1
|
Lányi O, Kéri S, Pálffy Z, Polner B. Can you believe your eyes? Positive schizotypy is associated with increased susceptibility to the Müller-Lyer illusion. Schizophr Res 2024; 264:327-335. [PMID: 38215568 DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2023.12.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2022] [Revised: 11/27/2023] [Accepted: 12/17/2023] [Indexed: 01/14/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESIS Visual illusions provide a unique opportunity to understand cognitive and perceptual alterations in schizophrenia-spectrum conditions. Schizophrenia patients often exhibit increased susceptibility to the Müller-Lyer illusion. Here, we investigate susceptibility to the Müller-Lyer visual illusion in the general population with different levels of schizotypy. STUDY DESIGN We assessed a population-based convenience sample (N = 263) on an online platform. In addition to basic demographics, participants completed the Müller-Lyer illusion, the Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS) to measure perceptual anomalies, and the Multidimensional Schizotypy Scale - Brief (MSS-B) for schizotypic traits. To evaluate what predicts susceptibility to the illusion, we fitted a large set of multilevel logistic regression models and performed model averaging over the coefficients. STUDY RESULTS We found support for increased illusion susceptibility among individuals with high positive schizotypy. However, we did not find a comparable effect for anomalous perceptions alone, or for negative or disorganized schizotypy. CONCLUSIONS The increased Müller-Lyer effect in positive schizotypy might be specific to delusion-like beliefs and magical ideation. Further research is needed to clarify how a hierarchical Bayesian formulation of brain function (e.g. imbalances between bottom-up perceptual processing and substantial reliance on prior expectations) can account for the Müller-Lyer effect in schizophrenia-spectrum conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Orsolya Lányi
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Semmelweis University, Balassa utca 6, Budapest 1082, Hungary
| | - Szabolcs Kéri
- Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Egry József utca 1, Budapest 1111, Hungary; National Institute of Mental Health, Neurology and Neurosurgery - Nyírő Gyula Hospital, Lehel utca 59-61, Budapest 1135, Hungary
| | - Zsófia Pálffy
- Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Egry József utca 1, Budapest 1111, Hungary.
| | - Bertalan Polner
- Institute of Psychology, ELTE, Eötvös Loránd University, Izabella utca 46, Budapest 1064, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Chen L, Wu B, Yu H, Sperandio I. Network dynamics underlying alterations in apparent object size. Brain Commun 2024; 6:fcae006. [PMID: 38250057 PMCID: PMC10799746 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcae006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2023] [Revised: 12/14/2023] [Accepted: 01/09/2024] [Indexed: 01/23/2024] Open
Abstract
A target circle surrounded by small circles looks larger than an identical circle surrounded by large circles (termed as the Ebbinghaus illusion). While previous research has shown that both early and high-level visual regions are involved in the generation of the illusion, it remains unclear how these regions work together to modulate the illusion effect. Here, we used functional MRI and dynamic causal modelling to investigate the neural networks underlying the illusion in conditions where the focus of attention was manipulated via participants directing their attention to and maintain fixation on only one of the two illusory configurations at a time. Behavioural findings confirmed the presence of the illusion. Accordingly, functional MRI activity in the extrastriate cortex accounted for the illusory effects: apparently larger circles elicited greater activation than apparently smaller circles. Interestingly, this spread of activity for size overestimation was accompanied by a decrease in the inhibitory self-connection in the extrastriate region, and an increase in the feedback connectivity from the precuneus to the extrastriate region. These findings demonstrate that the representation of apparent object size relies on feedback projections from higher- to lower-level visual areas, highlighting the crucial role of top-down signals in conscious visual perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lihong Chen
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
- Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Dalian 116029, Liaoning Province, China
| | - Baoyu Wu
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
- Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Dalian 116029, Liaoning Province, China
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310058, China
| | - Haoyang Yu
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
- Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Dalian 116029, Liaoning Province, China
| | - Irene Sperandio
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto 38068, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
von Gal A, Boccia M, Nori R, Verde P, Giannini AM, Piccardi L. Neural networks underlying visual illusions: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Neuroimage 2023; 279:120335. [PMID: 37591478 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120335] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2023] [Revised: 07/05/2023] [Accepted: 08/14/2023] [Indexed: 08/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Visual illusions have long been used to study visual perception and contextual integration. Neuroimaging studies employ illusions to identify the brain regions involved in visual perception and how they interact. We conducted an Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) meta-analysis and meta-analytic connectivity modeling on fMRI studies using static and motion illusions to reveal the neural signatures of illusory processing and to investigate the degree to which different areas are commonly recruited in perceptual inference. The resulting networks encompass ventral and dorsal regions, including the inferior and middle occipital cortices bilaterally in both types of illusions. The static and motion illusion networks selectively included the right posterior parietal cortex and the ventral premotor cortex respectively. Overall, these results describe a network of areas crucially involved in perceptual inference relying on feed-back and feed-forward interactions between areas of the ventral and dorsal visual pathways. The same network is proposed to be involved in hallucinogenic symptoms characteristic of schizophrenia and other disorders, with crucial implications in the use of illusions as biomarkers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Maddalena Boccia
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy; Cognitive and Motor Rehabilitation and Neuroimaging Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
| | - Raffaella Nori
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | - Paola Verde
- Italian Air Force Experimental Flight Center, Aerospace Medicine Department, Pratica di Mare, Rome, Italy
| | | | - Laura Piccardi
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy; San Raffaele Cassino Hospital, Cassino, FR, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Hoba S, Fink GR, Zeng H, Weidner R. View Normalization of Object Size in the Right Parietal Cortex. Vision (Basel) 2022; 6:41. [PMID: 35893758 PMCID: PMC9326632 DOI: 10.3390/vision6030041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2022] [Revised: 06/07/2022] [Accepted: 06/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Prior knowledge alters perception already on early levels of processing. For instance, judging the display size of an object is affected by its familiar size. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we investigated the neural processes involved in resolving ambiguities between familiar object size and physical object size in 33 healthy human subjects. The familiar size was either small or large, and the object was displayed as either small or large. Thus, the size of the displayed object was either congruent or incongruent with its internally stored canonical size representation. Subjects were asked to indicate where the stimuli appeared on the screen as quickly and accurately as possible, thereby ensuring that differential activations cannot be ascribed to explicit object size judgments. Incongruent (relative to congruent) object displays were associated with enhanced activation of the right intraparietal sulcus (IPS). These data are consistent with but extend previous patient studies, which found the right parietal cortex involved in matching visual objects presented atypically to prototypical object representations, suggesting that the right IPS supports view normalization of objects. In a second experiment, using a parametric design, a region-of-interest analysis supported this notion by showing that increases in size mismatch between the displayed size of an object and its familiar viewing size were associated with an increased right IPS activation. We conclude that the right IPS performs view normalization of mismatched information about the internally stored prototypical size and the current viewing size of an object.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylvia Hoba
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-3, Research Center Jülich, 52425 Jülich, Germany; (S.H.); (G.R.F.)
| | - Gereon R. Fink
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-3, Research Center Jülich, 52425 Jülich, Germany; (S.H.); (G.R.F.)
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital Cologne, Cologne University, 50937 Cologne, Germany
| | - Hang Zeng
- Center for Educational Science and Technology, Beijing Normal University at Zhuhai, Zhuhai 519087, China
| | - Ralph Weidner
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-3, Research Center Jülich, 52425 Jülich, Germany; (S.H.); (G.R.F.)
