1
|
Kılıç F, Dövencioğlu D. Visual softness perception can be manipulated through exploratory procedures. Perception 2024; 53:674-687. [PMID: 39053476 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241261772] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/27/2024]
Abstract
Both visual and haptic softness perception have recently been shown to have multiple dimensions, such as deformability, granularity, fluidity, surface softness, and roughness. During haptic exploration, people adjust their hand motions (exploratory procedures, EPs) based on the material qualities of the object and the particular information they intend to acquire. Some of these EPs are also shown to be associated with perceived softness dimensions, for example, stroking a silk blouse or applying pressure to a pillow. Here, we aimed to investigate whether we can manipulate observers' judgments about softness attributes through exposure to videos of others performing various EPs on everyday soft materials. In two experiments, participants watched two videos of the same material: one with a corresponding EP and the other without correspondence; then, they judged these materials based on 12 softness-related adjectives (semantic differentiation method). The results of the second experiment suggested that when the EP is congruent with the dimension from which the material is chosen, the ratings for the adjectives from the same dimension are higher than the incongruent EP. This study provides evidence that participants can assess material properties from optic and mechanical cues without needing haptic signals. Additionally, our findings indicate that manipulating the hand motion can selectively facilitate material-related judgments.
Collapse
|
2
|
Colonna S, Mazzanti M. Pelvic Palpatory Tests in Manual Therapy and Osteopathy: A Critical Review of the Literature and Suggestions for New Research. Cureus 2024; 16:e64066. [PMID: 39114222 PMCID: PMC11304509 DOI: 10.7759/cureus.64066] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/07/2024] [Indexed: 08/10/2024] Open
Abstract
Manual therapists apply physical interventions to the entire structure of the body to promote healing, prevent pathologies, and improve patient health. In osteopathic practice, palpatory evaluation is considered a fundamental clinical practice requirement for identifying somatic dysfunction. Most of the articles published in this area have failed to demonstrate a level of reproducibility that supports palpation in evidence-based clinical practice. When considering the poor reliability of the palpatory tests highlighted in the literature, a discrepancy is noted with what is known about the tactile sensitivity of human hands. For static touch, the minimum size that can be detected, in the absence of applied movement or vibration, is approximately 0.2 mm. Yet, it seems that this high level of precision is insufficient to ensure reliability in the tests used to evaluate osteopathic somatic dysfunction. The purpose that underscores this article is to determine how these two contradictory elements, high sensitivity and low reliability, can be present in palpatory tests. The article reports the literature findings regarding palpatory tests of pelvic, which is an important structure for clinical purposes. Additionally, a critical review of how these studies were conducted is provided to identify any elements that may justify the obtained results. Following recent accredited guidelines present in the literature, we propose suggestions on vision training methods, manual perception refinement training, the search for anatomical markers, and the position of the examiner in relation to the examinee that may be useful for future studies on the topic covered by the article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Saverio Colonna
- Orthopedic Surgery, Spine Center, Bologna, ITA
- Education, Osteopathic Spine Center Education, Bologna, ITA
| | - Marco Mazzanti
- Education, Osteopathic Spine Center Education, Bologna, ITA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Jeschke M, Zoeller AC, Drewing K. Humans flexibly use visual priors to optimize their haptic exploratory behavior. Sci Rep 2024; 14:14906. [PMID: 38942980 PMCID: PMC11213930 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-65958-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2023] [Accepted: 06/25/2024] [Indexed: 06/30/2024] Open
Abstract
Humans can use prior information to optimize their haptic exploratory behavior. Here, we investigated the usage of visual priors, which mechanisms enable their usage, and how the usage is affected by information quality. Participants explored different grating textures and discriminated their spatial frequency. Visual priors on texture orientation were given each trial, with qualities randomly varying from high to no informational value. Adjustments of initial exploratory movement direction orthogonal to the textures' orientation served as an indicator of prior usage. Participants indeed used visual priors; the more so the higher the priors' quality (Experiment 1). Higher task demands did not increase the direct usage of visual priors (Experiment 2), but possibly fostered the establishment of adjustment behavior. In Experiment 3, we decreased the proportion of high-quality priors presented during the session, hereby reducing the contingency between high-quality priors and haptic information. In consequence, even priors of high quality ceased to evoke movement adjustments. We conclude that the establishment of adjustment behavior results from a rather implicit contingency learning. Overall, it became evident that humans can autonomously learn to use rather abstract visual priors to optimize haptic exploration, with the learning process and direct usage substantially depending on the priors' quality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michaela Jeschke
- Experimental Psychology, Justus-Liebig University, 35390, Gießen, Germany.
| | - Aaron C Zoeller
- Experimental Psychology, Justus-Liebig University, 35390, Gießen, Germany
| | - Knut Drewing
- Experimental Psychology, Justus-Liebig University, 35390, Gießen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ali Y, Montani V, Cesari P. Neural underpinnings of the interplay between actual touch and action imagination in social contexts. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 17:1274299. [PMID: 38292652 PMCID: PMC10826515 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1274299] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2023] [Accepted: 12/21/2023] [Indexed: 02/01/2024] Open
Abstract
While there is established evidence supporting the involvement of the sense of touch in various actions, the neural underpinnings of touch and action interplay in a social context remain poorly understood. To prospectively investigate this phenomenon and offer further insights, we employed a combination of motor and sensory components by asking participants to imagine exerting force with the index finger while experiencing their own touch, the touch of one another individual, the touch of a surface, and no touch. Based on the assumption that the patterns of activation in the motor system are similar when action is imagined or actually performed, we proceeded to apply a single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the primary motor cortex (M1) while participants engaged in the act of imagination. Touch experience was associated with higher M1 excitability in the presence and in the absence of force production imagination, but only during force production imagination M1 excitability differed among the types of touch: both biological sources, the self-touch and the touch of one other individual, elicited a significant increase in motor system activity when compared to touching a non-living surface or in the absence of touch. A strong correlation between individual touch avoidance questionnaire values and facilitation in the motor system was present while touching another person, indicating a social aspect for touch in action. The present study unveils the motor system correlates when the sensory/motor components of touch are considered in social contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Paola Cesari
- Department of Neuroscience, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Li B, Gerling GJ. An individual's skin stiffness predicts their tactile discrimination of compliance. J Physiol 2023; 601:5777-5794. [PMID: 37942821 PMCID: PMC10872733 DOI: 10.1113/jp285271] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2023] [Accepted: 10/19/2023] [Indexed: 11/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Individual differences in tactile acuity have been correlated with age, gender and finger size, whereas the role of the skin's stiffness has been underexplored. Using an approach to image the 3-D deformation of the skin surface during contact with transparent elastic objects, we evaluate a cohort of 40 young participants, who present a diverse range of finger size, skin stiffness and fingerprint ridge breadth. The results indicate that skin stiffness generally correlates with finger size, although individuals with relatively softer skin can better discriminate compliant objects. Analysis of contact at the skin surface reveals that softer skin generates more prominent patterns of deformation, in particular greater rates of change in contact area, which correlate with higher rates of perceptual discrimination of compliance, regardless of finger size. Moreover, upon applying hyaluronic acid to soften individuals' skin, we observe immediate, marked and systematic changes in skin deformation and consequent improvements in perceptual acuity in differentiating compliance. Together, the combination of 3-D imaging of the skin surface, biomechanics measurements, multivariate regression and clustering, and psychophysical experiments show that subtle distinctions in skin stiffness modulate the mechanical signalling of touch and shape individual differences in perceptual acuity. KEY POINTS: Although declines in tactile acuity with ageing are a function of multiple factors, for younger people, the current working hypothesis has been that smaller fingers are better at informing perceptual discrimination because of a higher density of neural afferents. To decouple relative impacts on tactile acuity of skin properties of finger size, skin stiffness, and fingerprint ridge breadth, we combined 3-D imaging of skin surface deformation, biomechanical measurements, multivariate regression and clustering, and psychophysics. The results indicate that skin stiffness generally correlates with finger size, although it more robustly correlates with and predicts an individual's perceptual acuity. In particular, more elastic skin generates higher rates of deformation, which correlate with perceptual discrimination, shown most dramatically by softening each participant's skin with hyaluronic acid. In refining the current working hypothesis, we show the skin's stiffness strongly shapes the signalling of touch and modulates individual differences in perceptual acuity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bingxu Li
- Systems and Information Engineering, Mechanical Engineering, School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- Systems and Information Engineering, Mechanical Engineering, School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Li B, Gerling GJ. An individual's skin stiffness predicts their tactile acuity. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.07.17.548686. [PMID: 37502933 PMCID: PMC10370135 DOI: 10.1101/2023.07.17.548686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/29/2023]
