1
|
Li L, Song M, Cai Q. The Words Children Hear and See: Lexical Diversity Across-Modalities and Its Impact on Lexical Development. Dev Sci 2025; 28:e13601. [PMID: 39740222 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13601] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2024] [Revised: 11/08/2024] [Accepted: 12/02/2024] [Indexed: 01/02/2025]
Abstract
Early vocabulary development benefits from diverse lexical exposures within children's language environment. However, the influence of lexical diversity on children as they enter middle childhood and are exposed to multimodal language inputs remains unclear. This study evaluates global and local aspects of lexical diversity in three 1.6-million-word child-directed corpora, representing average Chinese children's speech, print, and media language environments. Additionally, pseudo-multimodal samples were compiled from the three corpora to compare with the unimodal environments on lexical diversity. We then investigated the associations between lexical diversity and the acquisition of 361 words spanning early-to-middle childhood. The findings show that print and pseudo-multimodal language provided the most diverse lexical environments, whereas speech exhibited the least diversity. However, speech diversity most strongly predicted lexical development, particularly before the onset of middle childhood. Exploratory analysis revealed that lexical diversity of other modalities emerged as stronger predictors thereafter. Early lexical development was best predicted by words' variations in connectivity with other words within an immediate context, whereas in middle childhood, variations in words' occurrences in larger context windows became the primary predictor, implicating children's growing ability to attend to linguistic contexts of increasing sizes. Importantly, higher diversity was consistently associated with earlier word acquisition across measures and developmental phases. These findings underscore the critical role of varied lexical experiences in children's language development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luan Li
- Speech-Language-Hearing Center, School of Foreign Languages, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
- Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), Affiliated Mental Health Center (ECNU), Institute of Brain and Education Innovation, School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
- National Research Centre for Language and Well-Being, Shanghai, China
| | - Ming Song
- Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), Affiliated Mental Health Center (ECNU), Institute of Brain and Education Innovation, School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
- Shanghai Changning Mental Health Center, Shanghai, China
- Shanghai Center for Brain Science and Brain-Inspired Technology, Shanghai, China
| | - Qing Cai
- Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), Affiliated Mental Health Center (ECNU), Institute of Brain and Education Innovation, School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
- Shanghai Changning Mental Health Center, Shanghai, China
- Shanghai Center for Brain Science and Brain-Inspired Technology, Shanghai, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Wojcik EH, Goulding SJ. Distribution of words across the first years of life: A longitudinal analysis of everyday language input to three English-learning infants. INFANCY 2025; 30:e12622. [PMID: 39317965 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12622] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/19/2024] [Revised: 08/16/2024] [Accepted: 08/23/2024] [Indexed: 09/26/2024]
Abstract
Many in-lab studies have demonstrated that the distribution of word learning moments affects the strength and quality of word representations. How are words distributed in speech to children in their daily lives, and how is distribution related to other input characteristics? The present study analyzes transcripts of language input to English-learning infants from three longitudinal, naturalistic corpora captured between 6 and 39 months of age. To describe how word frequency varies across time, we calculated dispersion scores for all word types for each child. Dispersion quantifies the deviation of observed frequencies in each recording session from expected (uniform across sessions) word frequency, providing a measure of how evenly word utterances were spread across sessions. Dispersion is strongly correlated with frequency and moderately correlated with concreteness across all corpora, such that high frequency and low concreteness words are more evenly dispersed. Correlations with measures of age of acquisition (AoA) varied across corpora, and dispersion did not reliably predict AoA above and beyond frequency and concreteness. The contradiction between the current results and results from in-lab experiments is discussed. This study provides a foundation to explore how word learning unfolds across time and contexts in the real world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erica H Wojcik
- Department of Psychology, Skidmore College, Saratoga Springs, New York, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Nencheva ML, Schwab JF, Lew-Williams C, Fausey CM. Word Repetition and Isolation are Intertwined in Children's Early Language Experiences. Open Mind (Camb) 2024; 8:1330-1347. [PMID: 39654817 PMCID: PMC11627589 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2024] [Accepted: 10/01/2024] [Indexed: 12/12/2024] Open
Abstract
Infants experience language in the context of a dynamic environment in which many cues co-occur. However, experimenters often reduce language input to individual cues a priori without considering how children themselves may experience incoming information, leading to potentially inaccurate conclusions about how learning works outside of the lab. Here, we examined the shared temporal dynamics of two historically separated cues that are thought to support word learning: repetition of the same word in nearby utterances, and isolation of individual word tokens (i.e., single-word utterances). In a large database of North American English, we found that word repetition and isolation frequently co-occurred in children's natural language experiences, and the extent to which they did so was linked to words' earlier age of acquisition. This investigation emphasizes children's experiences in time as a way to understand the learning cues in the language environment, which may help researchers build learning theories that are grounded in real-world structure.
Collapse
|
4
|
Farrow J, Wasik BA, Hindman AH. Exploring the relations between teachers' high-quality language features and preschoolers and kindergarteners' vocabulary learning. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2024:1-29. [PMID: 39512097 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000485] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2024]
Abstract
This study explored the use of sophisticated vocabulary, complex syntax, and decontextualized language (including book information, conceptual information, past/future experiences, and vocabulary information) in teachers' instructional interactions with children during the literacy block in prekindergarten and kindergarten classrooms. The sample included 33 teachers and 421 children. We examined correlations among these language features and their unique contributions to children's vocabulary learning. Teachers who used more sophisticated vocabulary also engaged in more decontextualized talk about vocabulary and past/future experiences. Additionally, teachers' use of complex syntax was uniquely associated with talk about conceptual information. Both complex syntax and conceptual information talk predicted children's vocabulary learning; however, complex syntax emerged as the sole predictor when accounting for this relationship. This finding suggests that decontextualized talk about concepts, characterized by complex language structures, may facilitate vocabulary acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- JeanMarie Farrow
- College of Education and Human Development, Georgia State University
| | - Barbara A Wasik
- College of Education and Human Development, Temple University
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Green C, Keogh K, Sun H, O'Brien B. The Children's Picture Books Lexicon (CPB-LEX): A large-scale lexical database from children's picture books. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:4504-4521. [PMID: 37566336 PMCID: PMC11289352 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02198-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/10/2023] [Indexed: 08/12/2023]
Abstract
This article presents CPB-LEX, a large-scale database of lexical statistics derived from children's picture books (age range 0-8 years). Such a database is essential for research in psychology, education and computational modelling, where rich details on the vocabulary of early print exposure are required. CPB-LEX was built through an innovative method of computationally extracting lexical information from automatic speech-to-text captions and subtitle tracks generated from social media channels dedicated to reading picture books aloud. It consists of approximately 25,585 types (wordforms) and their frequency norms (raw and Zipf-transformed), a lexicon of bigrams (two-word sequences and their transitional probabilities) and a document-term matrix (which shows the importance of each word in the corpus in each book). Several immediate contributions of CPB-LEX to behavioural science research are reported, including that the new CPB-LEX frequency norms strongly predict age of acquisition and outperform comparable child-input lexical databases. The database allows researchers and practitioners to extract lexical statistics for high-frequency words which can be used to develop word lists. The paper concludes with an investigation of how CPB-LEX can be used to extend recent modelling research on the lexical diversity children receive from picture books in addition to child-directed speech. Our model shows that the vocabulary input from a relatively small number of picture books can dramatically enrich vocabulary exposure from child-directed speech and potentially assist children with vocabulary input deficits. The database is freely available from the Open Science Framework repository: https://tinyurl.com/4este73c .