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Candini M, D’Angelo M, Frassinetti F. Time Interaction With Two Spatial Dimensions: From Left/Right to Near/Far. Front Hum Neurosci 2022; 15:796799. [PMID: 35115914 PMCID: PMC8804530 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.796799] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2021] [Accepted: 12/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
In this study, we explored the time and space relationship according to two different spatial codings, namely, the left/right extension and the reachability of stimulus along a near/far dimension. Four experiments were carried out in which healthy participants performed the time and spatial bisection tasks in near/far space, before and after short or long tool-use training. Stimuli were prebisected horizontal lines of different temporal durations in which the midpoint was manipulated according to the Muller-Lyer illusion. The perceptual illusory effects emerged in spatial but not temporal judgments. We revealed that temporal and spatial representations dynamically change according to the action potentialities of an individual: temporal duration was perceived as shorter and the perceived line’s midpoint was shifted to the left in far than in near space. Crucially, this dissociation disappeared following a long but not short tool-use training. Finally, we observed age-related differences in spatial attention which may be crucial in building the memory temporal standard to categorize durations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michela Candini
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
- Unit of Recovery and Functional Rehabilitation, Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri IRCCS, Institute of Castel Goffredo, Mantova, Italy
- *Correspondence: Michela Candini,
| | - Mariano D’Angelo
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
- Unit of Recovery and Functional Rehabilitation, Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri IRCCS, Institute of Castel Goffredo, Mantova, Italy
| | - Francesca Frassinetti
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
- Unit of Recovery and Functional Rehabilitation, Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri IRCCS, Institute of Castel Goffredo, Mantova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Chen S, Weidner R, Zeng H, Fink GR, Müller HJ, Conci M. Feedback from lateral occipital cortex to V1/V2 triggers object completion: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and dynamic causal modeling. Hum Brain Mapp 2021; 42:5581-5594. [PMID: 34418200 PMCID: PMC8559483 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.25637] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2020] [Revised: 06/24/2021] [Accepted: 08/06/2021] [Indexed: 01/31/2023] Open
Abstract
Illusory figures demonstrate the visual system's ability to integrate disparate parts into coherent wholes. We probed this object integration process by either presenting an integrated diamond shape or a comparable ungrouped configuration that did not render a complete object. Two tasks were used that either required localization of a target dot (relative to the presented configuration) or discrimination of the dot's luminance. The results showed that only when the configuration was task relevant (in the localization task), performance benefited from the presentation of an integrated object. Concurrent functional magnetic resonance imaging was performed and analyzed using dynamic causal modeling to investigate the (causal) relationship between regions that are associated with illusory figure completion. We found object‐specific feedback connections between the lateral occipital cortex (LOC) and early visual cortex (V1/V2). These modulatory connections persisted across task demands and hemispheres. Our results thus provide direct evidence that interactions between mid‐level and early visual processing regions engage in illusory figure perception. These data suggest that LOC first integrates inputs from multiple neurons in lower‐level cortices, generating a global shape representation while more fine‐graded object details are then determined via feedback to early visual areas, independently of the current task demands.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Siyi Chen
- Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, München, Germany
| | - Ralph Weidner
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Center Jülich, Jülich, Germany
| | - Hang Zeng
- Center for Educational Science and Technology, Beijing Normal University at Zhuhai, Zhuhai, China
| | - Gereon R Fink
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Center Jülich, Jülich, Germany.,Department of Neurology, University Hospital Cologne, Cologne University, Cologne, Germany
| | - Hermann J Müller
- Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, München, Germany
| | - Markus Conci
- Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, München, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Surkys T. An auditory time perception illusion analogous to the visual Müller-Lyer illusion. Eur J Neurosci 2021; 54:6512-6519. [PMID: 34523749 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.15459] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2020] [Revised: 08/07/2021] [Accepted: 09/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
According to the centroid hypothesis, the visual Müller-Lyer-type illusions in which subjects misperceive lines or gaps as longer or shorter depending on surrounding distracters result from the pooling of positioning neural signals such that the perceived object is shifted towards these distracters. However, it is uncertain if this type of pooling is a more general principal that influences perceptions in other sensory modalities, including time perception based on auditory signals. In this study, I show that by applying the principles of the centroid hypothesis, an audial time duration illusion can be constructed. The perception of two sequential time intervals, which were defined by three short tone signals, was distorted by placing distracting white noise sounds near each signal. Misperception magnitude, which peaked at 31%, changed with the time interval between the tone signals and distracters; the relationship between the target-distracter distance and the illusion strength closely paralleled with that of a Müller-Lyer-type illusion, whereby the visual objects were analogically arranged in space rather than time. These results demonstrate that even if signals and distracters are distinguishable, the neural mechanisms for estimating time duration utilize coarser sampling to preserve processing resources at the expense of high accuracy. I hypothesize that systems that are dedicated to visual length and time duration estimations are based on similar perceptual magnitude evaluation algorithms. Moreover, this signal pooling principle may be applicable to other perceptual modalities across different species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tadas Surkys
- Institute of Research of Biological Systems and Genetics, Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Kaunas, Lithuania
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Leivada E, Mitrofanova N, Westergaard M. Bilinguals are better than monolinguals in detecting manipulative discourse. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0256173. [PMID: 34492035 PMCID: PMC8423309 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256173] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2021] [Accepted: 07/30/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
One of the most contentious topics in cognitive science concerns the impact of bilingualism on cognitive functions and neural resources. Research on executive functions has shown that bilinguals often perform better than monolinguals in tasks that require monitoring and inhibiting automatic responses. The robustness of this effect is a matter of an ongoing debate, with both sides approaching bilingual cognition mainly through measuring abilities that fall outside the core domain of language processing. However, the mental juggling that bilinguals perform daily involves language. This study takes a novel path to bilingual cognition by comparing the performance of monolinguals and bilinguals in a timed task that features a special category of stimulus, which has the peculiar ability to manipulate the cognitive parser into treating it as well-formed while it is not: grammatical illusions. The results reveal that bilinguals outperform monolinguals in detecting illusions, but they are also slower across the board in judging the stimuli, illusory or not. We capture this trade-off by proposing the Plurilingual Adaptive Trade-off Hypothesis (PATH), according to which the adaptation of bilinguals' cognitive abilities may (i) decrease fallibility to illusions by means of recruiting sharpened top-down control processes, but (ii) this is part of a larger bundle of effects, not all of which are necessarily advantageous.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evelina Leivada
- Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona, Spain
- UiT-The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
| | | | - Marit Westergaard
- UiT-The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
- NTNU Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Chen L, Xu Q, Shen L, Yuan T, Wang Y, Zhou W, Jiang Y. Distinct Contributions of Genes and Environment to Visual Size Illusion and the Underlying Neural Mechanism. Cereb Cortex 2021; 32:1014-1023. [PMID: 34379728 PMCID: PMC8889949 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab262] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2021] [Revised: 06/29/2021] [Accepted: 06/30/2021] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
As exemplified by the Ebbinghaus illusion, the perceived size of an object can be significantly biased by its surrounding context. The phenomenon is experienced by humans as well as other species, hence likely evolutionarily adaptive. Here, we examined the heritability of the Ebbinghaus illusion using a combination of the classic twin method and multichannel functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Results show that genes account for over 50% of the variance in the strength of the experienced illusion. Interestingly, activations evoked by the Ebbinghaus stimuli in the early visual cortex are explained by genetic factors whereas those in the posterior temporal cortex are explained by environmental factors. In parallel, the feedforward functional connectivity between the occipital cortex and the temporal cortex is modulated by genetic effects whereas the feedback functional connectivity is entirely shaped by environment, despite both being significantly correlated with the strength of the experienced illusion. These findings demonstrate that genetic and environmental factors work in tandem to shape the context-dependent visual size illusion, and shed new light on the links among genes, environment, brain, and subjective experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lihong Chen
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, P.R. China.,Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian 116029, P.R. China.,State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, P.R. China
| | - Qian Xu
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, P.R. China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, P.R. China.,Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, P.R. China
| | - Li Shen
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, P.R. China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, P.R. China.,Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, P.R. China
| | - Tian Yuan
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, P.R. China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, P.R. China.,Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, P.R. China
| | - Ying Wang
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, P.R. China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, P.R. China.,Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, P.R. China
| | - Wen Zhou
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, P.R. China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, P.R. China.,Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, P.R. China
| | - Yi Jiang
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, P.R. China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, P.R. China.,Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, P.R. China.,Institute of Artificial Intelligence, Hefei Comprehensive National Science Center, Hefei 230088, P.R. China
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Lõoke M, Marinelli L, Guérineau C, Agrillo C, Mongillo P. Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) are susceptible to the Kanizsa's triangle illusion. Anim Cogn 2021; 25:43-51. [PMID: 34269930 PMCID: PMC8904331 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-021-01533-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2021] [Revised: 07/02/2021] [Accepted: 07/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The ability to complete partially missing contours is widespread across the animal kingdom, but whether this extends to dogs is still unknown. To address this gap in knowledge, we assessed dogs' susceptibility to one of the most common contour illusions, the Kanizsa's triangle. Six dogs were trained to discriminate a triangle from other geometrical figures using a two-alternative conditioned discrimination task. Once the learning criterion was reached, dogs were presented with the Kanizsa's triangle and a control stimulus, where inducers were rotated around their centre, so as to disrupt what would be perceived as a triangle by a human observer. As a group, dogs chose the illusory triangle significantly more often than control stimuli. At the individual level, susceptibility to the illusion was shown by five out of six dogs. This is the first study where dogs as a group show susceptibility to a visual illusion in the same manner as humans. Moreover, the analyses revealed a negative effect of age on susceptibility, an effect that was also found in humans. Altogether, this suggests that the underling perceptual mechanisms are similar between dogs and humans, and in sharp contrast with other categories of visual illusions to which the susceptibility of dogs has been previously assessed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miina Lõoke
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Piazzetta del Donatore, 4, 35020, Legnaro, Italy
| | - Lieta Marinelli
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Piazzetta del Donatore, 4, 35020, Legnaro, Italy.