Abstract
Individual differences in tactile acuity have been correlated with age, gender, and finger size, while the role of the skin's stiffness has been underexplored. Using an approach to image the 3-D deformation of the skin surface while in contact with transparent elastic objects, we evaluate a cohort of 40 young participants, who present a diverse range of finger size, skin stiffness, and fingerprint ridge breadth. The results indicate that skin stiffness generally correlates with finger size, although individuals with relatively softer skin can better discriminate compliant objects. Analysis of contact at the skin surface reveals that softer skin generates more prominent patterns of deformation, in particular greater rates of change in contact area, which correlate with higher rates of perceptual discrimination, regardless of finger size. Moreover, upon applying hyaluronic acid to soften individuals' skin, we observe immediate, marked and systematic changes in skin deformation and consequent improvements in perceptual acuity. Together, the combination of 3-D imaging of the skin surface, biomechanics measurements, multivariate regression and clustering, and psychophysical experiments show that subtle distinctions in skin stiffness modulate the mechanical signaling of touch and shape individual differences in perceptual acuity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bingxu Li
- Systems and Information Engineering, Mechanical Engineering, University of Virginia
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- Systems and Information Engineering, Mechanical Engineering, University of Virginia
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Li B, Hauser SC, Gerling GJ. Faster Indentation Influences Skin Deformation To Reduce Tactile Discriminability of Compliant Objects. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2023; 16:215-227. [PMID: 37028048 PMCID: PMC10357367 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2023.3253256] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/19/2023]
Abstract
To discriminate the compliance of soft objects, we rely upon spatiotemporal cues in the mechanical deformation of the skin. However, we have few direct observations of skin deformation over time, in particular how its response differs with indentation velocities and depths, and thereby helps inform our perceptual judgments. To help fill this gap, we develop a 3D stereo imaging method to observe contact of the skin's surface with transparent, compliant stimuli. Experiments with human-subjects, in passive touch, are conducted with stimuli varying in compliance, indentation depth, velocity, and time duration. The results indicate that contact durations greater than 0.4 s are perceptually discriminable. Moreover, compliant pairs delivered at higher velocities are more difficult to discriminate because they induce smaller differences in deformation. In a detailed quantification of the skin's surface deformation, we find that several, independent cues aid perception. In particular, the rate of change of gross contact area best correlates with discriminability, across indentation velocities and compliances. However, cues associated with skin surface curvature and bulk force are also predictive, for stimuli more and less compliant than skin, respectively. These findings and detailed measurements seek to inform the design of haptic interfaces.
Collapse
|
8
|
Song Y, Lv S, Wang F, Li M. Hardness-and-Type Recognition of Different Objects Based on a Novel Porous Graphene Flexible Tactile Sensor Array. MICROMACHINES 2023; 14:217. [PMID: 36677278 PMCID: PMC9860881 DOI: 10.3390/mi14010217] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/26/2022] [Revised: 01/09/2023] [Accepted: 01/12/2023] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
Accurately recognizing the hardness and type of different objects by tactile sensors is of great significance in human-machine interaction. In this paper, a novel porous graphene flexible tactile sensor array with great performance is designed and fabricated, and it is mounted on a two-finger mechanical actuator. This is used to detect various tactile sequence features from different objects by slightly squeezing them by 2 mm. A Residual Network (ResNet) model, with excellent adaptivity and feature extraction ability, is constructed to realize the recognition of 4 hardness categories and 12 object types, based on the tactile time sequence signals collected by the novel sensor array; the average accuracies of hardness and type recognition are 100% and 99.7%, respectively. To further verify the classification ability of the ResNet model for the tactile feature information detected by the sensor array, the Multilayer Perceptron (MLP), LeNet, Multi-Channel Deep Convolutional Neural Network (MCDCNN), and ENCODER models are built based on the same dataset used for the ResNet model. The average recognition accuracies of the 4hardness categories, based on those four models, are 93.6%, 98.3%, 93.3%, and 98.1%. Meanwhile, the average recognition accuracies of the 12 object types, based on the four models, are 94.7%, 98.9%, 85.0%, and 96.4%. All of the results demonstrate that the novel porous graphene tactile sensor array has excellent perceptual performance and the ResNet model can very effectively and precisely complete the hardness and type recognition of objects for the flexible tactile sensor array.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yang Song
- School of Electronic and Information Engineering, Anhui Jianzhu University, Hefei 230601, China
- Key Laboratory of Building Information Acquisition and Measurement Control Technology, Anhui Jianzhu University, Hefei 230601, China
| | - Shanna Lv
- School of Electronic and Information Engineering, Anhui Jianzhu University, Hefei 230601, China
| | - Feilu Wang
- School of Electronic and Information Engineering, Anhui Jianzhu University, Hefei 230601, China
- Key Laboratory of Building Information Acquisition and Measurement Control Technology, Anhui Jianzhu University, Hefei 230601, China
| | - Mingkun Li
- School of Electronic and Information Engineering, Anhui Jianzhu University, Hefei 230601, China
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Deflorio D, Di Luca M, Wing AM. Skin and Mechanoreceptor Contribution to Tactile Input for Perception: A Review of Simulation Models. Front Hum Neurosci 2022; 16:862344. [PMID: 35721353 PMCID: PMC9201416 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.862344] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2022] [Accepted: 03/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
We review four current computational models that simulate the response of mechanoreceptors in the glabrous skin to tactile stimulation. The aim is to inform researchers in psychology, sensorimotor science and robotics who may want to implement this type of quantitative model in their research. This approach proves relevant to understanding of the interaction between skin response and neural activity as it avoids some of the limitations of traditional measurement methods of tribology, for the skin, and neurophysiology, for tactile neurons. The main advantage is to afford new ways of looking at the combined effects of skin properties on the activity of a population of tactile neurons, and to examine different forms of coding by tactile neurons. Here, we provide an overview of selected models from stimulus application to neuronal spiking response, including their evaluation in terms of existing data, and their applicability in relation to human tactile perception.
Collapse
|
10
|
Metzger A, Toscani M. Unsupervised learning of haptic material properties. eLife 2022; 11:64876. [PMID: 35195520 PMCID: PMC8865843 DOI: 10.7554/elife.64876] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2020] [Accepted: 12/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
When touching the surface of an object, its spatial structure translates into a vibration on the skin. The perceptual system evolved to translate this pattern into a representation that allows to distinguish between different materials. Here, we show that perceptual haptic representation of materials emerges from efficient encoding of vibratory patterns elicited by the interaction with materials. We trained a deep neural network with unsupervised learning (Autoencoder) to reconstruct vibratory patterns elicited by human haptic exploration of different materials. The learned compressed representation (i.e., latent space) allows for classification of material categories (i.e., plastic, stone, wood, fabric, leather/wool, paper, and metal). More importantly, classification performance is higher with perceptual category labels as compared to ground truth ones, and distances between categories in the latent space resemble perceptual distances, suggesting a similar coding. Crucially, the classification performance and the similarity between the perceptual and the latent space decrease with decreasing compression level. We could further show that the temporal tuning of the emergent latent dimensions is similar to properties of human tactile receptors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Metzger
- Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Bournemouth, United Kingdom.,Department of Psychology, Justus-Liebig University, Giessen, Germany
| | - Matteo Toscani
- Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Bournemouth, United Kingdom.,Department of Psychology, Justus-Liebig University, Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Wang L, Li Q, Lam J, Wang Z. Tactual Recognition of Soft Objects From Deformation Cues. IEEE Robot Autom Lett 2022. [DOI: 10.1109/lra.2021.3119393] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
12
|
Metzger A, Toscani M, Valsecchi M, Drewing K. Target Search and Inspection Strategies in Haptic Search. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2021; 14:804-815. [PMID: 33929965 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2021.3076847] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Haptic search is a common everyday task, usually consisting of two processes: target search and target analysis. During target search we need to know where our fingers are in space, remember the already completed path and the outline of the remaining space. During target analysis we need to understand whether the detected potential target is the desired one. Here we characterized dynamics of exploratory movements in these two processes. In our experiments participants searched for a particular configuration of symbols on a rectangular tactile display. We observed that participants preferentially moved the hand parallel to the edges of the tactile display during target search, which possibly eased orientation within the search space. After a potential target was detected by any of the fingers, there was higher probability that subsequent exploration was performed by the index or the middle finger. At the same time, these fingers dramatically slowed down. Being in contact with the potential target, the index and the middle finger moved within a smaller area than the other fingers, which rather seemed to move away to leave them space. These results suggest that the middle and the index finger are specialized for fine analysis in haptic search.