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clarence Green
- Faculty of Education, University of Hong Kong, Pok Fu Lam, Hong Kong
| | - Kathleen Keogh
- Senior Lecturer, Centre for Smart Analytics & Institute of Innovation, Science and Sustainability, Federation University Australia, Mount Helen, Australia.
| | - He Sun
- Centre for Research in Child Language, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Beth O'Brien
- Centre for Research in Child Development, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Ratner NB, Han Y, Yang JS. Should We Stop Using Lexical Diversity Measures in Children's Language Sample Analysis? AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2024; 33:1986-2001. [PMID: 38838249 PMCID: PMC11253636 DOI: 10.1044/2024_ajslp-23-00457] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2023] [Revised: 03/09/2024] [Accepted: 04/11/2024] [Indexed: 06/07/2024]
Abstract
PURPOSE Prior work has identified weaknesses in commonly used indices of lexical diversity in spoken language samples, such as type-token ratio (TTR) due to sample size and elicitation variation, we explored whether TTR and other diversity measures, such as number of different words/100 (NDW), vocabulary diversity (VocD), and the moving average TTR would be more sensitive to child age and clinical status (typically developing [TD] or developmental language disorder [DLD]) if samples were obtained from standardized prompts. METHOD We utilized archival data from the norming samples of the Test of Narrative Language and the Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument. We examined lexical diversity and other linguistic properties of the samples, from a total of 1,048 children, ages 4-11 years; 798 of these were considered TD, whereas 250 were categorized as having a language learning disorder. RESULTS TTR was the least sensitive to child age or diagnostic group, with good potential to misidentify children with DLD as TD and TD children as having DLD. Growth slopes of NDW were shallow and not very sensitive to diagnostic grouping. The strongest performing measure was VocD. Mean length of utterance, TNW, and verbs/utterance did show both good growth trajectories and ability to distinguish between clinical and typical samples. CONCLUSIONS This study, the largest and best controlled to date, re-affirms that TTR should not be used in clinical decision making with children. A second popular measure, NDW, is not measurably stronger in terms of its psychometric properties. Because the most sensitive measure of lexical diversity, VocD, is unlikely to gain popularity because of reliance on computer-assisted analysis, we suggest alternatives for the appraisal of children's expressive vocabulary skill.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nan Bernstein Ratner
- Hearing and Speech Sciences, Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD
| | - Youngjin Han
- Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, University of Maryland, College Park, MD
| | - Ji Seung Yang
- Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, University of Maryland, College Park, MD
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Casey K, Potter CE, Lew-Williams C, Wojcik EH. Moving beyond "nouns in the lab": Using naturalistic data to understand why infants' first words include uh-oh and hi. Dev Psychol 2023; 59:2162-2173. [PMID: 37824228 PMCID: PMC10872816 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001630] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2023]
Abstract
Why do infants learn some words earlier than others? Many theories of early word learning focus on explaining how infants map labels onto concrete objects. However, words that are more abstract than object nouns, such as uh-oh, hi, more, up, and all-gone, are typically among the first to appear in infants' vocabularies. We combined a behavioral experiment with naturalistic observational research to explore how infants learn and represent this understudied category of high-frequency, routine-based non-nouns, which we term "everyday words." In Study 1, we found that a conventional eye-tracking measure of comprehension was insufficient to capture U.S.-based English-learning 10- to 16-month-old infants' emerging understanding of everyday words. In Study 2, we analyzed the visual and social scenes surrounding caregivers' and infants' use of everyday words in a naturalistic video corpus. This ecologically motivated research revealed that everyday words rarely co-occurred with consistent visual referents, making their early learnability difficult to reconcile with dominant word learning theories. Our findings instead point to complex patterns in the types of situations associated with everyday words that could contribute to their early representation in infants' vocabularies. By leveraging both experimental and observational methods, this investigation underscores the value of using naturalistic data to broaden theories of early learning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
8
|
Knabe ML, Schonberg CC, Vlach HA. Does the public know what researchers know? Perceived task difficulty impacts adults' intuitions about children's early word learning. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2023; 8:45. [PMID: 37486427 PMCID: PMC10366060 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-023-00493-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2022] [Accepted: 06/19/2023] [Indexed: 07/25/2023] Open
Abstract
The present study examined adults' understanding of children's early word learning. Undergraduates, non-parents, parents, and Speech-Language Pathologists (N = 535, 74% female, 56% White) completed a survey with 11 word learning principles from the perspective of a preschooler. Questions tested key principles from early word learning research. For each question, participants were prompted to select an answer based on the perspective of a preschooler. Adults demonstrated aligned intuitions for all principles except those derived from domain-general theories, regardless of experience with language development (Experiment 1). Experiment 2 revealed that perceived difficulty of a task for a preschooler impacted adults' reasoning about word learning processes. Experiment 3 ruled out level of confidence and interest as mechanisms to explain the results. These results highlight disconnects in knowledge between the cognitive development research community and the general public. Therefore, efforts must be made to communicate scientific findings to the broader non-academic community, emphasizing children's ability to excel at word learning in the face of task difficulty.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melina L Knabe
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1025 W. Johnson St., Madison, WI, 53703, USA.