| | - Cécile Guérineau
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Piazzetta del Donatore, 4, 35020, Legnaro, Italy
| | - Christian Agrillo
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, 35131, Padua, Italy.,Padua Neuroscience Center, University of Padua, 35131, Padua, Italy
| | - Paolo Mongillo
- Laboratory of Applied Ethology, Department of Comparative Biomedicine and Food Science, University of Padua, Piazzetta del Donatore, 4, 35020, Legnaro, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Costa ALL, Silva RC, Coelho-Cordeiro PH, da Silveira FS, Barros M, Caixeta FV, Maior RS. The Perception of the Müller-Lyer Visual Illusion in Schizophrenics and Non-human Primates: A Translational Approach. Front Behav Neurosci 2021; 15:641776. [PMID: 34122024 PMCID: PMC8192968 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.641776] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2020] [Accepted: 04/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The Müller-Lyer Illusion (MLI) has been suggested as a potential marker for the perceptual impairments observed in schizophrenia patients. Along with some positive symptoms, these deficits are not easily modeled in rodent experiments, and novel animal models are warranted. Previously, MK-801 was shown to reduce susceptibility to MLI in monkeys, raising the prospects of an effective perception-based model. Here, we evaluate the translational feasibility of the MLI task under NMDA receptor blockage as a primate model for schizophrenia. In Experiment 1, eight capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) were trained on a touchscreen MLI task. Upon reaching the learning criteria, the monkeys were given ketamine (0.3 mg/kg; i.m.) or saline on four consecutive days and then retested on the MLI task. In Experiment 2, eight chronic schizophrenia patients (and eight matching controls) were tested on the Brentano version of the MLI. Under saline treatment, monkeys were susceptible to MLI, similarly to healthy human participants. Repeated ketamine administrations, however, failed to improve their performance as previous results with MK-801 had shown. Schizophrenic patients, on the other hand, showed a higher susceptibility to MLI when compared to healthy controls. In light of the present and previous studies, the MLI task shows consistent results across monkeys and humans. In spite of potentially being an interesting translational model of schizophrenia, the MLI task warrants further refinement in non-human primates and a broader sample of schizophrenia subtypes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ana Luísa Lamounier Costa
- Primate Center, Institute of Biology, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil.,Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil
| | - Ronaldo Coelho Silva
- Primate Center, Institute of Biology, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil.,Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil
| | - Pedro H Coelho-Cordeiro
- Primate Center, Institute of Biology, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil.,Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil
| | - Fernando Silva da Silveira
- Primate Center, Institute of Biology, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil.,Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil
| | - Marilia Barros
- Primate Center, Institute of Biology, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil.,Department of Pharmacy, School of Health Sciences, University of Brasilia, Brasilia, Brazil
| | - Fabio Viegas Caixeta
- Primate Center, Institute of Biology, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil.,Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil
| | - Rafael S Maior
- Primate Center, Institute of Biology, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil.,Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Brasília, Brasilia, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Geers L, Pesenti M, Derosiere G, Duque J, Dricot L, Andres M. Role of the fronto-parietal cortex in prospective action judgments. Sci Rep 2021; 11:7454. [PMID: 33811223 PMCID: PMC8018944 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-86719-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2020] [Accepted: 03/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Prospective judgments about one's capability to perform an action are assumed to involve mental simulation of the action. Previous studies of motor imagery suggest this simulation is supported by a large fronto-parietal network including the motor system. Experiment 1 used fMRI to assess the contribution of this fronto-parietal network to judgments about one's capacity to grasp objects of different sizes between index and thumb. The neural network underlying prospective graspability judgments overlapped the fronto-parietal network involved in explicit motor imagery of grasping. However, shared areas were located in the right hemisphere, outside the motor cortex, and were also activated during perceptual length judgments, suggesting a contribution to object size estimate rather than motor simulation. Experiment 2 used TMS over the motor cortex to probe transient excitability changes undetected with fMRI. Results show that graspability judgments elicited a selective increase of excitability in the thumb and index muscles, which was maximal before the object display and intermediate during the judgment. Together, these findings suggest that prospective action judgments do not rely on the motor system to simulate the action per se but to refresh the memory of one's maximal grip aperture and facilitate its comparison with object size in right fronto-parietal areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laurie Geers
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Mauro Pesenti
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.,Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Gerard Derosiere
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Julie Duque
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Laurence Dricot
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Michael Andres
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. .,Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, Brussels, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Bunce C, Gray KLH, Cook R. The perception of interpersonal distance is distorted by the Müller-Lyer illusion. Sci Rep 2021; 11:494. [PMID: 33436801 PMCID: PMC7803751 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-80073-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2020] [Accepted: 12/14/2020] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
There is growing interest in how human observers perceive social scenes containing multiple people. Interpersonal distance is a critical feature when appraising these scenes; proxemic cues are used by observers to infer whether two people are interacting, the nature of their relationship, and the valence of their current interaction. Presently, however, remarkably little is known about how interpersonal distance is encoded within the human visual system. Here we show that the perception of interpersonal distance is distorted by the Müller-Lyer illusion. Participants perceived the distance between two target points to be compressed or expanded depending on whether face pairs were positioned inside or outside the to-be-judged interval. This illusory bias was found to be unaffected by manipulations of face direction. These findings aid our understanding of how human observers perceive interpersonal distance and may inform theoretical accounts of the Müller-Lyer illusion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carl Bunce
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London, WC1E7HX, UK
| | - Katie L H Gray
- School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
| | - Richard Cook
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London, WC1E7HX, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Chen L, Wu B, Qiao C, Liu DQ. Resting EEG in alpha band predicts individual differences in visual size perception. Brain Cogn 2020; 145:105625. [PMID: 32932108 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105625] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2020] [Revised: 08/04/2020] [Accepted: 09/02/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Human visual size perception results from an interaction of external sensory information and internal state. The cognitive mechanisms involved in the processing of context-dependent visual size perception have been found to be innate in nature to some extent, suggesting that visual size perception might correlate with human intrinsic brain activity. Here we recorded human resting alpha activity (8-12 Hz), which is an inverse indicator of sustained alertness. Moreover, we measured an object's perceived size in a two-alternative forced-choice manner and the Ebbinghaus illusion magnitude which is a classic illustration of context-dependent visual size perception. The results showed that alpha activity along the ventral visual pathway, including left V1, right LOC and bilateral inferior temporal gyrus, negatively correlated with an object's perceived size. Moreover, alpha activity in the left superior temporal gyrus positively correlated with size discrimination threshold and size illusion magnitude. The findings provide clear evidence that human visual size perception scales as a function of intrinsic alertness, with higher alertness linking to larger perceived size of objects and better performance in size discrimination and size illusion tasks, and suggest that individual variation in resting-state brain activity provides a neural explanation for individual variation in cognitive performance of normal participants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lihong Chen
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, PR China; Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian, PR China.