Collapse
|
13
|
Cavdan M, Drewing K, Doerschner K. The look and feel of soft are similar across different softness dimensions. J Vis 2021; 21:20. [PMID: 34581768 PMCID: PMC8479577 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.10.20] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2021] [Accepted: 08/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The softness of objects can be perceived through several senses. For instance, to judge the softness of a cat's fur, we do not only look at it, we often also run our fingers through its coat. Recently, we have shown that haptically perceived softness covaries with the compliance, viscosity, granularity, and furriness of materials (Dovencioglu, Üstün, Doerschner, & Drewing, 2020). However, it is unknown whether vision can provide similar information about the various aspects of perceived softness. Here, we investigated this question in an experiment with three conditions: in the haptic condition, blindfolded participants explored materials with their hands, in the static visual condition participants were presented with close-up photographs of the same materials, and in the dynamic visual condition participants watched videos of the hand-material interactions that were recorded in the haptic condition. After haptically or visually exploring the materials, participants rated them on various attributes. Our results show a high overall perceptual correspondence among the three experimental conditions. With a few exceptions, this correspondence tended to be strongest between haptic and dynamic visual conditions. These results are discussed with respect to information potentially available through the senses, or through prior experience, when judging the softness of materials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Müge Cavdan
- Justus Liebig University, Department of Psychology, Giessen, Germany
| | - Knut Drewing
- Justus Liebig University, Department of Psychology, Giessen, Germany
| | - Katja Doerschner
- Justus Liebig University, Department of Psychology, Giessen, Germany
- Bilkent University, National Magnetic Resonance Research Center, Ankara, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Physical correlates of human-like softness elicit high tactile pleasantness. Sci Rep 2021; 11:16510. [PMID: 34389767 PMCID: PMC8363669 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-96044-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2021] [Accepted: 08/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Touching an object can elicit affective sensations. Because these sensations are critical for social interaction, tactile preferences may be adapted to the characteristics of the human body. We have previously shown that compliance, a physical correlate of softness, increased the tactile pleasantness of a deformable surface. However, the extent to which object compliance similar to the human body elicits tactile pleasantness remains unknown. We addressed this question by using a wide range of compliances and by measuring the distribution of compliance of human body parts. The participants numerically estimated the perceived pleasantness or softness while pushing tactile stimuli with their right index fingers. The perceived softness monotonically increased with increasing compliance and then leveled off around the end of the stimulus range. By contrast, pleasantness showed an inverse U pattern as a function of compliance, reaching the maximum between 5 and 7 mm/N. This range of compliance was within that for both hand and arm. These results indicate that objects with similar compliance levels as those of human body parts yield the highest pleasantness when pushing them.
Collapse
|
15
|
Cavdan M, Doerschner K, Drewing K. Task and Material Properties Interactively Affect Softness Explorations Along Different Dimensions. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2021; 14:603-614. [PMID: 33784626 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2021.3069626] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Haptic research has frequently equated softness with the compliance of elastic objects. However, in a recent study we have suggested that compliance is not the only perceived material dimension underlying what is commonly called softness [1 ]. Here, we investigate, whether the different perceptual dimensions of softness affect how materials are haptically explored. Specifically, we tested whether also the task, i.e., the attribute that a material is being judged on, might affect how a material is explored. To this end we selected 15 adjectives and 19 materials that each associate with different softness dimensions for the study. In the experiment, while participants freely explored and rated the materials, we recorded their hand movements. These movements were subsequently categorized into distinct exploratory procedures (EPs) and analyzed in a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). The results of this analysis suggest that the pattern of EPs depended not only on the material's softness dimension and the task (i.e., what attributes were rated), but also on an interaction between the two factors. Taken together, our findings support the notion of multiple perceptual dimensions of softness and suggest that participants actively adapt their EPs in a nuanced way when judging a particular softness dimensions for a given material.
Collapse
|
16
|
Doi K, Sakaguchi S, Nishimura T, Fujimoto H, Ino S. Assessing the Stiffness Perception of Acupressure Massage Beginning Learners: A Pilot Study. SENSORS 2021; 21:s21072472. [PMID: 33918315 PMCID: PMC8038168 DOI: 10.3390/s21072472] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2021] [Revised: 03/03/2021] [Accepted: 03/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Visually impaired licensed therapists must have the ability to perceive stiffness through their fingertips in the school for the blind. The teachers strive to provide careful introductory education based on a quantitative assessment of new students’ basic stiffness perception. However, assessment materials to help teachers understand new students’ stiffness perception are lacking. This study aimed to develop suitable fundamental assessment materials that visually impaired licensed teachers could use to quantitatively assess the difference in the stiffness perception ability of beginning learners in the early stages of learning. They were asked to discriminate the presented materials one at a time, which consisted of thermoplastic elastomers with different degrees of stiffness. We used these materials to compare the beginning learners’ ability to perceive stiffness with that of teachers and found that teachers answered correctly at an overall significantly higher rate. Specifically, the teachers’ correct response rate (78.8%) for the stiffness perception of all presented stimuli was approximately 15% higher than the beginning learners’ correct response rate (64.2%). These results revealed areas of stiffness that are difficult for beginning learners to identify.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kouki Doi
- Department of Information and Support, National Institute of Special Needs Education, Yokosuka 239-8585, Japan
- Correspondence:
| | - Saito Sakaguchi
- Graduate School of Human Sciences, Waseda University, Tokorozawa 359-1192, Japan;
| | - Takahiro Nishimura
- Center for Promoting Education for Persons with Developmental Disabilities, National Institute of Special Needs Education, Yokosuka 239-8585, Japan;
| | - Hiroshi Fujimoto
- Faculty of Human Sciences, Waseda University, Tokorozawa 359-1192, Japan;
| | - Shuichi Ino
- Human Informatics and Interaction Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Tsukuba 305-8566, Japan;
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Xu C, Wang Y, Gerling GJ. An elasticity-curvature illusion decouples cutaneous and proprioceptive cues in active exploration of soft objects. PLoS Comput Biol 2021; 17:e1008848. [PMID: 33750948 PMCID: PMC8016306 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008848] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2020] [Revised: 04/01/2021] [Accepted: 03/03/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Our sense of touch helps us encounter the richness of our natural world. Across a myriad of contexts and repetitions, we have learned to deploy certain exploratory movements in order to elicit perceptual cues that are salient and efficient. The task of identifying optimal exploration strategies and somatosensory cues that underlie our softness perception remains relevant and incomplete. Leveraging psychophysical evaluations combined with computational finite element modeling of skin contact mechanics, we investigate an illusion phenomenon in exploring softness; where small-compliant and large-stiff spheres are indiscriminable. By modulating contact interactions at the finger pad, we find this elasticity-curvature illusion is observable in passive touch, when the finger is constrained to be stationary and only cutaneous responses from mechanosensitive afferents are perceptible. However, these spheres become readily discriminable when explored volitionally with musculoskeletal proprioception available. We subsequently exploit this phenomenon to dissociate relative contributions from cutaneous and proprioceptive signals in encoding our percept of material softness. Our findings shed light on how we volitionally explore soft objects, i.e., by controlling surface contact force to optimally elicit and integrate proprioceptive inputs amidst indiscriminable cutaneous contact cues. Moreover, in passive touch, e.g., for touch-enabled displays grounded to the finger, we find those spheres are discriminable when rates of change in cutaneous contact are varied between the stimuli, to supplant proprioceptive feedback.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chang Xu
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia, United States of America
| | - Yuxiang Wang
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia, United States of America
| | - Gregory J. Gerling
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
A Systematic Comparison of Perceptual Performance in Softness Discrimination with Different Fingers. Atten Percept Psychophys 2020; 82:3696-3709. [PMID: 32686066 PMCID: PMC7536162 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-020-02100-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
In studies investigating haptic softness perception, participants are typically instructed to explore soft objects by indenting them with their index finger. In contrast, performance with other fingers has rarely been investigated. We wondered which fingers are used in spontaneous exploration and if performance differences between fingers can explain spontaneous usage. In Experiment 1 participants discriminated the softness of two rubber stimuli with hardly any constraints on finger movements. Results indicate that humans use successive phases of different fingers and finger combinations during an exploration, preferring index, middle, and (to a lesser extent) ring finger. In Experiment 2 we compared discrimination thresholds between conditions, with participants using one of the four fingers of the dominant hand. Participants compared the softness of rubber stimuli in a two-interval forced choice discrimination task. Performance with index and middle finger was better as compared to ring and little finger, the little finger was the worst. In Experiment 3 we again compared discrimination thresholds, but participants were told to use constant peak force. Performance with the little finger was worst, whereas performance for the other fingers did not differ. We conclude that in spontaneous exploration the preference of combinations of index, middle, and partly ring finger seems to be well chosen, as indicated by improved performance with the spontaneously used fingers. Better performance seems to be based on both different motor abilities to produce force, mainly linked to using index and middle finger, and different sensory sensitivities, mainly linked to avoiding the little finger.