| | - Christina C Schonberg
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1025 W. Johnson St., Madison, WI, 53703, USA
- IXL, 777 Mariners Island Blvd., Suite 600, San Mateo, CA, 94404, USA
| | - Haley A Vlach
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1025 W. Johnson St., Madison, WI, 53703, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Gámez PB, Palermo F, Perry JS, Galindo M. Spanish-English bilingual toddlers' vocabulary skills: The role of caregiver language input and warmth. Dev Sci 2023; 26:e13308. [PMID: 35913423 PMCID: PMC10644905 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13308] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2021] [Revised: 06/07/2022] [Accepted: 06/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
There is a well-documented link between bilingual language development and the relative amounts of exposure to each language. Less is known about the role of quality indicators of caregiver-child interactions in bilingual homes, including caregiver input diversity, warmth and sensitivity. This longitudinal study examines the relation between caregiver input (lexical diversity, amount), warmth and sensitivity and bilingual toddlers' subsequent vocabulary outcomes. We video-recorded caregiver-child interactions in Spanish-English Latino homes when toddlers (n = 47) were 18 months of age (M = 18.32 months; SD = 1.02 months). At the 24-month follow-up, we measured children's vocabulary as total vocabulary (English, Spanish combined) as well as within language (Spanish, English). Results revealed that Spanish lexical diversity exposure at 18 months from caregivers was positively associated with children's Spanish and total vocabulary scores at 24 months, while English lexical diversity was positively associated with children's English scores; lexical diversity and amount were highly correlated. Additionally, caregivers' warmth was positively associated with children's Spanish, English and total vocabulary scores. Together, these factors accounted for substantial variance (30-40%) in vocabulary outcomes. Notably, caregiver input accounted for more variance in single language outcomes than did caregiver warmth, whereas caregiver warmth uniquely accounted for more variance in total vocabulary scores. Our findings extend prior research findings by suggesting that children's dual language development may depend on their exposure to a diverse set of words, not only amount of language exposure, as well as warm interactions with caregivers. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/q1V_7fz5wog HIGHLIGHTS: Video-recorded observations of caregiver-child interactions revealed warmth and high sensitivity from Latino caregivers. Linguistically-detailed analyses of caregiver input revealed wide variation in the diversity of Spanish and English directed at 18-month-old bilingual toddlers. Bilingual toddlers' vocabulary (single language, total) was positively associated with caregivers' diverse input and warmth, thus extending prior findings on bilinguals' amount of language exposure. Findings suggest that caregivers' lexical diversity explains more variance in bilingual toddlers' single language outcomes, whereas warmth explains more variance in total vocabulary scores.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Perla B Gámez
- Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, USA
| | - Francisco Palermo
- College of Education and Human Development, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA
| | - Jordan S Perry
- Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, USA
| | - Maily Galindo
- Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Karmazyn-Raz H, Smith LB. Sampling statistics are like story creation: a network analysis of parent-toddler exploratory play. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20210358. [PMID: 36571129 PMCID: PMC9791483 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0358] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2022] [Accepted: 09/04/2022] [Indexed: 12/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Actions in the world elicit data for learning and do so in a stream of interconnected events. Here, we provide evidence on how toddlers with their parent sample information by acting on toys during exploratory play. We observed 10 min of free-flowing and unconstrained object exploration of by toddlers (mean age 21 months) and parents in a room with many available objects (n = 32). Borrowing concepts and measures from the study of narratives, we found that the toy selections are not a string of unrelated events but exhibit a suite of what we call coherence statistics: Zipfian distributions, burstiness and a network structure. We discuss the transient memory processes that underlie the moment-to-moment toy selections that create this coherence and the role of these statistics in the development of abstract and generalizable systems of knowledge. This article is part of the theme issue 'Concepts in interaction: social engagement and inner experiences'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hadar Karmazyn-Raz
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47401, USA
| | - Linda B. Smith
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47401, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Slone LK, Abney DH, Smith LB, Yu C. The temporal structure of parent talk to toddlers about objects. Cognition 2023; 230:105266. [PMID: 36116401 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105266] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2021] [Revised: 08/24/2022] [Accepted: 08/25/2022] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
Toddlers learn words in the context of speech from adult social partners. The present studies quantitatively describe the temporal context of parent speech to toddlers about objects in individual real-world interactions. We show that at the temporal scale of a single play episode, parent talk to toddlers about individual objects is predominantly, but not always, clustered. Clustered speech is characterized by repeated references to the same object close in time, interspersed with lulls in speech about the object. Clustered temporal speech patterns mirror temporal patterns observed at longer timescales, and persisted regardless of play context. Moreover, clustered speech about individual novel objects predicted toddlers' learning of those objects' novel names. Clustered talk may be optimal for toddlers' word learning because it exploits domain-general principles of human memory and attention, principles that may have evolved precisely because of the clustered structure of natural events important to humans, including human behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren K Slone
- Indiana University, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 1101 E. 10th St., Bloomington, IN 47405-7007, USA; Hope College, Department of Psychology, Holland, MI 49423, USA.
| | - Drew H Abney
- Indiana University, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 1101 E. 10th St., Bloomington, IN 47405-7007, USA; University of Georgia, Department of Psychology, 125 Baldwin St., Athens, GA 30602, USA
| | - Linda B Smith
- Indiana University, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 1101 E. 10th St., Bloomington, IN 47405-7007, USA; University of East Anglia, School of Psychology, Norwich, Norfolk, UK
| | - Chen Yu
- Indiana University, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 1101 E. 10th St., Bloomington, IN 47405-7007, USA; University of Texas at Austin, Department of Psychology, Austin, TX 786712, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Lavi-Rotbain O, Arnon I. Zipfian Distributions in Child-Directed Speech. Open Mind (Camb) 2023; 7:1-30. [PMID: 36891353 PMCID: PMC9987348 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00070] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2022] [Accepted: 11/30/2022] [Indexed: 12/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Across languages, word frequency and rank follow a power law relation, forming a distribution known as the Zipfian distribution. There is growing experimental evidence that this well-studied phenomenon may be beneficial for language learning. However, most investigations of word distributions in natural language have focused on adult-to-adult speech: Zipf's law has not been thoroughly evaluated in child-directed speech (CDS) across languages. If Zipfian distributions facilitate learning, they should also be found in CDS. At the same time, several unique properties of CDS may result in a less skewed distribution. Here, we examine the frequency distribution of words in CDS in three studies. We first show that CDS is Zipfian across 15 languages from seven language families. We then show that CDS is Zipfian from early on (six-months) and across development for five languages with sufficient longitudinal data. Finally, we show that the distribution holds across different parts of speech: Nouns, verbs, adjectives and prepositions follow a Zipfian distribution. Together, the results show that the input children hear is skewed in a particular way from early on, providing necessary (but not sufficient) support for the postulated learning advantage of such skew. They highlight the need to study skewed learning environments experimentally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ori Lavi-Rotbain
- The Edmond and Lilly Safra Center for Brain Sciences, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Inbal Arnon
- Department of Psychology, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Macbeth A, Atagi N, Montag JL, Bruni MR, Chiarello C. Assessing language background and experiences among heritage bilinguals. Front Psychol 2022; 13:993669. [PMID: 36275266 PMCID: PMC9584748 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.993669] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2022] [Accepted: 09/12/2022] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