| | - Baoyu Wu
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, PR China; Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian, PR China
| | - Congying Qiao
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, PR China; Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian, PR China
| | - Dong-Qiang Liu
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, PR China; Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian, PR China.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Whitwell RL, Sperandio I, Buckingham G, Chouinard PA, Goodale MA. Grip Constancy but Not Perceptual Size Constancy Survives Lesions of Early Visual Cortex. Curr Biol 2020; 30:3680-3686.e5. [PMID: 32735814 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2020] [Revised: 06/27/2020] [Accepted: 07/08/2020] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
Object constancies are central constructs in theories of visual phenomenology. A powerful example is "size constancy," in which the perceived size of an object remains stable despite changes in viewing distance [1-4]. Evidence from neuropsychology [5], neuroimaging [6-11], transcranial magnetic stimulation [12, 13], single-unit and lesion studies in monkey [14-20], and computational modeling [21] suggests that re-entrant processes involving reciprocal interactions between primary visual cortex (V1) and extrastriate visual areas [22-26] play an essential role in mediating size constancy. It is seldom appreciated, however, that object constancies must also operate for the visual guidance of goal-directed action. For example, when reaching out to pick up an object, the hand's in-flight aperture scales with size of the goal object [27-30] and is refractory to the decrease in retinal-image size with increased viewing distance [31-41] (Figure 1), a phenomenon we call "grip constancy." Does grip constancy, like perceptual constancy, depend on V1 or can it be mediated by pathways that bypass it altogether? We tested these possibilities in an individual, M.C., who has bilateral lesions encompassing V1 and much of the ventral visual stream. We show that her perceptual estimates of object size co-vary with retinal-image size rather than real-world size as viewing distance varies. In contrast, M.C. shows near-normal scaling of in-flight grasp aperture to object size despite changes in viewing distance. Thus, although early visual cortex is necessary for perceptual object constancy, it is unnecessary for grip constancy, which is mediated instead by separate visual inputs to dorsal-stream visuomotor areas [42-48].
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert L Whitwell
- Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada.
| | - Irene Sperandio
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto 38068, Italy
| | - Gavin Buckingham
- Department of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter EX1 2LU, UK
| | - Philippe A Chouinard
- Department of Psychology and Counselling, La Trobe University, Bendigo 3550, Australia
| | - Melvyn A Goodale
- Brain and Mind Institute, Department of Psychology, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON N6A 5C2, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Visual Size Processing in Early Visual Cortex Follows Lateral Occipital Cortex Involvement. J Neurosci 2020; 40:4410-4417. [PMID: 32350038 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2437-19.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2019] [Revised: 04/06/2020] [Accepted: 04/06/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Neural activation in the early visual cortex (EVC) reflects the perceived rather than retinal size of stimuli, suggesting that feedback possibly from extrastriate regions modulates retinal size information in EVC. Meanwhile, the lateral occipital cortex (LOC) has been suggested to be critically involved in object size processing. To test for the potential contributions of feedback modulations on size representations in EVC, we investigated the dynamics of relevant processes using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Specifically, we briefly disrupted the neural activity of EVC and LOC at early, intermediate, and late time windows while participants performed size judgment tasks in either an illusory or neutral context. TMS over EVC and LOC allowed determining whether these two brain regions are relevant for generating phenomenological size impressions. Furthermore, the temporal order of TMS effects allowed inferences on the dynamics of information exchange between the two areas. Particularly, if feedback signals from LOC to EVC are crucial for generating altered size representations in EVC, then TMS effects over EVC should be observed simultaneously or later than the effects following LOC stimulation. The data from 20 humans (13 females) revealed that TMS over both EVC and LOC impaired illusory size perception. However, the strongest effects of TMS applied over EVC occurred later than those of LOC, supporting a functionally relevant feedback modulation from LOC to EVC for scaling size information. Our results suggest that context integration and the concomitant change of perceived size require LOC and result in modulating representations in EVC via recurrent processing.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT How we perceive an object's size is not entirely determined by its physical size or the size of its retinal representation but also the spatial context. Using transcranial magnetic stimulation, we investigated the role of the early visual cortex (EVC) and the higher-level visual area, lateral occipital cortex (LOC), known to be critically involved in object processing, in transforming an initial retinal representation into one that reflects perceived size. Transcranial magnetic stimulation altered size perception earlier over LOC compared with EVC, suggesting that context integration and the concomitant change in perceived size representations in EVC rely on feedback from LOC.
Collapse
|
17
|
Tracking the completion of parts into whole objects: Retinotopic activation in response to illusory figures in the lateral occipital complex. Neuroimage 2020; 207:116426. [PMID: 31794856 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116426] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2019] [Revised: 11/26/2019] [Accepted: 11/30/2019] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Illusory figures demonstrate the visual system's ability to integrate separate parts into coherent, whole objects. The present study was performed to track the neuronal object construction process in human observers, by incrementally manipulating the grouping strength within a given configuration until the emergence of a whole-object representation. Two tasks were employed: First, in the spatial localization task, object completion could facilitate performance and was task-relevant, whereas it was irrelevant in the second, luminance discrimination task. Concurrent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) used spatial localizers to locate brain regions representing task-critical illusory-figure parts to investigate whether the step-wise object construction process would modulate neural activity in these localized brain regions. The results revealed that both V1 and the lateral occipital complex (LOC, with sub-regions LO1 and LO2) were involved in Kanizsa figure processing. However, completion-specific activations were found predominantly in LOC, where neural activity exhibited a modulation in accord with the configuration's grouping strength, whether or not the configuration was relevant to performing the task at hand. Moreover, right LOC activations were confined to LO2 and responded primarily to surface and shape completions, whereas left LOC exhibited activations in both LO1 and LO2 and was related to encoding shape structures with more detail. Together, these results demonstrate that various grouping properties within a visual scene are integrated automatically in LOC, with sub-regions located in different hemispheres specializing in the component sub-processes that render completed objects.
Collapse
|
18
|
Everything is subjective under water surface, too: visual illusions in fish. Anim Cogn 2020; 23:251-264. [PMID: 31897795 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-019-01341-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2019] [Revised: 12/09/2019] [Accepted: 12/13/2019] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
The study of visual illusions has captured the attention of comparative psychologists since the last century, given the unquestionable advantage of investigating complex perceptual mechanisms with relatively simple visual patterns. To date, the observation of animal behavior in the presence of visual illusions has been largely confined to mammal and bird studies. Recently, there has been increasing interest in investigating fish, too. The attention has been particularly focused on guppies, redtail splitfin and bamboo sharks. Overall, the tested species were shown to experience a human-like perception of different illusory phenomena involving size, number, motion, brightness estimation and illusory contours. However, in some cases, no illusory effects, or evidence for a reverse illusion, were also reported. Here, we review the current state of the art in this field. We conclude that a wider investigation of visual illusions in fish is fundamental to form a broader comprehension of perceptual systems of vertebrates. Furthermore, we believe that this type of investigation could help us to address general important issues in perceptual studies, such as the role of ecology in shaping perceptual systems, the existence of interindividual variability in the visual perception of nonhuman species and the role of cortical activity in the emergence of visual illusions.
Collapse
|
19
|
|
20
|
Cermeño-Aínsa S. The cognitive penetrability of perception: A blocked debate and a tentative solution. Conscious Cogn 2019; 77:102838. [PMID: 31678779 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102838] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2019] [Revised: 10/03/2019] [Accepted: 10/12/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Despite the extensive body of psychological findings suggesting that cognition influences perception, the debate between defenders and detractors of the cognitive penetrability of perception persists. While detractors demand more strictness in psychological experiments, proponents consider that empirical studies show that cognitive penetrability occurs. These considerations have led some theorists to propose that the debate has reached a dead end. The issue about where perception ends and cognition begins is, I argue, one of the reasons why the debate is cornered. Another reason is the inability of psychological studies to present uncontroversial interpretations of the results obtained. To dive into other kinds of empirical sources is, therefore, required to clarify the debate. In this paper, I explain where the debate is blocked, and suggest that neuroscientific evidence together with the predictive coding account, might decant the discussion on the side of the penetrability thesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sergio Cermeño-Aínsa
- Departamento de Filosofía, Facultad de Filosofía y Letras, 08193 Cerdanyola del Vallés, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Evidence of top-down modulation of the Brentano illusion but not of the glare effect by transcranial direct current stimulation. Exp Brain Res 2019; 237:2111-2121. [PMID: 31190083 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-019-05577-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2018] [Accepted: 06/06/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been widely used for modulating sensory, motor and cognitive functions, but there are only few attempts to induce and change illusory perception. Visual illusions have been the most traditional and effective way to investigate visual processing through the comparison between physical reality and subjective reports. Here we used tDCS to modulate two different visual illusions, namely the Brentano illusion and the glare effect, with the aim of uncovering the influence of top-down mechanisms on bottom-up visual perception in two experiments. In Experiment 1, to a first group of subjects, real and sham cathodal tDCS (2 mA, 10 min) were applied over the left and right posterior parietal cortices (PPC). In Experiment 2, real and sham cathodal tDCS were applied to the left and right occipital cortices (OC) to a second group of participants. Results showed that tDCS was effective in modulating only the Brentano illusion, but not the glare effect. tDCS increased the Brentano illusion but specifically for the stimulated cortical area (right PPC), illusion direction (leftward), visual hemispace (left), and illusion length (160 mm). These findings suggest the existence of an inhibitory modulation of top-down mechanisms on bottom-up visual processing specifically for the Brentano illusion, but not for the glare effect. The lack of effect of occipital tDCS should consider the possible role of ocular compensation or of the unstimulated hemisphere, which deserves further investigations.