Collapse
|
19
|
Sakai S, Takatori R, Nomura M, Uehara K. Useful parameters for the motion analysis of facial skin care in Japanese women. J Physiol Anthropol 2020; 39:22. [PMID: 32831147 PMCID: PMC7507946 DOI: 10.1186/s40101-020-00234-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2019] [Accepted: 08/06/2020] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Facial skin care (FSC) is an important routine for Japanese women. Hand motions during FSC physically affect psychological state. However, it is very difficult to evaluate hand motions during personal and complex FSC. The objective of this study was to find out objective and quantitative parameters for hand motions during facial skin care (FSC). Women who enjoy and soothe during FSC (Enjoyment group (E group), n = 20) or not (non-enjoyment group (NE group), n = 19) were recruited by an advance questionnaire. The same lotion, emulsion, and cream were provided to all subjects, and they used sequentially in the same way as the women’s daily FSC. The motion of the marker on the back side of the right middle finger during FSC was tracked by a motion capture system. The heart rate variability (HRV) was also measured before and after FSC for evaluating psychological effect. Results The averaged acceleration (Avg. ACC), approximate entropy (ApEn), and power law scaling exponent (Rest γ) of the cumulative duration of slow motion from the sequential data of acceleration were evaluated. Compared to the NE group, the E group showed a lower Avg. ACC when using emulsion (p = 0.005) and cream (p = 0.007), a lower ApEn when using emulsion (p = 0.003), and a lower Rest γ (p = 0.024) when using all items, suggesting that compared to the NE group, the E group had more tender and regular motion, and sustainable slow motions, especially in the use of emulsion. In the E group, the low/high-frequency component of HRV decreased significantly after FSC, suggesting suppression of sympathetic activity (p = 0.045). NE group did not. For all subjects, ApEn and Rest γ showed significantly positive correlation with the increase in the low/high-frequency component of HRV after FSC (p < 0.01). ApEn showed significantly negative correlation with the increase in the high-frequency component of HRV after FSC (p < 0.05). Avg. ACC did not show significant correlation with them. These results suggested that the behavior of FSC influences the autonomic nerve system. Conclusions ApEn and Rest γ are useful parameters for evaluating quality of hand motions during FSC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shingo Sakai
- Skincare Products Research, Kao Corporation, 5-3-28, Kotobuki-cho, Odawara, Kanagawa, 250-0002, Japan.
| | - Ruako Takatori
- Graduate School of System Informatics, Kobe University, 1-1, Rokkodai, Nada, 657-8501, Japan.,Present address: Core Device Development Sec., Service Development HQ, R&D Dept., Dwango Co., Ltd., Kabukiza Tower, 4-12-14 Ginza, Chuo-ku, Tokyo, 104-0061, Japan
| | - Mika Nomura
- Kansei Value Development Sensory Science Research, Kao Corporation, 5-3-28, Kotobuki-cho, Odawara, Kanagawa, 250-0002, Japan
| | - Kuniaki Uehara
- Graduate School of System Informatics, Kobe University, 1-1, Rokkodai, Nada, 657-8501, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Li B, Hauser S, Gerling GJ. Identifying 3-D spatiotemporal skin deformation cues evoked in interacting with compliant elastic surfaces. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM : [PROCEEDINGS]. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM 2020; 2020:35-40. [PMID: 34458383 PMCID: PMC8395532 DOI: 10.1109/haptics45997.2020.ras.hap20.22.5a9b38d8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
We regularly touch soft, compliant fruits and tissues. To help us discriminate them, we rely upon cues embedded in spatial and temporal deformation of finger pad skin. However, we do not yet understand, in touching objects of various compliance, how such patterns evolve over time, and drive perception. Using a 3-D stereo imaging technique in passive touch, we develop metrics for quantifying skin deformation, across compliance, displacement, and time. The metrics map 2-D estimates of terminal contact area to 3-D metrics that represent spatial and temporal changes in penetration depth, surface curvature, and force. To do this, clouds of thousands of 3-D points are reduced in dimensionality into stacks of ellipses, to be more readily comparable between participants and trials. To evaluate the robustness of the derived 3-D metrics, human subjects experiments are performed with stimulus pairs varying in compliance and discriminability. The results indicate that metrics such as penetration depth and surface curvature can distinguish compliances earlier, at less displacement. Observed also are distinct modes of skin deformation, for contact with stiffer objects, versus softer objects that approach the skin's compliance. These observations of the skin's deformation may guide the design and control of haptic actuation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bingxu Li
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| | - Steven Hauser
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Xu C, Gerling GJ. Time-dependent Cues Encode the Minimum Exploration Time in Discriminating Naturalistic Compliances. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM : [PROCEEDINGS]. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM 2020; 2020:22-27. [PMID: 34447856 PMCID: PMC8386199 DOI: 10.1109/haptics45997.2020.ras.hap20.7.ec43f6a7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Our perception of compliance is informed by multi-dimensional tactile cues. Compared with stationary cues at terminal contact, time-dependent cues may afford optimal efficiency, speed, and fidelity. In this work, we investigate strategies by which temporal cues may encode compliances by modulating our exploration time. Two potential perceptual strategies are considered, inspired by memory representations within and between explorations. For either strategy, we introduce a unique computational approach. First, a curve similarity analysis, of accumulating touch force between sequentially explored compliances, generates a minimum time for discrimination. Second, a Kalman filtering approach derives a recognition time from progressive integration of stiffness estimates over time within a single exploration. Human-subjects experiments are conducted for both single finger touch and pinch grasp. The results indicate that for either strategy, by employing a more natural pinch grasp, time-dependent cues afford greater efficiency by reducing the exploration time, especially for harder objects. Moreover, compared to single finger touch, pinch grasp improves discrimination rates in judging plum ripeness. The time-dependent strategies as defined here appear promising, and may tie with the time-scales over which we make perceptual judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chang Xu
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Frequency of Switching Touching Mode Reflects Tactile Preference Judgment. Sci Rep 2020; 10:3022. [PMID: 32080252 PMCID: PMC7033153 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-59883-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2019] [Accepted: 02/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
We can judge affective aspects of objects by actively exploring them with our hands. Previous studies have mainly focused on how the physical properties of an object’s surface affect tactile preference evaluations. However, despite the widely accepted notion that the participant’s strategy has a great impact on how they explore an object, there is a lack of investigations of hand motion during preference judgment and its impact on preference rating. This paper recruits the recurrence plot technique to illustrate the temporal dynamics of explorative hand motion. In an experiment, participants were asked to freely explore the surface of tactile stimuli and rate their tactile preference for them. The temporal dynamics of finger velocity and force were visualized and characterized by using recurrence quantification analysis. We found correlations between preference ratings and recurrence features that represent the temporal dynamics of explorative hand motion, in addition to correlations between preference ratings and conventional time-averaged features (e.g., averaged finger velocity). One unique feature that correlated with preference ratings was TREND, which represents to what extent similar motion patterns repeatedly occur. The results of a subsidiary analysis supported the possibility that the TREND difference can be interpreted as the frequency of switching touching modes (e.g., stroking and pushing motions). Taken together, these results suggest that participants tend to perform the same hand motion repeatedly for preferable objects, while they tend to combine different touching modes for less preferable objects. They also indicate that the recurrence plot scheme is a promising way to extract humans’ strategies for tactile exploration.
