1
|
Nomano FY, Savage JL, Browning LE, Griffith SC, Russell AF. Breeding Phenology and Meteorological Conditions Affect Carer Provisioning Rates and Group-Level Coordination in Cooperative Chestnut-Crowned Babblers. Front Ecol Evol 2019. [DOI: 10.3389/fevo.2019.00423] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
2
|
|
3
|
Chestnut-crowned babbler calls are composed of meaningless shared building blocks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116:19579-19584. [PMID: 31501336 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1819513116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
A core component of human language is its combinatorial sound system: meaningful signals are built from different combinations of meaningless sounds. Investigating whether nonhuman communication systems are also combinatorial is hampered by difficulties in identifying the extent to which vocalizations are constructed from shared, meaningless building blocks. Here we present an approach to circumvent this difficulty and show that a pair of functionally distinct chestnut-crowned babbler (Pomatostomus ruficeps) vocalizations can be decomposed into perceptibly distinct, meaningless entities that are shared across the 2 calls. Specifically, by focusing on the acoustic distinctiveness of sound elements using a habituation-discrimination paradigm on wild-caught babblers under standardized aviary conditions, we show that 2 multielement calls are composed of perceptibly distinct sounds that are reused in different arrangements across the 2 calls. Furthermore, and critically, we show that none of the 5 constituent elements elicits functionally relevant responses in receivers, indicating that the constituent sounds do not carry the meaning of the call and so are contextually meaningless. Our work, which allows combinatorial systems in animals to be more easily identified, suggests that animals can produce functionally distinct calls that are built in a way superficially reminiscent of the way that humans produce morphemes and words. The results reported lend credence to the recent idea that language's combinatorial system may have been preceded by a superficial stage where signalers neither needed to be cognitively aware of the combinatorial strategy in place, nor of its building blocks.
Collapse
|
4
|
Capp E, Liebl AL, Cones AG, Russell AF. Advancing breeding phenology does not affect incubation schedules in chestnut-crowned babblers: Opposing effects of temperature and wind. Ecol Evol 2018; 8:696-705. [PMID: 29321906 PMCID: PMC5756856 DOI: 10.1002/ece3.3524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2017] [Revised: 09/23/2017] [Accepted: 10/02/2017] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Projecting population responses to climate change requires an understanding of climatic impacts on key components of reproduction. Here, we investigate the associations among breeding phenology, climate and incubation schedules in the chestnut-crowned babbler (Pomatostomus ruficeps), a 50 g passerine with female-only, intermittent incubation that typically breeds from late winter (July) to early summer (November). During daylight hours, breeding females spent an average of 33 min on the nest incubating (hereafter on-bouts) followed by 24-min foraging (hereafter off-bouts), leading to an average daytime nest attentiveness of 60%. Nest attentiveness was 25% shorter than expected from allometric calculations, largely because off-bout durations were double the expected value for a species with 16 g clutches (4 eggs × 4 g/egg). On-bout durations and daily attentiveness were both negatively related to ambient temperature, presumably because increasing temperatures allowed more time to be allocated to foraging with reduced detriment to egg cooling. By contrast, on-bout durations were positively associated with wind speed, in this case because increasing wind speed exacerbated egg cooling during off-bouts. Despite an average temperature change of 12°C across the breeding season, breeding phenology had no effect on incubation schedules. This surprising result arose because of a positive relationship between temperature and wind speed across the breeding season: Any benefit of increasing temperatures was canceled by apparently detrimental consequences of increasing wind speed on egg cooling. Our results indicate that a greater appreciation for the associations among climatic variables and their independent effects on reproductive investment are necessary to understand the effects of changing climates on breeding phenology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elliot Capp
- UNSW Arid Zone Research StationSchool of Biological, Earth and Environmental SciencesUniversity of New South WalesSydneyNSWAustralia
| | - Andrea L. Liebl
- Centre for Ecology and ConservationCollege of Life and Environmental SciencesUniversity of ExeterPenryn, CornwallUK
- Department of BiologyUniversity of South DakotaVermillionSDUSA
| | - Alexandra G. Cones
- Centre for Ecology and ConservationCollege of Life and Environmental SciencesUniversity of ExeterPenryn, CornwallUK
| | - Andrew F. Russell
- UNSW Arid Zone Research StationSchool of Biological, Earth and Environmental SciencesUniversity of New South WalesSydneyNSWAustralia