Collapse
|
22
|
Beware the Alar Base Optical Illusion in Assessment of Unilateral Cleft Lip Nasal Deformity. Plast Reconstr Surg 2019; 143:1157-1162. [PMID: 30921137 DOI: 10.1097/prs.0000000000005480] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND After repair of unilateral cleft lip, lateral drift of the alar base is expected and may require revision. Although the alar base appears wider than on the noncleft side, by measurement it could be symmetric or narrow. Is this an optical illusion? METHODS This is a retrospective photographic evaluation of children who underwent repair of unilateral complete cleft lip and palate. On submental standardized photographs, raters judged whether the alar base position was wider, equal to, or narrower than on the noncleft side. The true alar base position was measured by photogrammetry and the frequency of alar base misjudgment was assessed. On control photographs, the cleft side alar base was wider than the noncleft side. On test photographs, the cleft side alar base was narrower than the noncleft side. Possible association between misjudgment and nasal features was also evaluated. Data were analyzed using the Fisher's exact test. RESULTS Three raters assessed 61 patients aged 6 to 9 years and treated between 1995 and 2015. In 77 percent of control photographs, raters correctly judged the cleft side alar base as wider. In 65 percent of test photographs, raters incorrectly judged the alar base position as wider on the cleft side. Asymmetry of nostril outline and a transverse nostril axis were associated with misjudged photographs (p < 0.05). CONCLUSIONS Alar base assessment can be altered by an optical illusion. Simple anthropometry counters this misperception. Measurement should be repeated after correction of the deviated anterocaudal septum and elevation of the lower lateral cartilage. Thereafter, a decision is made on altering the alar base position. CLINICAL QUESTION/LEVEL OF EVIDENCE Diagnostic, II.
Collapse
|
23
|
Geers L, Pesenti M, Andres M. Visual illusions modify object size estimates for prospective action judgements. Neuropsychologia 2018; 117:211-221. [PMID: 29883576 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2018] [Revised: 05/16/2018] [Accepted: 06/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
How does the eye guide the hand in an ever-changing world? The perception-action model posits that visually-guided actions rely on object size estimates that are computed from an egocentric perspective independently of the visual context. Accordingly, adjusting grip aperture to object size should be resistant to illusions emerging from the contrast between a target and surrounding elements. However, experimental studies gave discrepant results that have remained difficult to explain so far. Visual and proprioceptive information of the acting hand are potential sources of ambiguity in previous studies because the on-line corrections they allow may contribute to masking the illusory effect. To overcome this problem, we investigated the effect on prospective action judgements of the Ebbinghaus illusion, a visual illusion in which the perceived size of a central circle varies according to the size of surrounding circles. Participants had to decide whether they thought they would be able to grasp the central circle of an Ebbinghaus display between their index finger and thumb, without moving their hands. A control group had to judge the size of the central circle relative to a standard. Experiment 1 showed that the illusion affected perceptual and grasping judgements similarly. We further investigated the interaction between visual illusions and grip aperture representation by examining the effect of concurrent motor tasks on grasping judgements. We showed that participants underestimated their ability to grasp the circle when they were squeezing a ball between their index finger and thumb (Experiment 2), whereas they overestimated their ability when their fingers were spread apart (Experiment 3). The illusion also affected the grasping judgement task and modulated the interference of the squeezing movement, with the illusion of largeness enhancing the underestimation of one's grasping ability observed in Experiment 2. We conclude that visual context and body posture both influence action anticipation, and that perception and action support each other.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laurie Geers
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
| | - Mauro Pesenti
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, Brussels, Belgium.
| | - Michael Andres
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Avenue Mounier 53, Brussels, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
More far is more right: Manual and ocular line bisections, but not the Judd illusion, depend on radial space. Brain Cogn 2018; 122:34-44. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.01.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2017] [Revised: 10/19/2017] [Accepted: 01/25/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
25
|
Medendorp WP, de Brouwer AJ, Smeets JB. Dynamic representations of visual space for perception and action. Cortex 2018; 98:194-202. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2016] [Revised: 09/06/2016] [Accepted: 11/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
26
|
Dormal V, Larigaldie N, Lefèvre N, Pesenti M, Andres M. Effect of perceived length on numerosity estimation: Evidence from the Müller-Lyer illusion. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2018; 71:2142-2151. [DOI: 10.1177/1747021817738720] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies showed that the magnitude information conveyed by sensory cues, such as length or surface, influences the ability to compare the numerosity of sets of objects. However, the perceptual nature of this representation and how it interacts with the processes involved in numerical judgements remain unclear. This study aims to address these issues by studying the interference of length on numerosity under different perceptual and response conditions. The first experiment shows that the influence of length does not depend on the actual length but on subjective values reflecting the way length is perceived in a given visual context. The Müller-Lyer illusion was used to manipulate the perceived length of two dot arrays independently of their actual length. When the length of two dot arrays was equal but perceived as different due to the illusion, participants erroneously reported differences in the number of dots contained in each array, evidencing a similar effect of Müller-Lyer illusion on length and numerosity comparison. This finding was replicated in a second experiment where participants had to give a verbal estimate of the number of dots contained in a given array, thereby eliminating the choice between a small or large response. Compared with a neutral condition, estimations were systematically larger than the actual number of dots as the illusory length increased. These results demonstrate that the illusory-induced experience of length influences numerosity estimation over and beyond objective cues and that this influence is not a response selection bias.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valérie Dormal
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Nathanaël Larigaldie
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Nathalie Lefèvre
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- Institute for Multidisciplinary Research in Quantitative Modelling and Analysis, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Mauro Pesenti
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Michael Andres
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Bertulis A, Surkys T, Bielevičius A. Summation of two illusions of extent. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 2018. [DOI: 10.21307/ane-2018-014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
28
|
Top-down Approach to the Investigation of the Neural Basis of Geometric-optical Illusions: Understanding the Brain as a Theoretical Entity. ARCHIVES OF NEUROSCIENCE 2017. [DOI: 10.5812/archneurosci.33683] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
29
|
Perceptual similarity and the neural correlates of geometrical illusions in human brain structure. Sci Rep 2017; 7:39968. [PMID: 28067329 PMCID: PMC5220349 DOI: 10.1038/srep39968] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2016] [Accepted: 11/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Geometrical visual illusions are an intriguing phenomenon, in which subjective perception consistently misjudges the objective, physical properties of the visual stimulus. Prominent theoretical proposals have been advanced attempting to find common mechanisms across illusions. But empirically testing the similarity between illusions has been notoriously difficult because illusions have very different visual appearances. Here we overcome this difficulty by capitalizing on the variability of the illusory magnitude across participants. Fifty-nine healthy volunteers participated in the study that included measurement of individual illusion magnitude and structural MRI scanning. We tested the Muller-Lyer, Ebbinghaus, Ponzo, and vertical-horizontal geometrical illusions as well as a non-geometrical, contrast illusion. We found some degree of similarity in behavioral judgments of all tested geometrical illusions, but not between geometrical illusions and non-geometrical, contrast illusion. The highest similarity was found between Ebbinghaus and Muller-Lyer geometrical illusions. Furthermore, the magnitude of all geometrical illusions, and particularly the Ebbinghaus and Muller-Lyer illusions, correlated with local gray matter density in the parahippocampal cortex, but not in other brain areas. Our findings suggest that visuospatial integration and scene construction processes might partly mediate individual differences in geometric illusory perception. Overall, these findings contribute to a better understanding of the mechanisms behind geometrical illusions.
