2014] [Accepted: 10/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Animals use vocalizations to exchange information about external events, their own physical or motivational state, or about individuality and social affiliation. Infant babbling can enhance the development of the full adult vocal repertoire by providing ample opportunity for practice. Giant otters are very social and frequently vocalizing animals. They live in highly cohesive groups, generally including a reproductive pair and their offspring born in different years. This basic social structure may vary in the degree of relatedness of the group members. Individuals engage in shared group activities and different social roles and thus, the social organization of giant otters provides a basis for complex and long-term individual relationships. We recorded and analysed the vocalizations of adult and neonate giant otters from wild and captive groups. We classified the adult vocalizations according to their acoustic structure, and described their main behavioural context. Additionally, we present the first description of vocalizations uttered in babbling bouts of new born giant otters. We expected to find 1) a sophisticated vocal repertoire that would reflect the species’ complex social organisation, 2) that giant otter vocalizations have a clear relationship between signal structure and function, and 3) that the vocal repertoire of new born giant otters would comprise age-specific vocalizations as well as precursors of the adult repertoire. We found a vocal repertoire with 22 distinct vocalization types produced by adults and 11 vocalization types within the babbling bouts of the neonates. A comparison within the otter subfamily suggests a relation between vocal and social complexity, with the giant otters being the socially and vocally most complex species.
Collapse
|
8
|
Schmidt R, Morrison A, Kunc HP. Sexy voices – no choices: male song in noise fails to attract females. Anim Behav 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.05.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
9
|
Kunc HP, Lyons GN, Sigwart JD, McLaughlin KE, Houghton JDR. Anthropogenic noise affects behavior across sensory modalities. Am Nat 2014; 184:E93-100. [PMID: 25226190 DOI: 10.1086/677545] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Many species are currently experiencing anthropogenically driven environmental changes. Among these changes, increasing noise levels are specifically a problem for species using acoustic signals (i.e., species relying on signals that use the same sensory modality as anthropogenic noise). Yet many species use other sensory modalities, such as visual and olfactory signals, to communicate. However, we have only little understanding of whether changes in the acoustic environment affect species that use sensory modalities other than acoustic signals. We studied the impact of anthropogenic noise on the common cuttlefish Sepia officinalis, which uses highly complex visual signals. We showed that cuttlefish adjusted their visual displays by changing their color more frequently during a playback of anthropogenic noise, compared with before and after the playback. Our results provide experimental evidence that anthropogenic noise has a marked effect on the behavior of species that are not reliant on acoustic communication. Thus, interference in one sensory channel, in this case the acoustic one, affects signaling in other sensory channels. By considering sensory channels in isolation, we risk overlooking the broader implications of environmental changes for the behavior of animals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hansjoerg P Kunc
- Institute for Global Food Security, Queen's University Belfast, School of Biological Sciences, Medical Biology Centre, 97 Lisburn Road, Belfast BT9 7BL, United Kingdom; and Queen's University Marine Laboratory, 12-13 The Strand, Portaferry, County Down BT22 1PF, United Kingdom
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Manser MB, Jansen DA, Graw B, Hollén LI, Bousquet CA, Furrer RD, le Roux A. Vocal Complexity in Meerkats and Other Mongoose Species. ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-800286-5.00006-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
|
11
|
Thompson AM, Ridley AR, Hockey PA, Finch FM, Britton A, Raihani NJ. The influence of siblings on begging behaviour. Anim Behav 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
12
|
Rödel HG, Landmann C, Starkloff A, Kunc HP, Hudson R. Absentee Mothering - Not So Absent? Responses of European Rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) Mothers to Pup Distress Calls. Ethology 2013. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.12149] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Heiko G. Rödel
- Laboratoire d'Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée E.A. 4443 (LEEC); Université Paris 13; Sorbonne Paris Cité; Villetaneuse; France
| | - Christina Landmann
- Department of Animal Physiology; University of Bayreuth; Bayreuth; Germany
| | - Anett Starkloff
- Department of Animal Physiology; University of Bayreuth; Bayreuth; Germany
| | - Hansjörg P. Kunc
- School of Biological Sciences; Queen's University Belfast; Belfast; United Kingdom
| | - Robyn Hudson
- Instituto de Investigaciones Biomédicas; Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México; Mexico, Distrito Federal; Mexico
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Geipel I, Kalko EK, Wallmeyer K, Knörnschild M. Postweaning maternal food provisioning in a bat with a complex hunting strategy. Anim Behav 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
14
|
McLaughlin KE, Kunc HP. Experimentally increased noise levels change spatial and singing behaviour. Biol Lett 2013; 9:20120771. [PMID: 23173189 DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0771] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
The reasons why animal populations decline in response to anthropogenic noise are still poorly understood. To understand how populations are affected by noise, we must understand how individuals are affected by noise. By modifying the acoustic environment experimentally, we studied the potential relationship between noise levels and both spatial and singing behaviour in the European robin (Erithacus rubecula). We found that with increasing noise levels, males were more likely to move away from the noise source and changed their singing behaviour. Our results provide the first experimental evidence in a free ranging species, that not merely the presence of noise causes changes in behaviour and distribution, but that the level of noise pollution plays a crucial role as well. Our results have important implications for estimating the impact of infrastructure which differs in the level of noise produced. Thus, governmental planning bodies should not only consider the physical effect on the landscape when assessing the impact of new infrastructure, but also the noise levels emitted, which may reduce the loss of suitable habitats available for animals.