The language backgrounds and experiences of bilinguals have been primarily characterized using self-report questionnaires and laboratory tasks, although each of these assessments have their strengths and weaknesses. The Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR), an audio recording device, has recently become more prominent as a method of assessing real-world language use. We investigated the relationships among these three assessment tools, to understand the shared variance in how these measures evaluated various aspects of the bilingual experience. Participants were 60 Southern California heritage bilingual college students who spoke a variety of heritage languages and began to learn English between the ages of 0-to 12-years. Participants completed both self-report and laboratory-based measures of language proficiency and use, and they wore the EAR for 4 days to capture representative samples of their day-to-day heritage language (HL) use. The results indicated that self-reported HL use and English age of acquisition were significant predictors of real-world language use as measured by the EAR. In addition, self-reported HL proficiency and laboratory-based HL proficiency, as measured by verbal fluency, were mutually predictive. While some variability was shared across different assessments, ultimately, none of the measures correlated strongly and each measure captured unique information about the heritage bilingual language experience, highlighting the dissociation between language experience measured at a single point in time and an accumulated life history with a heritage language. These findings may provide guidance for bilingualism researchers about which assessment tool, or combination of tools, may be best for their specific research questions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alessandra Macbeth
- Department of Psychology, Azusa Pacific University, Azusa, CA, United States
| | - Natsuki Atagi
- Department of Child and Adolescent Studies, California State University, Fullerton, Fullerton, CA, United States
| | - Jessica L. Montag
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States
| | - Michelle R. Bruni
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, Riverside, CA, United States
| | - Christine Chiarello
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, Riverside, CA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Elmlinger SL, Park D, Schwade JA, Goldstein MH. Comparing Word Diversity Versus Amount of Speech in Parents’ Responses to Infants’ Prelinguistic Vocalizations. IEEE Trans Cogn Dev Syst 2022. [DOI: 10.1109/tcds.2021.3095766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Deokgun Park
- Department of Computer Science and Engineering, University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Mendoza JK, Fausey CM. Everyday Parameters for Episode-to-Episode Dynamics in the Daily Music of Infancy. Cogn Sci 2022; 46:e13178. [PMID: 35938844 PMCID: PMC9542518 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2021] [Revised: 04/12/2022] [Accepted: 05/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Experience-dependent change pervades early human development. Though trajectories of developmental change have been well charted in many domains, the episode-to-episode schedules of experiences on which they are hypothesized to depend have not. Here, we took up this issue in a domain known to be governed in part by early experiences: music. Using a corpus of longform audio recordings, we parameterized the daily schedules of music encountered by 35 infants ages 6-12 months. We discovered that everyday music episodes, as well as the interstices between episodes, typically persisted less than a minute, with most daily schedules also including some very extended episodes and interstices. We also discovered that infants encountered music episodes in a bursty rhythm, rather than a periodic or random rhythm, over the day. These findings join a suite of recent discoveries from everyday vision, motor, and language that expand our imaginations beyond artificial learning schedules and enable theorists to model the history-dependence of developmental process in ways that respect everyday sensory histories. Future theories about how infants build knowledge across multiple episodes can now be parameterized using these insights from infants' everyday lives.
Collapse
|
16
|
Karmazyn-Raz H, Smith LB. Discourse with Few Words: Coherence Statistics, Parent-Infant Actions on Objects, and Object Names. LANGUAGE ACQUISITION 2022; 30:211-229. [PMID: 37736139 PMCID: PMC10513098 DOI: 10.1080/10489223.2022.2054342] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2021] [Accepted: 03/05/2022] [Indexed: 09/23/2023]
Abstract
The data for early object name learning is often conceptualized as a problem of mapping heard names to referents. However, infants do not hear object names as discrete events but rather in extended interactions organized around goal-directed actions on objects. The present study examined the statistical structure of the nonlinguistic events that surround parent naming of objects. Parents and 12-month -old infants were left alone in a room for 10 minutes with 32 objects available for exploration. Parent and infant handling of objects and parent naming of objects were coded. The four measured statistics were from measures used in the study of coherent discourse: (1) a frequency distribution in which actions were frequently directed to a few objects and more rarely to other objects; (2) repeated returns to the high-frequency objects over the 10-minute play period; (3) clustered repetitions, continuity, of actions on objects; and (4) structured networks of transitions among objects in play that connected all the played-with objects. Parent naming was infrequent but related to the statistics of object-directed actions. The implications of the discourse-like stream of actions are discussed in terms of learning mechanisms that could support rapid learning of object names from relatively few name-object co-occurrences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Linda B Smith
- Indiana University, Bloomington, US
- University of East Anglia, Norfolk, UK
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
To CKS, McLeod S, Sam KL, Law T. Predicting Which Children Will Normalize Without Intervention for Speech Sound Disorders. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2022; 65:1724-1741. [PMID: 35381182 DOI: 10.1044/2022_jslhr-21-00444] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The speech of some children does not follow a typical normalization trajectory, and they develop speech sound disorders (SSD). This study investigated predictive correlates of speech sound normalization in children who were at risk of SSD. METHOD A prospective population cohort study of 845 Cantonese-speaking preschoolers was conducted over 2.5 years to examine (a) children who resolved nonadult realizations of consonants (normalized) and (b) those who had persisting speech sound difficulties (did not normalize). From these 845, a sample of 82 participants characterized as having SSD (1.25 SDs below the mean in a standardized speech assessment, with a delay in initial consonant acquisition or with one or more atypical errors) was followed for 2 years at 6-month intervals or until the completion of their initial consonant inventory. Data from 43 children who did not receive speech-language pathology services were analyzed with survival analysis to model time to normalization while controlling for covariates. The target event (outcome) was the completion of their initial consonant inventory. RESULTS Under the no-intervention condition, the estimated median time to normalization was 6.59 years of age. Children who were more likely to normalize or normalized in a shorter time were stimulable to all errors and more intelligible as rated by caregivers using the Intelligibility in Context Scale. Those who showed atypical error patterns did not necessarily take longer to normalize. Similarly, expressive language ability was not significantly associated with speech normalization. CONCLUSIONS Stimulability and intelligibility were more useful prognostic factors of speech normalization when compared to (a)typicality of error patterns and expressive language ability. Children with low intelligibility and poor stimulability should be prioritized for speech-language pathology services given that their speech errors are less likely to resolve naturally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Sharynne McLeod
- Charles Sturt University, Bathurst, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Ka Lam Sam
- The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
| | - Thomas Law
- The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Clerkin EM, Smith LB. Real-world statistics at two timescales and a mechanism for infant learning of object names. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:e2123239119. [PMID: 35482916 PMCID: PMC9170168 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2123239119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/24/2021] [Accepted: 03/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Infants begin learning the visual referents of nouns before their first birthday. Despite considerable empirical and theoretical effort, little is known about the statistics of the experiences that enable infants to break into object–name learning. We used wearable sensors to collect infant experiences of visual objects and their heard names for 40 early-learned categories. The analyzed data were from one context that occurs multiple times a day and includes objects with early-learned names: mealtime. The statistics reveal two distinct timescales of experience. At the timescale of many mealtime episodes (n = 87), the visual categories were pervasively present, but naming of the objects in each of those categories was very rare. At the timescale of single mealtime episodes, names and referents did cooccur, but each name–referent pair appeared in very few of the mealtime episodes. The statistics are consistent with incremental learning of visual categories across many episodes and the rapid learning of name–object mappings within individual episodes. The two timescales are also consistent with a known cortical learning mechanism for one-episode learning of associations: new information, the heard name, is incorporated into well-established memories, the seen object category, when the new information cooccurs with the reactivation of that slowly established memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth M. Clerkin