Collapse
|
23
|
Xu C, He H, Hauser SC, Gerling GJ. Tactile Exploration Strategies With Natural Compliant Objects Elicit Virtual Stiffness Cues. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2020; 13:4-10. [PMID: 31841421 PMCID: PMC7147988 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2019.2959767] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
When interacting with deformable objects, tactile cues at the finger pad help inform our perception of material compliance. Nearly all prior studies have relied on highly homogenous, engineered materials such as silicone-elastomers and foams. In contrast, we employ soft plum fruit varying in ripeness; ecological substances associated with tasks of everyday life. In this article, we investigate volitional exploratory strategies and contact interactions, for comparison to engineered materials. New measurement techniques are introduced, including an ink-based method to capture finger pad to fruit contact interactions, and instrumented force and optical sensors to capture imposed force and displacement. Human-subjects experiments are conducted for both single finger touch and two finger grasp. The results indicate that terminal contact areas between soft and hard plums are indistinguishable, but the newly defined metric of virtual stiffness can differentiate between the fruits' ripeness, amidst their local variations in geometry, stiffness, and viscoelasticity. Moreover, it affords discrimination independent of one's touch force. This metric illustrates the tie between the deployment of active, exploratory strategies and the elicitation of optimal cues for perceptual discrimination. Compared to single finger touch, perceptual discrimination improves further in pinch grasp, which is indeed a more natural gesture for judging ripeness.
Collapse
|
24
|
Metzger A, Drewing K. Memory influences haptic perception of softness. Sci Rep 2019; 9:14383. [PMID: 31591427 PMCID: PMC6779751 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-50835-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2017] [Accepted: 09/20/2019] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
The memory of an object’s property (e.g. its typical colour) can affect its visual perception. We investigated whether memory of the softness of every-day objects influences their haptic perception. We produced bipartite silicone rubber stimuli: one half of the stimuli was covered with a layer of an object (sponge, wood, tennis ball, foam ball); the other half was uncovered silicone. Participants were not aware of the partition. They first used their bare finger to stroke laterally over the covering layer to recognize the well-known object and then indented the other half of the stimulus with a probe to compare its softness to that of an uncovered silicone stimulus. Across four experiments with different methods we showed that silicon stimuli covered with a layer of rather hard objects (tennis ball and wood) were perceived harder than the same silicon stimuli when being covered with a layer of rather soft objects (sponge and foam ball), indicating that haptic perception of softness is affected by memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Metzger
- Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Department of General Psychology, Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Strasse 10F, D-35394, Giessen, Germany.
| | - Knut Drewing
- Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Department of General Psychology, Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Strasse 10F, D-35394, Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Caldiran O, Tan HZ, Basdogan C. Visuo-Haptic Discrimination of Viscoelastic Materials. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2019; 12:438-450. [PMID: 31247562 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2019.2924212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
In our daily lives, we interact with different types of deformable materials. Regarding their mechanical behavior, some of those materials lie in a range that is between purely elastic and purely viscous. This range of mechanical behavior is described as viscoelasticity. In certain types of haptic interactions, such as assessment of ripeness of fruit, firmness of cheese, and consistency of organ tissue, we rely heavily on our haptic perception of viscoelastic materials. The relationship between the mechanical behavior of viscoelastic materials and our perception of them has been investigated in the field of psychorheology. However, our knowledge on how we perceive viscoelastic materials is still quite limited though some research work has already been done on purely elastic and purely viscous materials. History- and frequency-dependent behavior of viscoelastic materials result in a complex time-dependent response, which requires relatively more sophisticated models to investigate their behavior than those of purely elastic and viscous materials. In this study, we model viscoelasticity using a "springpot" (i.e., fractional-order derivative element) and express its behavior in the frequency domain using two physical parameters-"magnitude" and "phase" of complex stiffness. In the frequency domain, we are able to devise signal detection experiments where we can investigate the perception of viscoelastic materials using the perceptual terms of "firmness" and "bounciness," corresponding to the physical parameters of "magnitude" and "phase." The results of our experiments show that the just-noticeable difference (JND) for bounciness increases linearly with increasing "phase," following Weber's law, while the JND for firmness is surprisingly independent of the level of "phase."
Collapse
|
26
|
Metzger A, Drewing K. Effects of Stimulus Exploration Length and Time on the Integration of Information in Haptic Softness Discrimination. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2019; 12:451-460. [PMID: 30794519 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2019.2899298] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
In haptic perception, information is often sampled serially (e.g., a stimulus is repeatedly indented to estimate its softness), requiring that sensory information is retained and integrated over time. Hence, integration of sequential information is likely affected by memory. Particularly, when two sequentially explored stimuli are compared, integration of information on the second stimulus might be determined by the fading representation of the first stimulus. We investigated how the exploration length of the first stimulus and a temporal delay affect contributions of sequentially gathered estimates of the second stimulus in haptic softness discrimination. Participants subsequently explored two silicon rubber stimuli by indenting the first stimulus one or five times and the second stimulus always three times. In an additional experiment, we introduced a 5-s delay after the first stimulus was indented five times. We show that the longer the first stimulus is explored, the more estimates of the second stimulus' softness contribute to the discrimination of the two stimuli, independent of the delay. This suggests that the exploration length of the first stimulus influences the strength of its representation, persisting at least for 5 s, and determines how much information about the second stimulus is exploited for the comparison.
Collapse
|
27
|
Xu C, Hauser SC, Wang Y, Gerling GJ. Roles of Force Cues and Proprioceptive Joint Angles in Active Exploration of Compliant Objects. WORLD HAPTICS CONFERENCE. WORLD HAPTICS CONFERENCE 2019; 2019:10.1109/whc.2019.8816159. [PMID: 34765101 PMCID: PMC8580133 DOI: 10.1109/whc.2019.8816159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
We employ distinct exploratory procedures to improve our perceptual judgments of an object's properties. For instance, with respect to compliance, we exert pressure against a resisting force. The present work investigates ties between strategies for active control of the finger and resultant cues by which compliances may be discriminated. In particular, we employ elastic spheres that co-vary in compliance and radius, as these generate non-differentiable contact areas and are discriminable only in active touch with proprioceptive inputs. During human-subjects psychophysical experiments, we measure touch force, fingertip displacement, and joint kinematics. Two active touch paradigms are used, with and without a force constraint. First, in behaviorally-controlled situations that make force cues non-useful, the results indicate that participants can employ a force-matching strategy between the compliant objects and rely upon displacement-related cues to differentiate them. We show these cues are directly tied to a proprioception mechanism, specifically, the angle of the MCP joint. However, in the fully active paradigm, participants control displacements instead and discriminate via force-related cues. Similar to prior findings in passive touch, we find that force-related cues, likewise, are used in active touch for the optimal and efficient discrimination of compliant objects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chang Xu
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| | - Steven C Hauser
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| | - Yuxiang Wang
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- School of Engineering and Applied Science, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Zoeller AC, Lezkan A, Paulun VC, Fleming RW, Drewing K. Integration of prior knowledge during haptic exploration depends on information type. J Vis 2019; 19:20. [PMID: 30998830 DOI: 10.1167/19.4.20] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
When haptically exploring softness, humans use higher peak forces when indenting harder versus softer objects. Here, we investigated the influence of different channels and types of prior knowledge on initial peak forces. Participants explored two stimuli (hard vs. soft) and judged which was softer. In Experiment 1 participants received either semantic (the words "hard" and "soft"), visual (video of indentation), or prior information from recurring presentation (blocks of harder or softer pairs only). In a control condition no prior information was given (randomized presentation). In the recurring condition participants used higher initial forces when exploring harder stimuli. No effects were found in control and semantic conditions. With visual prior information, participants used less force for harder objects. We speculate that these findings reflect differences between implicit knowledge induced by recurring presentation and explicit knowledge induced by visual and semantic information. To test this hypothesis, we investigated whether explicit prior information interferes with implicit information in Experiment 2. Two groups of participants discriminated softness of harder or softer stimuli in two conditions (blocked and randomized). The interference group received additional explicit information during the blocked condition; the implicit-only group did not. Implicit prior information was only used for force adaptation when no additional explicit information was given, whereas explicit interfered with movement adaptation. The integration of prior knowledge only seems possible when implicit prior knowledge is induced-not with explicit knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron C Zoeller
- Department of General Psychology, Giessen University, Gießen, Germany
| | - Alexandra Lezkan
- Department of General Psychology, Giessen University, Gießen, Germany
| | - Vivian C Paulun
- Department of General Psychology, Giessen University, Gießen, Germany
| | - Roland W Fleming
- Department of General Psychology, Giessen University, Gießen, Germany
| | - Knut Drewing
- Department of General Psychology, Giessen University, Gießen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Higashi K, Okamoto S, Yamada Y, Nagano H, Konyo M. Hardness Perception Based on Dynamic Stiffness in Tapping. Front Psychol 2019; 9:2654. [PMID: 30662422 PMCID: PMC6328787 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02654] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2018] [Accepted: 12/10/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