Collapse
|
15
|
Thompson AM, Raihani NJ, Hockey PAR, Britton A, Finch FM, Ridley AR. The influence of fledgling location on adult provisioning: a test of the blackmail hypothesis. Proc Biol Sci 2013; 280:20130558. [PMID: 23576792 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0558] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
One theory to explain the existence of conspicuous solicitation is that it is a way for young to 'blackmail' carers into provisioning them, by threatening their own destruction. Fledgling birds offer a unique opportunity to investigate the 'blackmail theory', as their mobility enables them to influence the predation risk they face. We investigated a novel solicitation behaviour in fledgling pied babblers (Turdoides bicolor), where fledglings use their location to influence provisioning rates. We show that fledglings face a trade-off: the ground is a much more profitable location in terms of provisioning rate from adult carers, but they are at greater risk from predators owing to their limited flying ability and slow response to alarm calls. Young babbler fledglings move to the ground when hungry, signalling their state, and this stimulates adults to increase their provisioning rates. Once satiated, fledglings return to the safety of cover. By experimentally increasing terrestrial predation risk, we found that adults increased their provisioning rate to terrestrial but not arboreal fledglings. Thus, by moving to a riskier location, fledglings revealed their need and were able to manipulate adults to achieve higher provisioning rates. These results provide support for the 'blackmail theory'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A M Thompson
- Percy FitzPatrick Institute of African Ornithology, University of Cape Town, Western Cape, South Africa.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Gross K, Pasinelli G, Kunc H. Behavioral Plasticity Allows Short‐Term Adjustment to a Novel Environment. Am Nat 2010; 176:456-64. [DOI: 10.1086/655428] [Citation(s) in RCA: 161] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
|
17
|
|
18
|
|
19
|
Roulin A, Dreiss A, Fioravanti C, Bize P. Vocal sib–sib interactions: how siblings adjust signalling level to each other. Anim Behav 2009. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
20
|
Madden JR, Kunc HP, English S, Manser MB, Clutton-Brock TH. Calling in the gap: competition or cooperation in littermates' begging behaviour? Proc Biol Sci 2009; 276:1255-62. [PMID: 19129139 PMCID: PMC2660964 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1660] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Offspring are frequently raised alongside their siblings and are provisioned early in life by adults. Adult provisioning is stimulated by offspring begging, but it is unclear how each offspring should beg, given the begging behaviour of their siblings. It has previously been suggested that siblings may compete directly through begging for a fixed level of provisioning, or that siblings may cooperate in their begging in order to jointly elevate the level of provisioning by adults. We studied the begging behaviour of meerkat Suricata suricatta pups, explored how it changed as the begging behaviour of their littermates altered, and asked how the adults responded to group-level changes in begging. We found conflicting evidence for classic models of competitive and cooperative begging. Pups reared in larger litters begged at higher rates, yet experimentally increasing begging levels within groups caused individual begging rates to decrease. Pups decreased begging rates when close to other begging pups, and pups spaced further apart were fed more. Adults increased their overall level of provisioning as group levels of begging increased, but per capita provisioning decreased. Adults preferred to provision speakers playing back recordings of two pups begging alternately to recordings of the same two pups begging simultaneously. Therefore, we suggest that meerkat pups avoid some of the costs of direct competition imposed by an escalation of begging as other pups beg, by begging in gaps between the bouts of others or avoiding littermates. Such behaviour is also preferred by provisioning adults, thus providing additional benefits to the pups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joah R Madden
- Large Animal Research Group, Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
|
22
|
|
23
|
English S, Kunc HP, Madden JR, Clutton-Brock TH. Sex differences in responsiveness to begging in a cooperative mammal. Biol Lett 2008; 4:334-7. [PMID: 18505713 DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
In species where young are provisioned by both parents, males commonly contribute less to parental care than females, and are less responsive to variation in begging rates. Similar differences in the care of young occur among adults in cooperative breeders, but fewer studies have investigated whether these are associated with differences in responsiveness. Here, we present results from a playback experiment investigating responsiveness to begging in the meerkat (Suricata suricatta), a cooperatively breeding mammal. Although increased begging rate raised the feeding rate of adults of both sexes, there was no consistent tendency for females to be more responsive than males. However, when we examined changes in the proportion of food items found that were fed to pups (generosity), we found that females were more responsive than males to increased begging rate. These results can be explained in terms of sex differences in dispersal: in meerkats, females are philopatric and receive considerable benefits from investing in young, both directly, by increasing group size, and indirectly, by recruiting helpers if they inherit the breeding position. In addition, they emphasize that generosity provides a more sensitive measure of responsiveness to begging than feeding rate, as it accounts for variation in foraging success.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sinead English
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Bell MB. Receiver identity modifies begging intensity independent of need in banded mongoose (Mungos mungo) pups. Behav Ecol 2008. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arn104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|