Collapse
|
30
|
Mikellidou K, Gouws AD, Clawson H, Thompson P, Morland AB, Keefe BD. An Orientation Dependent Size Illusion Is Underpinned by Processing in the Extrastriate Visual Area, LO1. Iperception 2016; 7:2041669516667628. [PMID: 27733896 PMCID: PMC5040199 DOI: 10.1177/2041669516667628] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
We use the simple, but prominent Helmholtz's squares illusion in which a vertically striped square appears wider than a horizontally striped square of identical physical dimensions to determine whether functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) BOLD responses in V1 underpin illusions of size. We report that these simple stimuli which differ in only one parameter, orientation, to which V1 neurons are highly selective elicited activity in V1 that followed their physical, not perceived size. To further probe the role of V1 in the illusion and investigate plausible extrastriate visual areas responsible for eliciting the Helmholtz squares illusion, we performed a follow-up transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) experiment in which we compared perceptual judgments about the aspect ratio of perceptually identical Helmholtz squares when no TMS was applied against selective stimulation of V1, LO1, or LO2. In agreement with fMRI results, we report that TMS of area V1 does not compromise the strength of the illusion. Only stimulation of area LO1, and not LO2, compromised significantly the strength of the illusion, consistent with previous research that LO1 plays a role in the processing of orientation information. These results demonstrate the involvement of a specific extrastriate area in an illusory percept of size.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyriaki Mikellidou
- Department of Psychology, University of York, UK; University of Pisa, Italy
| | - André D Gouws
- York Neuroimaging Centre, Department of Psychology, University of York, UK
| | | | | | - Antony B Morland
- York Neuroimaging Centre, Department of Psychology, University of York, UK; Centre for Neuroscience, Hull-York Medical School, UK
| | - Bruce D Keefe
- York Neuroimaging Centre, Department of Psychology, University of York, UK
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Zimmermann E, Morrone MC, Burr D. Adaptation to size affects saccades with long but not short latencies. J Vis 2016; 16:2. [PMID: 27139583 PMCID: PMC5053361 DOI: 10.1167/16.7.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Maintained exposure to a specific stimulus property-such as size, color, or motion-induces perceptual adaptation aftereffects, usually in the opposite direction to that of the adaptor. Here we studied how adaptation to size affects perceived position and visually guided action (saccadic eye movements) to that position. Subjects saccaded to the border of a diamond-shaped object after adaptation to a smaller diamond shape. For saccades in the normal latency range, amplitudes decreased, consistent with saccading to a larger object. Short-latency saccades, however, tended to be affected less by the adaptation, suggesting that they were only partly triggered by a signal representing the illusory target position. We also tested size perception after adaptation, followed by a mask stimulus at the probe location after various delays. Similar size adaptation magnitudes were found for all probe-mask delays. In agreement with earlier studies, these results suggest that the duration of the saccade latency period determines the reference frame that codes the probe location.
Collapse
|
32
|
Shen L, Zhang M, Chen Q. The Poggendorff illusion driven by real and illusory contour: Behavioral and neural mechanisms. Neuropsychologia 2016; 85:24-34. [PMID: 26956926 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2015] [Revised: 02/06/2016] [Accepted: 03/04/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
The Poggendorff illusion refers to the phenomenon that the human brain misperceives a diagonal line as being apparently misaligned once the diagonal line is interrupted by two parallel edges, and the size of illusion is negatively correlated with the angle of interception of the oblique, i.e. the sharper the oblique angle, the larger the illusion. This optical illusion can be produced by both real and illusory contour. In this fMRI study, by parametrically varying the oblique angle, we investigated the shared and specific neural mechanisms underlying the Poggendorff illusion induced by real and illusory contour. At the behavioral level, not only the real but also the illusory contours were capable of inducing significant Poggendorff illusion. The size of illusion induced by the real contour, however, was larger than that induced by the illusory contour. At the neural level, real and illusory contours commonly activated more dorsal visual areas, and the real contours specifically activated more ventral visual areas. More importantly, examinations on the parametric modulation effects of the size of illusion revealed the specific neural mechanisms underlying the Poggendorff illusion induced by the real and the illusory contours, respectively. Left precentral gyrus and right middle occipital cortex were specifically involved in the Poggendorff illusion induced by the real contour. On the other hand, bilateral intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and right lateral occipital complex (LOC) were specifically involved in the Poggendorff illusion induced by the illusory contour. Functional implications of the above findings were further discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lu Shen
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Ming Zhang
- School of Education, Soochow University, Suzhou 215123, China
| | - Qi Chen
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; Epilepsy Center, Guangdong 999 Brain Hospital, Guangzhou 510631, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China.
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Abstract
UNLABELLED The visual and haptic perceptual systems are understood to share a common neural representation of object shape. A region thought to be critical for recognizing visual and haptic shape information is the lateral occipital complex (LOC). We investigated whether LOC is essential for haptic shape recognition in humans by studying behavioral responses and brain activation for haptically explored objects in a patient (M.C.) with bilateral lesions of the occipitotemporal cortex, including LOC. Despite severe deficits in recognizing objects using vision, M.C. was able to accurately recognize objects via touch. M.C.'s psychophysical response profile to haptically explored shapes was also indistinguishable from controls. Using fMRI, M.C. showed no object-selective visual or haptic responses in LOC, but her pattern of haptic activation in other brain regions was remarkably similar to healthy controls. Although LOC is routinely active during visual and haptic shape recognition tasks, it is not essential for haptic recognition of object shape. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The lateral occipital complex (LOC) is a brain region regarded to be critical for recognizing object shape, both in vision and in touch. However, causal evidence linking LOC with haptic shape processing is lacking. We studied recognition performance, psychophysical sensitivity, and brain response to touched objects, in a patient (M.C.) with extensive lesions involving LOC bilaterally. Despite being severely impaired in visual shape recognition, M.C. was able to identify objects via touch and she showed normal sensitivity to a haptic shape illusion. M.C.'s brain response to touched objects in areas of undamaged cortex was also very similar to that observed in neurologically healthy controls. These results demonstrate that LOC is not necessary for recognizing objects via touch.
Collapse
|
34
|
de Brouwer AJ, Smeets JB, Gutteling TP, Toni I, Medendorp WP. The Müller-Lyer illusion affects visuomotor updating in the dorsal visual stream. Neuropsychologia 2015; 77:119-27. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2015] [Revised: 07/23/2015] [Accepted: 08/10/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
35
|
Sovrano VA, da Pos O, Albertazzi L. The Müller-Lyer illusion in the teleost fish Xenotoca eiseni. Anim Cogn 2015; 19:123-32. [PMID: 26362031 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-015-0917-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2015] [Revised: 08/18/2015] [Accepted: 08/24/2015] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
Abstract
In the Müller-Lyer illusion, human subjects usually see a line with two inducers at its ends facing outwards as longer than an identical line with inducers at its ends facing inwards. We investigate the tendency for fish to perceive, in suitable conditions, line length according to the Müller-Lyer illusion. Redtail splitfins (Xenotoca eiseni, family Goodeidae) were trained to discriminate between two lines of different length. After reaching the learning criterion, the fish performed test trials, in which they faced two lines (black or red) of identical length, differing only in the context in terms of arrangement of the inducers, which were positioned at the ends of the line, either inward, outward, or perpendicular. Fish chose the stimulus that appear to humans as either longer or shorter, in accordance with the prediction of the Müller-Lyer illusion, consistently with the condition of the training. These results show that redtail splitfins tend to be subject to this particular illusion. The results of the study are discussed with reference to similar studies concerning the same illusion as recently observed in fish. Contrasting results are presented. The significance of the results in light of their possible evolutionary implications is also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valeria Anna Sovrano
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, P.zza Manifattura 1 and C.so Bettini 31, 38068, Rovereto, Trento, Italy.