A human can judge the hardness of an object based on the damped natural vibration caused by tapping the surface of the object using a fingertip. In this study, we investigated the influence of the dynamic characteristics of vibrations on the hardness perceived by tapping. Subjectively reported hardness values were related to the dynamic stiffness of several objects. The dynamic stiffness, which characterizes the impulsive response of an object, was acquired across the 40-1,000 Hz frequency range for cuboids of 14 types of materials by administering a hammering test. We performed two psychophysical experiments-a ranking task and a magnitude-estimation tasks-wherein participants rated the perceived hardness of each block by tapping it with a finger. We found that the perceptual effect of dynamic stiffness depends on the frequency. Its effect displayed a peak around 300 Hz and decreased or disappeared at higher frequencies, at which human perceptual capabilities are limited. The acquired results help design hardness experienced by products.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kosuke Higashi
- Department of Mechanical Systems Engineering, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
| | - Shogo Okamoto
- Department of Mechanical Systems Engineering, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
| | - Yoji Yamada
- Department of Mechanical Systems Engineering, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
| | - Hikaru Nagano
- Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Masashi Konyo
- Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Strese M, Hassen R, Noll A, Steinbach E. A Tactile Computer Mouse for the Display of Surface Material Properties. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2019; 12:18-33. [PMID: 30106740 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2018.2864751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
We present a novel input/output device to display the tactile properties of surface materials. The proposed Tactile Computer Mouse (TCM) is equipped with a series of actuators that can create perceptually relevant tactile cues to a user. The display capabilities of our TCM match the major tactile dimensions in human surface material perception, namely, hardness, friction, warmth, microscopic roughness, and macroscopic roughness. The TCM also preserves necessary interaction capabilities of a typical computer mouse. In addition to the TCM design, we introduce data acquisition procedures and concepts that are necessary to derive a parametric representation of a surface material and further demonstrate the corresponding rendering approach on the TCM. We conducted subjective experiments to determine tactile property ratings of real materials, perceived property ratings using the TCM, and how precisely subjects match the real materials to corresponding virtual material representations using the TCM in the absence of visual and audible clues. Our experimental results show that our TCM successfully displays the five fundamental tactile dimensions and that the twenty participants were able to perceive the TCM-produced virtual surface material tactile sensations with a recognition rate of 89.6 percent for ten different materials.
Collapse
|
31
|
Interdependences between finger movement direction and haptic perception of oriented textures. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0208988. [PMID: 30550578 PMCID: PMC6294351 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208988] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2018] [Accepted: 11/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Although the natural haptic perception of textures includes active finger movements, it is unclear how closely perception and movements are linked. Here we investigated this question using oriented textures. Textures that are composed of periodically repeating grooves have a clear orientation defined by the grooves. The direction of finger movement relative to texture orientation determines the availability of temporal cues to the spatial period of the texture. These cues are absent during movements directed in line with texture orientation, whereas movements orthogonal to texture orientation maximize the temporal frequency of stimulation. This may optimize temporal cues. In Experiment 1 we tested whether texture perception gets more precise the more orthogonal the movement direction is to the texture. We systematically varied the movement direction within a 2IFC spatial period discrimination task. As expected, perception was more precise (lower discrimination thresholds) when finger movements were directed closer towards the texture orthogonal as compared to in parallel to the texture. In Experiment 2 we investigated whether people adjust movement directions to the texture orthogonal in free exploration. We recorded movement directions during free exploration of standard and comparison gratings. The standard gratings were clearly oriented. The comparison gratings did not have a clear orientation defined by grooves. Participants adjusted movement directions to the texture orthogonal only for clearly oriented textures (standards). The adjustment to texture orthogonal was present in the final movement but not in the first movement. This suggests that movement adjustment is based on sensory signals for texture orientation that were gathered over the course of exploration. In Experiment 3 we assessed whether the perception of texture orientation and movement adjustments are based on shared sensory signals. We determined perceptual thresholds for orientation discrimination and computed 'movometric' thresholds from the stroke-by-stroke adjustment of movement direction. Perception and movements were influenced by a common factor, the spatial period, suggesting that the same sensory signals for texture orientation contribute to both. We conclude that people optimize texture perception by adjusting their movements in directions that maximize temporal cue frequency. Adjustments are performed on the basis of sensory signals that are also used for perception.
Collapse
|
32
|
Top-down modulation of shape and roughness discrimination in active touch by covert attention. Atten Percept Psychophys 2018; 81:462-475. [PMID: 30506325 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-018-1625-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Due to limitations in perceptual processing, information relevant to momentary task goals is selected from the vast amount of available sensory information by top-down mechanisms (e.g. attention) that can increase perceptual performance. We investigated how covert attention affects perception of 3D objects in active touch. In our experiment, participants simultaneously explored the shape and roughness of two objects in sequence, and were told afterwards to compare the two objects with regard to one of the two features. To direct the focus of covert attention to the different features we manipulated the expectation of a shape or roughness judgment by varying the frequency of trials for each task (20%, 50%, 80%), then we measured discrimination thresholds. We found higher discrimination thresholds for both shape and roughness perception when the task was unexpected, compared to the conditions in which the task was expected (or both tasks were expected equally). Our results suggest that active touch perception is modulated by expectations about the task. This implies that despite fundamental differences, active and passive touch are affected by feature selective covert attention in a similar way.
Collapse
|
33
|
Lezkan A, Metzger A, Drewing K. Active Haptic Exploration of Softness: Indentation Force Is Systematically Related to Prediction, Sensation and Motivation. Front Integr Neurosci 2018; 12:59. [PMID: 30555306 PMCID: PMC6281961 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2018] [Accepted: 11/16/2018] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Active finger movements play a crucial role in natural haptic perception. For the perception of different haptic properties people use different well-chosen movement schemes (Lederman and Klatzky, 1987). The haptic property of softness is stereotypically judged by repeatedly pressing one’s finger against an objects’ surface, actively indenting the object. It has been shown that people adjust the peak indentation forces of their pressing movements to the expected stimulus’ softness in order to improve perception (Kaim and Drewing, 2011). Here, we aim to clarify the mechanisms underlying such adjustments. We disentangle how people modulate executed peak indentation forces depending on predictive vs. sensory signals to softness, and investigate the influence of the participants’ motivational state on movement adjustments. In Experiment 1, participants performed a two alternative forced-choice (2AFC) softness discrimination task for stimulus pairs from one of four softness categories. We manipulated the predictability of the softness category. Either all stimuli of the same category were presented in a blocked fashion, which allowed predicting the softness category of the upcoming pair (predictive signals high), or stimuli from different categories were randomly intermixed, which made prediction impossible (predictive signals low). Sensory signals to softness category of the two stimuli in a pair are gathered during exploration. We contrasted the first indentation (sensory signals low) and last indentation (sensory signals high) in order to examine the effect of sensory signals. The results demonstrate that participants systematically apply lower forces when softer objects (as compared to harder objects) are indicated by predictive signals. Notably, sensory signals seemed to be not as relevant as predictive signals. However, in Experiment 2, we manipulated participant motivation by introducing rewards for good performance, and showed that the use of sensory information for movement adjustments can be fostered by high motivation. Overall, the present study demonstrates that exploratory movements are adjusted to the actual perceptual situation and that in the process of fine-tuning, closed- and open-loop mechanisms interact, with varying contributions depending on the observer’s motivation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Lezkan
- Department of General Psychology, Justus-Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Anna Metzger
- Department of General Psychology, Justus-Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Knut Drewing
- Department of General Psychology, Justus-Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Gerling GJ, Hauser SC, Soltis BR, Bowen AK, Fanta KD, Wang Y. A Standard Methodology to Characterize the Intrinsic Material Properties of Compliant Test Stimuli. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2018; 11:498-508. [PMID: 29993841 PMCID: PMC6396288 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2018.2825396] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/06/2023]
Abstract
Understanding how we perceive differences in material compliance, or 'softness,' is a central topic in the field of haptics. The intrinsic elasticity of an object is the primary factor thought to influence our perceptual estimates. Therefore, most studies test and report the elasticity of their stimuli, typically as stiffness or modulus. However, many reported estimates are of very high magnitude for silicone-elastomers, which may be due to artifacts in characterization technique. This makes it very difficult to compare the perceptual results between the studies. The work herein defines a standardized and easy-to-implement way to characterize test stimuli. The procedure involves the unconstrained, uniaxial compression of a plate into cylindrical substrates 10 mm tall by 10 mm diameter. The resultant force-displacement data are straightforwardly converted into stress-strain data, from which a modulus is readily derived. This procedure was used to re-characterize stimuli from prior studies. The revised results from the validated method herein are 200-1,100 percent lower than modulus values either reported and/or approximated from stiffness. This is practically significant when differences of 10-15 percent are perceptually discriminable. The re-characterized estimates are useful in comparing prior studies and designing new studies. Furthermore, this characterization methodology may help more readily bridge studies on perception with those designing technology.