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405-7007
| | - Linda B. Smith
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405-7007
- Cognitive Science Program, Indiana University, Bloomington, Bloomington, IN 47405-7007
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Pace A, Rojas R, Bakeman R, Adamson LB, Tamis-LeMonda CS, O'Brien Caughy M, Owen MT, Suma K. A Longitudinal Study of Language Use During Early Mother-Child Interactions in Spanish-Speaking Families Experiencing Low Income. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2022; 65:303-319. [PMID: 34890248 PMCID: PMC9150737 DOI: 10.1044/2021_jslhr-21-00329] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2021] [Revised: 08/18/2021] [Accepted: 09/09/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE This longitudinal study assessed continuity and stability of productive language (vocabulary and grammar) and discourse features (turn-taking; asking and responding to questions) during mother-child play. METHOD Parent-child language use in 119 Spanish-speaking, Mexican immigrant mothers and their children at two ages (M = 2.5 and 3.6 years) was evaluated from transcriptions of interactions. RESULTS Child productive language significantly increased over the year, whereas mothers showed commensurate increases in vocabulary diversity but very little change in grammatical complexity. Mother-child discourse was characterized by discontinuity: Mothers decreased their turn length and asked fewer questions while children increased on both measures. Rates of responding to questions remained high for both mothers and children even as children increased and mothers decreased over time. Mothers and children showed significant rank-order stability in productive language and measures of discourse. Mothers' rate of asking questions and children's responses to questions during the first interaction predicted children's receptive vocabulary a year later. CONCLUSIONS As children become more sophisticated communicators, language input remains important, with discourse features growing in relevance. Children's early opportunities to respond to parents' questions in the context of play benefit their language skills. This work extends the evidence base from monolingual English-speaking families and is interpreted in the context of prior research on parenting practices in U.S. families of Mexican origin.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amy Pace
- University of Washington, Seattle
| | - Raúl Rojas
- The University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Variability in Quantity and Quality of Early Linguistic Experience in Children With Cochlear Implants: Evidence from Analysis of Natural Auditory Environments. Ear Hear 2022; 43:685-698. [PMID: 34611118 PMCID: PMC8881322 DOI: 10.1097/aud.0000000000001136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Understanding how quantity and quality of language input vary across children with cochlear implants (CIs) is important for explaining sources of large individual differences in language outcomes of this at-risk pediatric population. Studies have mostly focused either on intervention-related, device-related, and/or patient-related factors, or relied on data from parental reports and laboratory-based speech corpus to unravel factors explaining individual differences in language outcomes among children with CIs. However, little is known about the extent to which children with CIs differ in quantity and quality of language input they experience in their natural linguistic environments. To address this knowledge gap, the present study analyzed the quantity and quality of language input to early-implanted children (age of implantation <23 mo) during the first year after implantation. DESIGN Day-long Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) recordings, derived from home environments of 14 early-implanted children, were analyzed to estimate numbers of words per day, type-token ratio (TTR), and mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLUm) in adults' speech. Properties of language input were analyzed across these three dimensions to examine how input in home environments varied across children with CIs in quantity, defined as number of words, and quality, defined as whether speech was child-directed or overheard. RESULTS Our per-day estimates demonstrated that children with CIs were highly variable in the number of total words (mean ± SD = 25,134 ± 9,267 words) and high-quality child-directed words (mean ± SD = 10,817 ± 7,187 words) they experienced in a day in their home environments during the first year after implantation. The results also showed that the patterns of variability across children in quantity and quality of language input changes depending on whether the speech was child-directed or overheard. Children also experienced highly different environments in terms of lexical diversity (as measured by TTR) and morphosyntactic complexity (as measured by MLUm) of language input. The results demonstrated that children with CIs varied substantially in the quantity and quality of language input experienced in their home environments. More importantly, individual children experienced highly variable amounts of high-quality, child-directed speech, which may drive variability in language outcomes across children with CIs. CONCLUSIONS Analyzing early language input in natural, linguistic environments of children with CIs showed that the quantity and quality of early linguistic input vary substantially across individual children with CIs. This substantial individual variability suggests that the quantity and quality of early linguistic input are potential sources of individual differences in outcomes of children with CIs and warrant further investigation to determine the effects of this variability on outcomes.
Collapse
|
21
|
Hindman AH, Farrow JM, Anderson K, Wasik BA, Snyder PA. Understanding Child-Directed Speech Around Book Reading in Toddler Classrooms: Evidence From Early Head Start Programs. Front Psychol 2021; 12:719783. [PMID: 34955952 PMCID: PMC8695438 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.719783] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2021] [Accepted: 10/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Child-directed speech (CDS), which can help children learn new words, has been rigorously studied among infants and parents in home settings. Yet, far less is known about the CDS that teachers use in classrooms with toddlers and children’s responses, an important question because many toddlers, particularly in high-need communities, attend group-care settings. This exploratory study examines the linguistic environment during teacher-led book readings in American Early Head Start classrooms serving 2-year-olds from households in poverty. Seven teachers in four classrooms were trained to emphasize target words while reading story and informational books. We first analyzed the nature and quality of their book readings from a macro-level, exploring global instructional quality [Classroom Assessment Scoring System (CLASS)] and linguistic complexity [i.e., diversity of vocabulary (D) and sophistication of syntax (MLU-w)], and we also examined micro-level teacher-child talk strategies and use of target words. Compared to prior research, these classrooms had similar global quality and syntactic complexity, although less lexical diversity. Exploratory results also revealed three distinct teacher talk patterns—teachers who emphasized (1) comments, (2) questions, and (3) a balance of the two. Question-focused teachers had more adult and child talk during reading, as well as more repetitions of target words, and stronger CLASS Engaged Support for Learning. However, comment-focused teachers used more diverse vocabulary and had stronger CLASS Emotional and Behavioral Support. Results illuminate the nature and quality of CDS in toddler classrooms, particularly in the context of an intervention emphasizing target vocabulary words, and highlight applications for professional development and questions for further research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Annemarie H Hindman
- Department of Teaching and Learning, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, United States
| | - Jean M Farrow
- Graduate School of Education, University of Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania, PA, United States
| | - Kate Anderson
- Center for Social Organization of Schools, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Barbara A Wasik
- Department of Teaching and Learning, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, United States
| | - Patricia A Snyder
- Anita Zucker Center for Excellence in Early Childhood Studies, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Nandy A, Nixon E, Quigley J. Communicative functions of parents' child-directed speech across dyadic and triadic contexts. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2021; 48:1281-1294. [PMID: 33557996 DOI: 10.1017/s030500092000080x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
This study examined the roles of parental gender and context in the communicative functions of parents' child-directed speech. Seventy three families with toddlers participated in the study. Dyadic and triadic parent-toddler interactions were videotaped during structured play activities. Results indicated context-dependent variability in parents' facilitative speech and gentle guidance. Parental gender effects were observed in parents' directive speech but no gender or contextual effects were observed in parents' referential speech. Results suggest the need for a closer examination of parental gender and contextual factors related to parents' speech functions.