- Centre for Ecology and ConservationCollege of Life and Environmental SciencesUniversity of ExeterPenryn, CornwallUK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Savage JL, Browning LE, Manica A, Russell AF, Johnstone RA. Turn-taking in cooperative offspring care: by-product of individual provisioning behavior or active response rule? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2017; 71:162. [PMID: 29081573 PMCID: PMC5644705 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-017-2391-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/19/2017] [Revised: 09/28/2017] [Accepted: 09/29/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Abstract For individuals collaborating to rear offspring, effective organization of resource delivery is difficult because each carer benefits when the others provide a greater share of the total investment required. When investment is provided in discrete events, one possible solution is to adopt a turn-taking strategy whereby each individual reduces its contribution rate after investing, only increasing its rate again once another carer contributes. To test whether turn-taking occurs in a natural cooperative care system, here we use a continuous time Markov model to deduce the provisioning behavior of the chestnut-crowned babbler (Pomatostomus ruficeps), a cooperatively breeding Australian bird with variable number of carers. Our analysis suggests that turn-taking occurs across a range of group sizes (2–6), with individual birds being more likely to visit following other individuals than to make repeat visits. We show using a randomization test that some of this apparent turn-taking arises as a by-product of the distribution of individual inter-visit intervals (“passive” turn-taking) but that individuals also respond actively to the investment of others over and above this effect (“active” turn-taking). We conclude that turn-taking in babblers is a consequence of both their individual provisioning behavior and deliberate response rules, with the former effect arising through a minimum interval required to forage and travel to and from the nest. Our results reinforce the importance of considering fine-scale investment dynamics when studying parental care and suggest that behavioral rules such as turn-taking may be more common than previously thought. Significance statement Caring for offspring is a crucial stage in the life histories of many animals and often involves conflict as each carer typically benefits when others contribute a greater share of the work required. One way to resolve this conflict is to monitor when other carers contribute and adopt a simple “turn-taking” rule to ensure fairness, but natural parental care has rarely been studied in sufficient detail to identify such rules. Our study investigates whether cooperatively breeding chestnut-crowned babblers “take turns” delivering food to offspring, and (if so) whether this a deliberate strategy or simply a by-product of independent care behavior. We find that babblers indeed take turns and conclude that part of the observed turn-taking is due to deliberate responsiveness, with the rest arising from the species’ breeding ecology. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1007/s00265-017-2391-4) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James L Savage
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ UK.,Behavioural Ecology Group, Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, De Elst 1, 6708 WD Wageningen, The Netherlands.,School of Biological, Earth, and Environmental Sciences, University College Cork, Distillery Fields, North Mall, Cork, T23 TK30 Ireland
| | - Lucy E Browning
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ UK.,Fowlers Gap Arid Zone Research Station, School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, 2052 NSW Australia
| | - Andrea Manica
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ UK
| | - Andrew F Russell
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Tremough Campus, Penryn, TR10 9FE UK
| | - Rufus A Johnstone
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EJ UK
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Riehl C, Frederickson ME. Cheating and punishment in cooperative animal societies. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2016; 371:20150090. [PMID: 26729930 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0090] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Cheaters-genotypes that gain a selective advantage by taking the benefits of the social contributions of others while avoiding the costs of cooperating-are thought to pose a major threat to the evolutionary stability of cooperative societies. In order for cheaters to undermine cooperation, cheating must be an adaptive strategy: cheaters must have higher fitness than cooperators, and their behaviour must reduce the fitness of their cooperative partners. It is frequently suggested that cheating is not adaptive because cooperators have evolved mechanisms to punish these behaviours, thereby reducing the fitness of selfish individuals. However, a simpler hypothesis is that such societies arise precisely because cooperative strategies have been favoured over selfish ones-hence, behaviours that have been interpreted as 'cheating' may not actually result in increased fitness, even when they go unpunished. Here, we review the empirical evidence for cheating behaviours in animal societies, including cooperatively breeding vertebrates and social insects, and we ask whether such behaviours are primarily limited by punishment. Our review suggests that both cheating and punishment are probably rarer than often supposed. Uncooperative individuals typically have lower, not higher, fitness than cooperators; and when evidence suggests that cheating may be adaptive, it is often limited by frequency-dependent selection rather than by punishment. When apparently punitive behaviours do occur, it remains an open question whether they evolved in order to limit cheating, or whether they arose before the evolution of cooperation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christina Riehl