Collapse
|
35
|
Xu C, Wang Y, Hauser SC, Gerling GJ. In the Tactile Discrimination of Compliance, Perceptual Cues in Addition to Contact Area Are Required. PROCEEDINGS OF THE HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY ... ANNUAL MEETING. HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY. ANNUAL MEETING 2018; 62:1535-1539. [PMID: 31787831 PMCID: PMC6884142 DOI: 10.1177/1541931218621347] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
In our ability to discriminate compliant, or 'soft,' objects, we rely upon information acquired from interactions at the finger pad. We have yet to resolve the most pertinent perceptual cues. However, doing so is vital for building effective, dynamic displays. By introducing psychophysical illusions through spheres of various size and elasticity, we investigate the utility of contact area cues, thought to be key in encoding compliance. For both active and passive touch, we determine finger pad-to-stimulus contact areas, using an ink-based procedure, as well as discrimination thresholds. The findings indicate that in passive touch, participants cannot discriminate certain small compliant versus large stiff spheres, which generate similar contact areas. In active touch, however, participants easily discriminate these spheres, though contact areas remain similar. Supplementary cues based on stimulus rate and/or proprioception seem vital. One cue that does differ for illusion cases is finger displacement given a volitionally applied force.
Collapse
|
36
|
Hauser SC, Gerling GJ. Imaging the 3-D Deformation of the Finger Pad When Interacting with Compliant Materials. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM : [PROCEEDINGS]. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM 2018; 2018:7-13. [PMID: 31080839 DOI: 10.1109/haptics.2018.8357145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
We need to understand the physics of how the skin of the finger pad deforms, and their tie to perception, to accurately reproduce a sense of compliance, or 'softness,' in tactile displays. Contact interactions with compliant materials are distinct from those with rigid surfaces where the skin flattens completely. To capture unique patterns in skin deformation over a range of compliances, we developed a stereo imaging technique to visualize the skin through optically clear stimuli. Accompanying algorithms serve to locate and track points marked with ink on the skin, correct for light refraction through stimuli, and estimate aspects of contact between skin and stimulus surfaces. The method achieves a 3-D spatial resolution of 60-120 microns and temporal resolution of 30 frames per second. With human subjects, we measured the skin's deformation over a range of compliances (61-266 kPa), displacements (0-4 mm), and velocities (1- 15 mm/s). The results indicate that the method can differentiate patterns of skin deformation between compliances, as defined by metrics including surface penetration depth, retention of geometric shape, and force per gross contact area. Observations of biomechanical cues of this sort are key to understanding the perceptual encoding of compliance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven C Hauser
- Departments of Systems and Information Engineering and Biomedical Engineering at the University of Virginia, USA
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- Departments of Systems and Information Engineering and Biomedical Engineering at the University of Virginia, USA
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Hauser SC, Gerling GJ, Hauser SC, Gerling GJ, Gerling GJ, Hauser SC. Force-Rate Cues Reduce Object Deformation Necessary to Discriminate Compliances Harder than the Skin. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2018; 11:232-240. [PMID: 28641270 PMCID: PMC6020043 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2017.2715845] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Grasping and manipulating an object requires us to perceive its material compliance. Compliance is thought to be encoded by relationships of force, displacement, and contact area at the finger pad. Prior work suggests that objects must be sufficiently deformed to become discriminable, but the utility of time-dependent cues has not been fully explored. The studies herein find that the availability of force-rate cues improve compliance discriminability so as to require less deformation of stimulus and finger pad. In particular, we tested the impact of controlling force-rate and displacement-rate cues in passive touch psychophysical experiments. An ink-based method to mark the finger pad was used to measure contact area per stimulus, simultaneously with displacement and force. Compliances spanned a range harder and softer than the finger pad. The results indicated harder compliances were discriminable at lower peak forces when the stimulus control mode was displacement-rate (0.5 N) compared to force-rate (1.3 N). That is, when displacement-rate was controlled to be equal between the two compliances, the resultant force-rate psychophysical cues could be more readily discriminated. In extending prior studies, while some magnitude of finger pad deformation may be sufficient for discriminability, temporal cues tied to force afford more efficient judgments.
Collapse
|
38
|
Yokosaka T, Kuroki S, Watanabe J, Nishida S, Yokosaka T, Kuroki S, Watanabe J, Nishida S, Kuroki S, Watanabe J, Yokosaka T, Nishida S. Estimating Tactile Perception by Observing Explorative Hand Motion of Others. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2018; 11:192-203. [PMID: 29911978 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2017.2775631] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
When we acquire tactile information about an object's surface, we actively move our hands. Past studies have shown a correlation between participants' (i.e., touchers') hand motion in tactile exploration and the evaluated tactile attributes of the object, which suggests that tactile perception can be estimated from statistical analysis of touchers' hand motion. Though it has been reported that the statistical analysis of hand motion can indeed estimate tactile perception, whether humans can estimate tactile perception by observing the hand motion of others remains unclear. To investigate this, we conducted experiments wherein observers watched point-light moving hands of touchers in tactile exploration and evaluated the material being touched. Our results show that, although observers' estimation of touchers' perception was not accurate, observers extracted information from touchers' hand motion for estimation, and the correlations within observers' estimation were high. These results suggest that human observers can estimate tactile perception through visual observation of the hand motion of others by adopting common strategies about the relationships between touchers' hand motion and tactile perception.
Collapse
|
39
|
Abstract
Where textures are defined by repetitive small spatial structures, exploration covering a greater extent will lead to signal repetition. We investigated how sensory estimates derived from these signals are integrated. In Experiment 1, participants stroked with the index finger one to eight times across two virtual gratings. Half of the participants discriminated according to ridge amplitude, the other half according to ridge spatial period. In both tasks, just noticeable differences (JNDs) decreased with an increasing number of strokes. Those gains from additional exploration were more than three times smaller than predicted for optimal observers who have access to equally reliable, and therefore equally weighted, estimates for the entire exploration. We assume that the sequential nature of the exploration leads to memory decay of sensory estimates. Thus, participants compare an overall estimate of the first stimulus, which is affected by memory decay, to stroke-specific estimates during the exploration of the second stimulus. This was tested in Experiments 2 and 3. The spatial period of one stroke across either the first or second of two sequentially presented gratings was slightly discrepant from periods in all other strokes. This allowed calculating weights of stroke-specific estimates in the overall percept. As predicted, weights were approximately equal for all strokes in the first stimulus, while weights decreased during the exploration of the second stimulus. A quantitative Kalman filter model of our assumptions was consistent with the data. Hence, our results support an optimal integration model for sequential information given that memory decay affects comparison processes.
Collapse
|
40
|
Yokosaka T, Kuroki S, Watanabe J, Nishida S. Linkage between Free Exploratory Movements and Subjective Tactile Ratings. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS 2017; 10:217-225. [PMID: 27810836 DOI: 10.1109/toh.2016.2613055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
We actively move our hands and eyes when exploring the external world and gaining information about object's attributes. Previous studies showing that how we touch might be related to how we felt led us to consider whether we could decode observers' subjective tactile experiences only by analyzing their exploratory movements without explicitly asking how they perceived. However, in those studies, explicit judgment tasks were performed about specific tactile attributes that were prearranged by experimenters. Here, we systematically investigated whether exploratory movements can explain tactile ratings even when participants do not need to judge any tactile attributes. While measuring both hand and eye movements, we asked participants to touch materials freely without judging any specific tactile attributes (free-touch task) or to evaluate one of four tactile attributes (roughness, hardness, slipperiness, and temperature). We found that tactile ratings in the judgment tasks correlated with exploratory movements even in the free-touch task and that eye movements as well as hand movements correlated with tactile ratings. These results might open up the possibility of decoding tactile experiences by exploratory movements.