Collapse
|
23
|
Mendoza JK, Fausey CM. Quantifying Everyday Ecologies: Principles for Manual Annotation of Many Hours of Infants' Lives. Front Psychol 2021; 12:710636. [PMID: 34552533 PMCID: PMC8450442 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.710636] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2021] [Accepted: 07/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Everyday experiences are the experiences available to shape developmental change. Remarkable advances in devices used to record infants' and toddlers' everyday experiences, as well as in repositories to aggregate and share such recordings across teams of theorists, have yielded a potential gold mine of insights to spur next-generation theories of experience-dependent change. Making full use of these advances, however, currently requires manual annotation. Manually annotating many hours of everyday life is a dedicated pursuit requiring significant time and resources, and in many domains is an endeavor currently lacking foundational facts to guide potentially consequential implementation decisions. These realities make manual annotation a frequent barrier to discoveries, as theorists instead opt for narrower scoped activities. Here, we provide theorists with a framework for manually annotating many hours of everyday life designed to reduce both theoretical and practical overwhelm. We share insights based on our team's recent adventures in the previously uncharted territory of everyday music. We identify principles, and share implementation examples and tools, to help theorists achieve scalable solutions to challenges that are especially fierce when annotating extended timescales. These principles for quantifying everyday ecologies will help theorists collectively maximize return on investment in databases of everyday recordings and will enable a broad community of scholars—across institutions, skillsets, experiences, and working environments—to make discoveries about the experiences upon which development may depend.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer K Mendoza
- Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, United States
| | - Caitlin M Fausey
- Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, United States
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Creaghe N, Quinn S, Kidd E. Symbolic play provides a fertile context for language development. INFANCY 2021; 26:980-1010. [PMID: 34297890 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12422] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2020] [Revised: 06/21/2021] [Accepted: 06/29/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
In this study, we test the hypothesis that symbolic play represents a fertile context for language acquisition because its inherent ambiguity elicits communicative behaviors that positively influence development. Infant-caregiver dyads (N = 54) participated in two 20-minute play sessions six months apart (Time 1 = 18 months, Time 2 = 24 months). During each session, the dyads played with two sets of toys that elicited either symbolic or functional play. The sessions were transcribed and coded for several features of dyadic interaction and language; infants' linguistic proficiency was measured via parental report. The two contexts elicited different communicative and linguistic behaviors. Notably, the symbolic play condition resulted in significantly greater conversational turn-taking than functional play, and also resulted in the greater use of questions and mimetics in infant-directed speech (IDS). In contrast, caregivers used more imperative clauses in functional play. Correlational and regression analyses showed that frequent properties of symbolic play (i.e., turn-taking, yes-no questions, mimetics) were positively related to infants' language proficiency, whereas frequent features of functional play (i.e., imperatives in IDS) were negatively related. The results provide evidence supporting the hypothesis that symbolic play is a fertile context for language development, driven by the need to negotiate meaning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Noëlie Creaghe
- Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Sara Quinn
- Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Evan Kidd
- Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
MARCHMAN VA, WEISLEDER A, HURTADO N, FERNALD A. Accuracy of the Language Environment Analyses (LENA TM) system for estimating child and adult speech in laboratory settings. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2021; 48:605-620. [PMID: 32690113 PMCID: PMC8178803 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000920000380] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Laboratory observations are a mainstay of language development research, but transcription is costly. We test whether speech recognition technology originally designed for day-long contexts can be usefully applied to this use-case. We compared automated adult word and child vocalization counts from Language Environment Analysis (LENATM) to those of transcribers in 20-minute play sessions with Spanish-speaking dyads (n = 104) at 1;7 and 2;2. For adult words, results indicated moderate associations but large absolute differences. Associations for child vocalizations were weaker with larger absolute discrepancies. LENA has moderate potential to ease the burden of transcription in some research and clinical applications.