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, 106A Guyot Hall, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA
| | - Megan E Frederickson
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Toronto, 25 Willcocks Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5S 3B2
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Sorato E, Griffith SC, Russell AF. The price of associating with breeders in the cooperatively breeding chestnut‐crowned babbler: foraging constraints, survival and sociality. J Anim Ecol 2016; 85:1340-51. [DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.12539] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2015] [Accepted: 04/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Enrico Sorato
- Department of Biological Sciences Macquarie University Sydney NSW 2109 Australia
- Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology (IFM) Linköping University Linköping 58183 Sweden
| | - Simon C. Griffith
- Department of Biological Sciences Macquarie University Sydney NSW 2109 Australia
- Fowlers Gap Arid Zone Research Station School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences University of New South Wales Sydney NSW 2052 Australia
| | - Andy F. Russell
- Fowlers Gap Arid Zone Research Station School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences University of New South Wales Sydney NSW 2052 Australia
- Centre for Ecology and Conservation College of Life and Environmental Sciences University of Exeter, Tremough Campus Penryn TR10 9FE UK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Kramer J, Klauke N, Bauer M, Martin Schaefer H. No Evidence for Enforced Alloparental Care in a Cooperatively Breeding Parrot. Ethology 2016. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.12486] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Jos Kramer
- Department of Evolutionary Biology; Institute of Zoology; Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz; Mainz Germany
- Department of Animal Ecology and Evolution; Institute of Zoology; Albert Ludwigs University of Freiburg; Freiburg im Breisgau Germany
| | - Nadine Klauke
- Department of Animal Ecology and Evolution; Institute of Zoology; Albert Ludwigs University of Freiburg; Freiburg im Breisgau Germany
| | - Michael Bauer
- Department of Animal Ecology and Evolution; Institute of Zoology; Albert Ludwigs University of Freiburg; Freiburg im Breisgau Germany
| | - H. Martin Schaefer
- Department of Animal Ecology and Evolution; Institute of Zoology; Albert Ludwigs University of Freiburg; Freiburg im Breisgau Germany
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Liebl A, Nomano F, Browning L, Russell A. Experimental evidence for fully additive care among male carers in the cooperatively breeding chestnut-crowned babbler. Anim Behav 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.02.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
10
|
Liebl AL, Browning LE, Russell AF. Manipulating carer number versus brood size: complementary but not equivalent ways of quantifying carer effects on offspring. Behav Ecol 2016; 27:1247-1254. [PMID: 27418754 PMCID: PMC4943111 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arw038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2015] [Revised: 02/01/2016] [Accepted: 02/03/2016] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Measuring the causal effects of increasing carer number on offspring success is required to understand the evolution of cooperative care systems. Here, we did so using 2 experimental techniques in the chestnut-crowned babbler from outback Australia. Both carer removal and brood size manipulations indicate causal effects of helpers on offspring food acquisition. However, the results were not equivalent, with nestlings receiving more food following brood size manipulations, even after controlling for similar carer to offspring ratios. Experiments designed to quantify the effects of increasing numbers of carers on levels of offspring care are rare in cooperative breeding systems, where offspring are reared by individuals additional to the breeding pair. This paucity might stem from disagreement over the most appropriate manipulations necessary to elucidate these effects. Here, we perform both carer removal and brood enhancement experiments to test the effects of numbers of carers and carer:offspring ratios on provisioning rates in the cooperatively breeding chestnut-crowned babbler (Pomatostomus ruficeps). Removing carers caused linear reductions in overall brood provisioning rates. Further analyses failed to provide evidence that this effect was influenced by territory quality or disruption of group dynamics stemming from the removals. Likewise, adding nestlings to broods caused linear increases in brood provisioning rates, suggesting carers are responsive to increasing offspring demand. However, the 2 experiments did not generate quantitatively equivalent results: Each nestling received more food following brood size manipulation than carer removal, despite comparable carer:offspring ratios in each. Following an at-hatching split-design cross-fostering manipulation to break any links between prehatching maternal effects and posthatching begging patterns, we found that begging intensity increased in larger broods after controlling for metrics of hunger. These findings suggest that manipulation of brood size can, in itself, influence nestling provisioning rates when begging intensity is affected by scramble competition. We highlight that carer number and brood size manipulations are complimentary but not equivalent; adopting both can yield greater overall insight into carer effects in cooperative breeding systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A L Liebl