Collapse
|
41
|
Endo H. Pressing movements and perceived force and displacement are influenced by object stiffness. Physiol Behav 2016; 163:203-210. [PMID: 27188980 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2016] [Revised: 05/12/2016] [Accepted: 05/13/2016] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Despite many previous studies on stiffness perception, few have investigated the exploratory procedures involved. This study evaluated whether stiffness range influences pressing movements and perception of force and displacement during stiffness discrimination tasks. Force and displacement data were obtained from 30 participants. Peak values of force and displacement, pressing duration and number of presses were analyzed. Two kinds of subjective evaluations were also recorded: perceived difference in force/displacement used to discriminate between specimens, and perceived effort. Although the number of presses and pressing duration were constant across a wide stiffness range, pressing strength was adjusted for the stiffness of objects, with harder specimens pressed more strongly. Further, even if the stiffnesses of two compared specimens were different, the pressing forces applied to the specimens approached the same magnitude at a higher stiffness range. Differences in force were most easily perceived at lower stiffness ranges, while displacement differences were perceived more readily at higher stiffness ranges. These results were consistent with those of previous studies. Finally, the reasons why stiffness range influenced pressing movements and perceived differences in force/displacement are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hiroshi Endo
- Human Informatics Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Central 6, 1-1 Higashi, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8566, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Wang Y, Baba Y, Lumpkin EA, Gerling GJ. Computational modeling indicates that surface pressure can be reliably conveyed to tactile receptors even amidst changes in skin mechanics. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116:218-28. [PMID: 27098029 PMCID: PMC4961760 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00624.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2015] [Accepted: 04/18/2016] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Distinct patterns in neuronal firing are observed between classes of cutaneous afferents. Such differences may be attributed to end-organ morphology, distinct ion-channel complements, and skin microstructure, among other factors. Even for just the slowly adapting type I afferent, the skin's mechanics for a particular specimen might impact the afferent's firing properties, especially given the thickness and elasticity of skin can change dramatically over just days. Here, we show computationally that the skin can reliably convey indentation magnitude, rate, and spatial geometry to the locations of tactile receptors even amid changes in skin's structure. Using finite element analysis and neural dynamics models, we considered the skin properties of six mice that span a representative cohort. Modeling the propagation of the surface stimulus to the interior of the skin demonstrated that there can be large variance in stresses and strains near the locations of tactile receptors, which can lead to large variance in static firing rate. However, variance is significantly reduced when the stimulus tip is controlled by surface pressure and compressive stress is measured near the end organs. This particular transformation affords the least variability in predicted firing rates compared with others derived from displacement, force, strain energy density, or compressive strain. Amid changing skin mechanics, stimulus control by surface pressure may be more naturalistic and optimal and underlie how animals actively explore the tactile environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuxiang Wang
- Department of Systems and Information Engineering, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia; Department of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia
| | - Yoshichika Baba
- Department of Dermatology, Columbia University College of Physicians & Surgeons, New York, New York; and
| | - Ellen A Lumpkin
- Department of Dermatology, Columbia University College of Physicians & Surgeons, New York, New York; and Department of Physiology & Cellular Biophysics, Columbia University College of Physicians & Surgeons, New York, New York
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- Department of Systems and Information Engineering, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia; Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia;
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Hauser SC, Gerling GJ. Measuring tactile cues at the fingerpad for object compliances harder and softer than the skin. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM : [PROCEEDINGS]. IEEE HAPTICS SYMPOSIUM 2016; 2016:247-252. [PMID: 27331072 DOI: 10.1109/haptics.2016.7463185] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Distinguishing an object's compliance, into percepts of "softness" and "hardness," is crucial to our ability to grasp and manipulate it. Biomechanical cues at the skin's surface such as contact area and force rate have been thought to help encode compliance. However, no one has directly measured contact area with compliant materials, and few studies have considered compliances softer than the fingerpad. Herein, we developed a novel method to precisely measure the area in contact between compliant stimuli and the fingerpad, at given levels of force and displacement. To determine the method's robustness, we conducted psychophysical and biomechanical experiments with human subjects. The results indicate that cues including contact area at stimulus peak force of 3 Newtons, force rate over stimulus movement and at peak force, displacement and/or time to reach peak force may help in discriminating compliances while the directional spread of contact area is less important. Between softer and harder compliances, some cues were slightly more evident, though not yet definitively. Based upon the method's utility, the next step is to conduct broader experiments to distill the mixture of cues that encode compliance. The importance of such work lies in building haptic displays, for example, to render virtual tissues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven C Hauser
- Graduate student in Biomedical Engineering at the University of Virginia, VA 22904 USA
| | - Gregory J Gerling
- Associate professor in Systems and Information Engineering at the University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904 USA
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Jamieson ES, Chandler JH, Culmer PR, Manogue M, Mon-Williams M, Wilkie RM. Can virtual reality trainers improve the compliance discrimination abilities of trainee surgeons? ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE OF THE IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE 2015; 2015:466-469. [PMID: 26736300 DOI: 10.1109/embc.2015.7318400] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
The assessment of tissue compliance using a handheld tool is an important skill in medical areas such as laparoscopic and dental surgery. The increasing prevalence of virtual reality devices raises the question of whether we can exploit these systems to accelerate the training of compliance discrimination in trainee surgeons. We used a haptic feedback device and stylus to assess the abilities of naïve participants to detect compliance differences with and without knowledge of results (KR) (groups 1 and 2), as well as the abilities of participants who had undergone repetitive training over several days (group 3). Kinematic analyses were carried out to objectively measure the probing action. Untrained participants had poor detection thresholds (mean just noticeable difference, JND = 33%), and we found no effect of KR (provided after each trial) on performance (mean JND = 35%). Intensive training dramatically improved group performance (mean JND = 12%). Probing action (in particular, slower movement execution) was associated with better detection thresholds, but training did not lead to systematic changes in probing behaviour. These findings set a benchmark for training systems that act to increase perceptual sensitivity and guide the learner toward optimal movement strategies to improve discrimination.
Collapse
|
45
|
Tactile sensor using acoustic reflection for lump detection in laparoscopic surgery. Int J Comput Assist Radiol Surg 2014; 10:183-93. [DOI: 10.1007/s11548-014-1067-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2014] [Accepted: 04/22/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
46
|
Wismeijer DA, Gegenfurtner KR, Drewing K. Learning from vision-to-touch is different than learning from touch-to-vision. Front Integr Neurosci 2012. [PMID: 23181012 PMCID: PMC3501647 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We studied whether vision can teach touch to the same extent as touch seems to teach vision. In a 2 × 2 between-participants learning study, we artificially correlated visual gloss cues with haptic compliance cues. In two “natural” tasks, we tested whether visual gloss estimations have an influence on haptic estimations of softness and vice versa. In two “novel” tasks, in which participants were either asked to haptically judge glossiness or to visually judge softness, we investigated how perceptual estimates transfer from one sense to the other. Our results showed that vision does not teach touch as efficient as touch seems to teach vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dagmar A Wismeijer
- Allgemeine Psychologie, Justus-Liebig Universität Gießen Gießen, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Drewing K. After experience with the task humans actively optimize shape discrimination in touch by utilizing effects of exploratory movement direction. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2012; 141:295-303. [PMID: 23079190 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.09.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2012] [Revised: 08/08/2012] [Accepted: 09/13/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The active control of exploratory movements is an integral part of active touch. We investigated and manipulated the relationship between the haptic discrimination performance for small bumps and the direction of exploratory movements relative to the body. Shape discrimination performance varied with the direction of stimulus exploration. Experimental manipulations successfully changed the normative relation between exploratory direction and discrimination performance. If participants were rewarded for "good perceptual performance" and had the choice, they displayed clear strategic preferences for exploratory directions that yield optimal performance-but only after having extensive experience with the changed perceptual conditions. Overall, the findings suggest that participants can actively adapt their exploratory movements in order to optimize haptic discrimination performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Knut Drewing
- Institute for Psychology, Giessen-University, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|