Collapse
|
26
|
Ko ES, Jo J, On KW, Zhang BT. Introducing the Ko Corpus of Korean Mother-Child Interaction. Front Psychol 2021; 11:602623. [PMID: 33456445 PMCID: PMC7805280 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.602623] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2020] [Accepted: 11/30/2020] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
We describe a corpus of speech taking place between 30 Korean mother–child pairs, divided in three groups of Prelexical (M = 0;08), Early-Lexical (M = 1;02), and Advanced-Lexical (M = 2;03). In addition to the child-directed speech (CDS), this corpus includes two different formalities of adult-directed speech (ADS), i.e., family-directed ADS (ADS_Fam) and experimenter-directed ADS (ADS_Exp). Our analysis of the MLU in CDS, family-, and experimenter-directed ADS found significant differences between CDS and ADS_Fam, and between ADS_Fam and ADS_Exp, but not between CDS and ADS_Exp. Our finding suggests that researchers should pay more attention to controlling the level of formality in CDS and ADS when comparing the two registers for their speech characteristics. The corpus was transcribed in the CHAT format of the CHILDES system, so users can easily extract data related to verbal behavior in the mother–child interaction using the CLAN program of CHILDES.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eon-Suk Ko
- Department of English Language and Literature, Chosun University, Gwangju, South Korea
| | - Jinyoung Jo
- Department of Linguistics, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
| | - Kyung-Woon On
- Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Byoung-Tak Zhang
- Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Masek LR, Paterson SJ, Golinkoff RM, Bakeman R, Adamson LB, Owen MT, Pace A, Hirsh‐Pasek K. Beyond talk: Contributions of quantity and quality of communication to language success across socioeconomic strata. INFANCY 2020; 26:123-147. [DOI: 10.1111/infa.12378] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2019] [Revised: 10/23/2020] [Accepted: 10/29/2020] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Roger Bakeman
- Department of Psychology Georgia State University Atlanta GA USA
| | | | - Margaret Tresch Owen
- School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences The University of Texas at Dallas Richardson TX USA
| | - Amy Pace
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences The University of Washington Seattle WA USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
28
|
Menendez D, Mathiaparanam ON, Liu D, Seitz V, Alibali MW, Rosengren KS. Representing Variability: The Case of Life Cycle Diagrams. CBE LIFE SCIENCES EDUCATION 2020; 19:ar49. [PMID: 32870076 PMCID: PMC8711823 DOI: 10.1187/cbe.19-11-0251] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2019] [Revised: 06/30/2020] [Accepted: 07/05/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Two foundational concepts in biology education are 1) offspring are not identical to their parents, and 2) organisms undergo changes throughout their lives. These concepts are included in both international and U.S. curricular standards. Research in psychology has shown that children often have difficulty understanding these concepts, as they are inconsistent with their intuitive theories of the biological world. Additionally, prior research suggests that diagrams are commonly used in instruction and that their features influence student learning. Given this prior work, we explored the characteristics of life cycle diagrams and discuss possible implications for student learning. We examined 75 life cycle diagrams from books, including five biology or general science textbooks and 25 specialized trade books focusing on biology for children. We also examined 633 life cycle diagrams from a publicly available online database of science diagrams. Most diagrams failed to show any within-species variability. Additionally, many diagrams had perceptually rich backgrounds, which prior research suggests might hinder learning. We discuss how the design characteristics of diagrams may reinforce students' intuitive theories of biology, which might make it difficult for students to understand key biological concepts in the future.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Menendez
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, 53706
| | | | - David Liu
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, 53706
| | - Vienne Seitz
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, 53706
| | - Martha W. Alibali
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, 53706
| | - Karl S. Rosengren
- Department of Psychology and Department of Brain and Cognitive Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York, 14627
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Ossewaarde R, Jonkers R, Jalvingh F, Bastiaanse R. Quantifying the Uncertainty of Parameters Measured in Spontaneous Speech of Speakers With Dementia. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2020; 63:2255-2270. [PMID: 32598210 DOI: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-19-00222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Purpose Corpus analyses of spontaneous language fragments of varying length provide useful insights in the language change caused by brain damage, such as caused by some forms of dementia. Sample size is an important experimental parameter to consider when designing spontaneous language analyses studies. Sample length influences the confidence levels of analyses. Machine learning approaches often favor to use as much language as available, whereas language evaluation in a clinical setting is often based on truncated samples to minimize annotation labor and to limit any discomfort for participants. This article investigates, using Bayesian estimation of machine learned models, what the ideal text length should be to minimize model uncertainty. Method We use the Stanford parser to extract linguistic variables and train a statistic model to distinguish samples by speakers with no brain damage from samples by speakers with probable Alzheimer's disease. We compare the results to previously published models that used CLAN for linguistic analysis. Results The uncertainty around six individual variables and its relation to sample length are reported. The same model with linguistic variables that is used in all three experiments can predict group membership better than a model without them. One variable (concept density) is more informative when measured using the Stanford tools than when measured using CLAN. Conclusion For our corpus of German speech, the optimal sample length is found to be around 700 words long. Longer samples do not provide more information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roelant Ossewaarde
- Center for Language and Cognition Groningen, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, the Netherlands
- Institute for ICT, HU University of Applied Science, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Roel Jonkers
- Center for Language and Cognition Groningen, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, the Netherlands
| | - Fedor Jalvingh
- Center for Language and Cognition Groningen, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, the Netherlands
| | - Roelien Bastiaanse
- Center for Language and Cognition Groningen, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, the Netherlands
- Center for Language and Brain, NRU Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Dilley L, Lehet M, Wieland EA, Arjmandi MK, Kondaurova M, Wang Y, Reed J, Svirsky M, Houston D, Bergeson T. Individual Differences in Mothers' Spontaneous Infant-Directed Speech Predict Language Attainment in Children With Cochlear Implants. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2020; 63:2453-2467. [PMID: 32603621 PMCID: PMC7838839 DOI: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-19-00229] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2019] [Revised: 01/17/2020] [Accepted: 04/01/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Purpose Differences across language environments of prelingually deaf children who receive cochlear implants (CIs) may affect language acquisition; yet, whether mothers show individual differences in how they modify infant-directed (ID) compared with adult-directed (AD) speech has seldom been studied. This study assessed individual differences in how mothers realized speech modifications in ID register and whether these predicted differences in language outcomes for children with CIs. Method Participants were 36 dyads of mothers and their children aged 0;8-2;5 (years;months) at the time of CI implantation. Mothers' spontaneous speech was recorded in a lab setting in ID or AD conditions before ~15 months postimplantation. Mothers' speech samples were characterized for acoustic-phonetic and lexical properties established as canonical indices of ID speech to typically hearing infants, such as vowel space area differences, fundamental frequency variability, and speech rate. Children with CIs completed longitudinal administrations of one or more standardized language assessment instruments at variable intervals from 6 months to 9.5 years postimplantation. Standardized scores on assessments administered longitudinally were used to calculate linear regressions, which gave rise to predicted language scores for children at 2 years postimplantation and language growth over 2-year intervals. Results Mothers showed individual differences in how they modified speech in ID versus AD registers. Crucially, these individual differences significantly predicted differences in estimated language outcomes at 2 years postimplantation in children with CIs. Maternal speech variation in lexical quantity and vowel space area differences across ID and AD registers most frequently predicted estimates of language attainment in children with CIs, whereas prosodic differences played a minor role. Conclusion Results support that caregiver language behaviors play a substantial role in explaining variability in language attainment in children receiving CIs. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.12560147.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Dilley
- Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Michigan State University, East Lansing
| | - Matthew Lehet
- Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Michigan State University, East Lansing
| | - Elizabeth A. Wieland
- Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Michigan State University, East Lansing
| | - Meisam K. Arjmandi
- Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Michigan State University, East Lansing
| | - Maria Kondaurova
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, KY
| | - Yuanyuan Wang
- Department of Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, The Ohio State University, Columbus
| | - Jessa Reed
- Department of Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, The Ohio State University, Columbus
| | - Mario Svirsky
- Department of Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, New York University, New York City
| | - Derek Houston
- Department of Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, The Ohio State University, Columbus
- Nationwide Children’s Hospital, Columbus, OH
| | - Tonya Bergeson
- Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Butler University, Indianapolis
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Circumspection in using automated measures: Talker gender and addressee affect error rates for adult speech detection in the Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) system. Behav Res Methods 2020; 53:113-138. [DOI: 10.3758/s13428-020-01419-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
32
|
Quigley J, Nixon E. Infant language predicts fathers' vocabulary in infant-directed speech. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2020; 47:146-158. [PMID: 31030683 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000919000205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
Research on sources of individual difference in parental Infant-Directed Speech (IDS) is limited and there is a particular lack of research on fathers' compared to mothers' speech. This study examined the predictive relations between infant characteristics and variability in paternal lexical diversity (LD) in dyadic free play with two-year-olds (M = 24.1 months, SD = 1.39, 35 girls). Ten minutes of interaction for sixty-four father-infant dyads were transcribed and multiple regression analyses were performed to examine the effects of a set of distal and proximal sources of infant influence on paternal LD. Fathers' LD was predicted only by infant language, both standardised language scores and dynamic language measures, and was not predicted by infant age, gender, executive function, or temperament. Findings are discussed in the light of the complex interplay of factors contributing to variability in IDS and the infant's linguistic environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean Quigley
- School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
| | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Wulff DU, De Deyne S, Jones MN, Mata R. New Perspectives on the Aging Lexicon. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23:686-698. [PMID: 31288976 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2019] [Revised: 05/06/2019] [Accepted: 05/07/2019] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
The field of cognitive aging has seen considerable advances in describing the linguistic and semantic changes that happen during the adult life span to uncover the structure of the mental lexicon (i.e., the mental repository of lexical and conceptual representations). Nevertheless, there is still debate concerning the sources of these changes, including the role of environmental exposure and several cognitive mechanisms associated with learning, representation, and retrieval of information. We review the current status of research in this field and outline a framework that promises to assess the contribution of both ecological and psychological aspects to the aging lexicon.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dirk U Wulff
- University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland; Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.