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, University of Trento, C.so Bettini 31, 38068, Rovereto, Trento, Italy.
| | - Osvaldo da Pos
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Via Venezia 8, 35131, Padua, Italy.
| | - Liliana Albertazzi
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, P.zza Manifattura 1 and C.so Bettini 31, 38068, Rovereto, Trento, Italy.
- Department of Humanities, University of Trento, Via Tommaso Gar 14, 38122, Trento, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Kreutzer S, Weidner R, Fink GR. Rescaling Retinal Size into Perceived Size: Evidence for an Occipital and Parietal Bottleneck. J Cogn Neurosci 2015; 27:1334-43. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00784] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
The spatial and temporal context of an object influence its perceived size. Two visual illusions illustrate this nicely: the size adaptation effect and the Ebbinghaus illusion. Whereas size adaptation affects size rescaling of a target circle via a previously presented, differently sized adaptor circle, the Ebbinghaus illusion alters perceived size by virtue of surrounding circles. In the classical Ebbinghaus setting, the surrounding circles are shown simultaneously with the target. However, size underestimation persists when the surrounding circles precede the target. Such a temporal separation of inducer and target circles in both illusions permits the comparison of BOLD signals elicited by two displays that, although objectively identical, elicit different percepts. The current study combined both illusions in a factorial design to identify a presumed common central mechanism involved in rescaling retinal into perceived size. At the behavioral level, combining both illusions did not affect perceived size further. At the neural level, however, this combination induced functional activation beyond that induced by either illusion separately: An underadditive activation pattern was found within left lingual gyrus, right supramarginal gyrus, and right superior parietal cortex. These findings provide direct behavioral and functional evidence for the presence of a neural bottleneck in rescaling retinal into perceived size, a process vital for visual perception.
Collapse
|
37
|
Tabei KI, Satoh M, Kida H, Kizaki M, Sakuma H, Sakuma H, Tomimoto H. Involvement of the Extrageniculate System in the Perception of Optical Illusions: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0128750. [PMID: 26083375 PMCID: PMC4470923 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128750] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2014] [Accepted: 05/01/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Research on the neural processing of optical illusions can provide clues for understanding the neural mechanisms underlying visual perception. Previous studies have shown that some visual areas contribute to the perception of optical illusions such as the Kanizsa triangle and Müller-Lyer figure; however, the neural mechanisms underlying the processing of these and other optical illusions have not been clearly identified. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we determined which brain regions are active during the perception of optical illusions. For our study, we enrolled 18 participants. The illusory optical stimuli consisted of many kana letters, which are Japanese phonograms. During the shape task, participants stated aloud whether they perceived the shapes of two optical illusions as being the same or not. During the word task, participants read aloud the kana letters in the stimuli. A direct comparison between the shape and word tasks showed activation of the right inferior frontal gyrus, left medial frontal gyrus, and right pulvinar. It is well known that there are two visual pathways, the geniculate and extrageniculate systems, which belong to the higher-level and primary visual systems, respectively. The pulvinar belongs to the latter system, and the findings of the present study suggest that the extrageniculate system is involved in the cognitive processing of optical illusions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ken-ichi Tabei
- Department of Dementia Prevention and Therapeutics, Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan
- Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan
- * E-mail:
| | - Masayuki Satoh
- Department of Dementia Prevention and Therapeutics, Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan
| | - Hirotaka Kida
- Department of Dementia Prevention and Therapeutics, Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan
| | | | | | - Hajime Sakuma
- Department of Radiology, Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan
| | - Hidekazu Tomimoto
- Department of Dementia Prevention and Therapeutics, Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan
- Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
García-Garibay OB, de Lafuente V. The Müller-Lyer illusion as seen by an artificial neural network. Front Comput Neurosci 2015; 9:21. [PMID: 25745398 PMCID: PMC4333816 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2015.00021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2014] [Accepted: 02/04/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
39
|
Zeman A, Obst O, Brooks KR. Complex cells decrease errors for the Müller-Lyer illusion in a model of the visual ventral stream. Front Comput Neurosci 2014; 8:112. [PMID: 25309411 PMCID: PMC4173309 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00112] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2014] [Accepted: 08/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
To improve robustness in object recognition, many artificial visual systems imitate the way in which the human visual cortex encodes object information as a hierarchical set of features. These systems are usually evaluated in terms of their ability to accurately categorize well-defined, unambiguous objects and scenes. In the real world, however, not all objects and scenes are presented clearly, with well-defined labels and interpretations. Visual illusions demonstrate a disparity between perception and objective reality, allowing psychophysicists to methodically manipulate stimuli and study our interpretation of the environment. One prominent effect, the Müller-Lyer illusion, is demonstrated when the perceived length of a line is contracted (or expanded) by the addition of arrowheads (or arrow-tails) to its ends. HMAX, a benchmark object recognition system, consistently produces a bias when classifying Müller-Lyer images. HMAX is a hierarchical, artificial neural network that imitates the “simple” and “complex” cell layers found in the visual ventral stream. In this study, we perform two experiments to explore the Müller-Lyer illusion in HMAX, asking: (1) How do simple vs. complex cell operations within HMAX affect illusory bias and precision? (2) How does varying the position of the figures in the input image affect classification using HMAX? In our first experiment, we assessed classification after traversing each layer of HMAX and found that in general, kernel operations performed by simple cells increase bias and uncertainty while max-pooling operations executed by complex cells decrease bias and uncertainty. In our second experiment, we increased variation in the positions of figures in the input images that reduced bias and uncertainty in HMAX. Our findings suggest that the Müller-Lyer illusion is exacerbated by the vulnerability of simple cell operations to positional fluctuations, but ameliorated by the robustness of complex cell responses to such variance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Astrid Zeman
- Department of Cognitive Science, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders (CCD), Macquarie University Sydney, NSW, Australia ; Digital Productivity and Services Flagship (DPAS), Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation Marsfield, NSW, Australia ; Perception in Action Research Centre, Macquarie University Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Oliver Obst
- Digital Productivity and Services Flagship (DPAS), Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation Marsfield, NSW, Australia
| | - Kevin R Brooks
- Perception in Action Research Centre, Macquarie University Sydney, NSW, Australia ; Department of Psychology, Macquarie University Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Weidner R, Plewan T, Chen Q, Buchner A, Weiss PH, Fink GR. The Moon Illusion and Size–Distance Scaling—Evidence for Shared Neural Patterns. J Cogn Neurosci 2014; 26:1871-82. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00590] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
A moon near to the horizon is perceived larger than a moon at the zenith, although—obviously—the moon does not change its size. In this study, the neural mechanisms underlying the “moon illusion” were investigated using a virtual 3-D environment and fMRI. Illusory perception of an increased moon size was associated with increased neural activity in ventral visual pathway areas including the lingual and fusiform gyri. The functional role of these areas was further explored in a second experiment. Left V3v was found to be involved in integrating retinal size and distance information, thus indicating that the brain regions that dynamically integrate retinal size and distance play a key role in generating the moon illusion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Thorsten Plewan
- 1Research Centre Jülich
- 2Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors (IfADo), Dortmund, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Testing day: The effects of processing bias induced by Navon stimuli on the strength of the Müller-Lyer illusion. Adv Cogn Psychol 2014; 10:9-14. [PMID: 24855498 PMCID: PMC3996713 DOI: 10.2478/v10053-008-0151-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2013] [Accepted: 10/14/2013] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Explanations for the cognitive basis of the Müller-Lyer illusion are still
frustratingly mixed. To date, Day’s (1989)
theory of perceptual compromise has received little empirical attention. In this
study, we examine the merit of Day’s hypothesis for the Müller-Lyer illusion by
biasing participants toward global or local visual processing through exposure
to Navon (1977) stimuli, which are known
to alter processing level preference for a short time. Participants
(N = 306) were randomly allocated to global, local, or
control conditions. Those in global or local conditions were exposed to Navon
stimuli for 5 min and participants were required to report on the global or
local stimulus features, respectively. Subsequently, participants completed a
computerized Müller-Lyer experiment where they adjusted the length of a line to
match an illusory-figure. The illusion was significantly stronger for
participants with a global bias, and significantly weaker for those with a local
bias, compared with the control condition. These findings provide empirical
support for Day’s “conflicting cues” theory of perceptual compromise in the
Müller-Lyer illusion.