- Centre for Ecology & Conservation, College of Life & Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter , Treliever Road, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9FE , UK and
| | - L E Browning
- UNSW Arid Zone Research Station, School of Biological, Earth & Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales , Sydney, New South Wales 2052 , Australia
| | - A F Russell
- Centre for Ecology & Conservation, College of Life & Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter , Treliever Road, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9FE , UK and
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Engesser S, Crane JMS, Savage JL, Russell AF, Townsend SW. Experimental Evidence for Phonemic Contrasts in a Nonhuman Vocal System. PLoS Biol 2015; 13:e1002171. [PMID: 26121619 PMCID: PMC4488142 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002171] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2014] [Accepted: 05/08/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to generate new meaning by rearranging combinations of meaningless sounds is a fundamental component of language. Although animal vocalizations often comprise combinations of meaningless acoustic elements, evidence that rearranging such combinations generates functionally distinct meaning is lacking. Here, we provide evidence for this basic ability in calls of the chestnut-crowned babbler (Pomatostomus ruficeps), a highly cooperative bird of the Australian arid zone. Using acoustic analyses, natural observations, and a series of controlled playback experiments, we demonstrate that this species uses the same acoustic elements (A and B) in different arrangements (AB or BAB) to create two functionally distinct vocalizations. Specifically, the addition or omission of a contextually meaningless acoustic element at a single position generates a phoneme-like contrast that is sufficient to distinguish the meaning between the two calls. Our results indicate that the capacity to rearrange meaningless sounds in order to create new signals occurs outside of humans. We suggest that phonemic contrasts represent a rudimentary form of phoneme structure and a potential early step towards the generative phonemic system of human language. The chestnut-crowned babbler, a social species of bird from the Australian outback, possesses the basic capacity to generate words, shedding new light on the origins of word generation in our own species. A major question in language evolution is how its generative power emerged. This power, which allows the communication of limitless thoughts and ideas, is a result of the combinatorial nature of human language: meaningless phonemes can be combined to form meaningful words (phonology), and words can be combined to form higher-order, meaningful structures (syntax). While previous work has indicated the potential for animals to form syntax-like constructions, there exists little convincing evidence for a basic phonemic capacity in animals. Here, we demonstrate, using analyses combined with natural observations and playback experiments, that the cooperatively breeding chestnut-crowned babbler reuses two meaningless acoustic elements to create two functionally distinct vocalizations. This result suggests the basic ability for phoneme structuring occurs outside of humans and provides insights into potential early evolutionary steps preceding the generative phonemic system of human language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sabrina Engesser
- Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Jodie M S Crane
- Department of Animal & Plant Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
| | - James L Savage
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Andrew F Russell
- Centre for Ecology & Conservation, College of Life & Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Penryn, Cornwall, United Kingdom; Fowlers Gap Arid Zone Research Station, School of Biological, Earth & Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
| | - Simon W Townsend
- Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Nomano FY, Browning LE, Savage JL, Rollins LA, Griffith SC, Russell AF. Unrelated helpers neither signal contributions nor suffer retribution in chestnut-crowed babblers. Behav Ecol 2015. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arv023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
|
13
|
Validation of an automated data collection method for quantifying social networks in collective behaviours. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-014-1757-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
14
|
Nomano FY, Browning LE, Rollins LA, Nakagawa S, Griffith SC, Russell AF. Feeding nestlings does not function as a signal of social prestige in cooperatively breeding chestnut-crowned babblers. Anim Behav 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
15
|
Carer provisioning rules in an obligate cooperative breeder: prey type, size and delivery rate. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2012. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-012-1419-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|