Collapse
|
42
|
Kelley LA, Kelley JL. Animal visual illusion and confusion: the importance of a perceptual perspective. Behav Ecol 2013. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/art118] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
43
|
|
44
|
Chouinard PA, Noulty WA, Sperandio I, Landry O. Global processing during the Müller-Lyer illusion is distinctively affected by the degree of autistic traits in the typical population. Exp Brain Res 2013; 230:219-31. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3646-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2012] [Accepted: 07/05/2013] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
45
|
Zeman A, Obst O, Brooks KR, Rich AN. The Müller-Lyer Illusion in a computational model of biological object recognition. PLoS One 2013; 8:e56126. [PMID: 23457510 PMCID: PMC3574021 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2012] [Accepted: 01/04/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Studying illusions provides insight into the way the brain processes information. The Müller-Lyer Illusion (MLI) is a classical geometrical illusion of size, in which perceived line length is decreased by arrowheads and increased by arrowtails. Many theories have been put forward to explain the MLI, such as misapplied size constancy scaling, the statistics of image-source relationships and the filtering properties of signal processing in primary visual areas. Artificial models of the ventral visual processing stream allow us to isolate factors hypothesised to cause the illusion and test how these affect classification performance. We trained a feed-forward feature hierarchical model, HMAX, to perform a dual category line length judgment task (short versus long) with over 90% accuracy. We then tested the system in its ability to judge relative line lengths for images in a control set versus images that induce the MLI in humans. Results from the computational model show an overall illusory effect similar to that experienced by human subjects. No natural images were used for training, implying that misapplied size constancy and image-source statistics are not necessary factors for generating the illusion. A post-hoc analysis of response weights within a representative trained network ruled out the possibility that the illusion is caused by a reliance on information at low spatial frequencies. Our results suggest that the MLI can be produced using only feed-forward, neurophysiological connections.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Astrid Zeman
- Department of Cognitive Science and the ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders CCD, Macquarie University, Macquarie Park, New South Wales, Australia.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Plewan T, Weidner R, Eickhoff SB, Fink GR. Ventral and Dorsal Stream Interactions during the Perception of the Müller-Lyer Illusion: Evidence Derived from fMRI and Dynamic Causal Modeling. J Cogn Neurosci 2012; 24:2015-29. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00258] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
The human visual system converts identically sized retinal stimuli into different-sized perceptions. For instance, the Müller-Lyer illusion alters the perceived length of a line via arrows attached to its end. The strength of this illusion can be expressed as the difference between physical and perceived line length. Accordingly, illusion strength reflects how strong a representation is transformed along its way from a retinal image up to a conscious percept. In this study, we investigated changes of effective connectivity between brain areas supporting these transformation processes to further elucidate the neural underpinnings of optical illusions. The strength of the Müller-Lyer illusion was parametrically modulated while participants performed either a spatial or a luminance task. Lateral occipital cortex and right superior parietal cortex were found to be associated with illusion strength. Dynamic causal modeling was employed to investigate putative interactions between ventral and dorsal visual streams. Bayesian model selection indicated that a model that involved bidirectional connections between dorsal and ventral stream areas most accurately accounted for the underlying network dynamics. Connections within this network were partially modulated by illusion strength. The data further suggest that the two areas subserve differential roles: Whereas lateral occipital cortex seems to be directly related to size transformation processes, activation in right superior parietal cortex may reflect subsequent levels of processing, including task-related supervisory functions. Furthermore, the data demonstrate that the observer's top–down settings modulate the interactions between lateral occipital and superior parietal regions and thereby influence the effect of illusion strength.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Simon B. Eickhoff
- 1Research Centre Jülich
- 2Heinrich-Heine University, Düsseldorf, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Wu X, Li W, Zhang M, Qiu J. The neural basis of impossible figures: Evidence from an fMRI study of the two-pronged trident. Neurosci Lett 2012; 508:17-21. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.11.064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2011] [Revised: 11/26/2011] [Accepted: 11/30/2011] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
48
|
The Poggendorff illusion effect influenced by top-down control: evidence from an event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport 2012; 22:739-43. [PMID: 21876468 DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0b013e32834ab40b] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Event-related brain potentials were used to examine the neural correlates of the visual illusion effect in the Poggendorff illusion. In this study, there were three tasks, namely, illusion task 1, illusion task 2 (similar to the classical Poggendorff figures, where the two oblique lines in which individuals were prone to judge to be collinear, were not collinear in fact), and baseline task. Scalp event-related brain potential analysis revealed that (a) both illusion task 1 and illusion task 2 elicited a more negative event-related brain potential deflection (N400-600) than did baseline task, approximately 400 ms after onset of the stimuli, and (b) high-level cognitive control system is, through enhancing the influence of the context on identifying the relationships of the two oblique lines, involved in generating the Poggendorff illusion.
Collapse
|
49
|
Mohr HM, Linder NS, Dennis H, Sireteanu R. Orientation-specific aftereffects to mentally generated lines. Perception 2011; 40:272-90. [PMID: 21692419 DOI: 10.1068/p6781] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
After staring at a pattern of tilted lines, subsequent lines appear to be tilted in the opposite direction (direct tilt aftereffect, TAE). In a previous fMRI study we have demonstrated a direct TAE solely induced by the mental imagination accompanied by adaptation of orientation-selective neurons located in the extrastriate cortex, supporting the assumption of a perception-like coding of mental images. In this study we enlarge and specify the evidence for a perception-like coding of orientation-imagination. First, we replicated the previously detected direct TAE induced by line imagination with altered design-variations to control possible perceptual task confounds. Second, we tried to induce two other orientation-specific aftereffects: indirect TAE and contrast-threshold elevation aftereffect by mental imagery. The results replicate a robust direct TAE by mental imagery and by visual stimulation, with no influence of attentional resource allocation or perceptual task confounds. We could not induce an indirect TAE, but observed a perception bias in the opposite direction of the indirect TAE. The mental imagery of lines induced no orientation-selective contrast-threshold elevation aftereffect. In general, mental imagery seems to influence visual perception, indicating that perceptual resources are used by mental imagery. However, the utilisation of visual resources seems to be somewhat different from utilisation by perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Harald M Mohr
- Department of Biological Psychology, Institute for Psychology, Johann Wolfgang Goethe-University, Mertonstrasse 17, 60054 Frankfurt, Germany
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Mancini F, Bolognini N, Bricolo E, Vallar G. Cross-modal Processing in the Occipito-temporal Cortex: A TMS Study of the Müller-Lyer Illusion. J Cogn Neurosci 2011; 23:1987-97. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21561] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
The Müller-Lyer illusion occurs both in vision and in touch, and transfers cross-modally from vision to haptics [Mancini, F., Bricolo, E., & Vallar, G. Multisensory integration in the Müller-Lyer illusion: From vision to haptics. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 818–830, 2010]. Recent evidence suggests that the neural underpinnings of the Müller-Lyer illusion in the visual modality involve the bilateral lateral occipital complex (LOC) and right superior parietal cortex (SPC). Conversely, the neural correlates of the haptic and cross-modal illusions have never been investigated previously. Here we used repetitive TMS (rTMS) to address the causal role of the regions activated by the visual illusion in the generation of the visual, haptic, and cross-modal visuo-haptic illusory effects, investigating putative modality-specific versus cross-modal underlying processes. rTMS was administered to the right and the left hemisphere, over occipito-temporal cortex or SPC. rTMS over left and right occipito-temporal cortex impaired both unisensory (visual, haptic) and cross-modal processing of the illusion in a similar fashion. Conversely, rTMS interference over left and right SPC did not affect the illusion in any modality. These results demonstrate the causal involvement of bilateral occipito-temporal cortex in the representation of the visual, haptic, and cross-modal Müller-Lyer illusion, in favor of the hypothesis of shared underlying processes. This indicates that occipito-temporal cortex plays a cross-modal role in perception both of illusory and nonillusory shapes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Flavia Mancini
- 1University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
- 2IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy
| | - Nadia Bolognini
- 1University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
- 2IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy
| | | | - Giuseppe Vallar
- 1University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
- 2IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|