| | | | | | - Rui Mata
- University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland; Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Clerkin EM, Smith LB. The everyday statistics of objects and their names: How word learning gets its start. COGSCI ... ANNUAL CONFERENCE OF THE COGNITIVE SCIENCE SOCIETY. COGNITIVE SCIENCE SOCIETY (U.S.). CONFERENCE 2019; 2019:240-246. [PMID: 34713275 PMCID: PMC8549651] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
A key question in early word learning is how infants learn their first object names despite a natural environment thought to provide messy data for linking object names to their referents. Using head cameras worn by 7 to 11-month-old infants in the home, we document the statistics of visual objects, spoken object names, and their co-occurrence in everyday meal time events. We show that the extremely right skewed frequency distribution of visual objects underlies word-referent co-occurrence statistics that set up a clear signal in the noise upon which infants could capitalize to learn their first object names.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth M Clerkin
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 E. 10 Street Bloomington, IN 47405
| | - Linda B Smith
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 E. 10 Street Bloomington, IN 47405
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Raz HK, Abney DH, Crandall D, Yu C, Smith LB. How do infants start learning object names in a sea of clutter? COGSCI ... ANNUAL CONFERENCE OF THE COGNITIVE SCIENCE SOCIETY. COGNITIVE SCIENCE SOCIETY (U.S.). CONFERENCE 2019; 2019:521-526. [PMID: 33634271 PMCID: PMC7903936] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Infants are powerful learners. A large corpus of experimental paradigms demonstrate that infants readily learn distributional cues of name-object co-occurrences. But infants' natural learning environment is cluttered: every heard word has multiple competing referents in view. Here we ask how infants start learning name-object co-occurrences in naturalistic learning environments that are cluttered and where there is much visual ambiguity. The framework presented in this paper integrates a naturalistic behavioral study and an application of a machine learning model. Our behavioral findings suggest that in order to start learning object names, infants and their parents consistently select a set of a few objects to play with during a set amount of time. What emerges is a frequency distribution of a few toys that approximates a Zipfian frequency distribution of objects for learning. We find that a machine learning model trained with a Zipf-like distribution of these object images outperformed the model trained with a uniform distribution. Overall, these findings suggest that to overcome referential ambiguity in clutter, infants may be selecting just a few toys allowing them to learn many distributional cues about a few name-object pairs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hadar Karmazyn Raz
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405 USA
| | - Drew H Abney
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405 USA
| | - David Crandall
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405 USA
| | - Chen Yu
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405 USA
| | - Linda B Smith
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405 USA
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Jayaraman S, Smith LB. Faces in early visual environments are persistent not just frequent. Vision Res 2018; 157:213-221. [PMID: 29852210 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2017] [Revised: 05/19/2018] [Accepted: 05/23/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The regularities in very young infants' visual worlds likely have out-sized effects on the development of the visual system because they comprise the first-in experience that tunes, maintains, and specifies the neural substrate from low-level to higher-level representations and therefore constitute the starting point for all other visual learning. Recent evidence from studies using head cameras suggests that the frequency of faces available in early infant visual environments declines over the first year and a half of life. The primary question for the present paper concerns the temporal structure of face experiences: Is frequency the key exposure dimension distinguishing younger and older infants' face experiences, or is it the duration for which faces remain in view? Our corpus of head-camera images collected as infants went about their daily activities consisted of over a million individually coded frames sampled at 0.2 Hz from 232 h of infant-perspective scenes, recorded from 51 infants aged 1 month to 15 months. The major finding from this corpus is that very young infants (1-3 months) not only have more frequent face experiences but also more temporally persistent ones. The repetitions of the same very few face identities presenting up-close and frontal views are exaggerated in more persistent runs of the same face, and these persistent runs are more frequent for the youngest infants. The implications of early experiences consisting of extended repeated exposures of up-close frontal views for visual learning are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Swapnaa Jayaraman
- Indiana University, 1101 E. 10th st., Bloomington, IN 47404, United States.
| | - Linda B Smith
- Indiana University, 1101 E. 10th st., Bloomington, IN 47404, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Smith LB, Jayaraman S, Clerkin E, Yu C. The Developing Infant Creates a Curriculum for Statistical Learning. Trends Cogn Sci 2018. [PMID: 29519675 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 115] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
New efforts are using head cameras and eye-trackers worn by infants to capture everyday visual environments from the point of view of the infant learner. From this vantage point, the training sets for statistical learning develop as the sensorimotor abilities of the infant develop, yielding a series of ordered datasets for visual learning that differ in content and structure between timepoints but are highly selective at each timepoint. These changing environments may constitute a developmentally ordered curriculum that optimizes learning across many domains. Future advances in computational models will be necessary to connect the developmentally changing content and statistics of infant experience to the internal machinery that does the learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Linda B Smith
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 East 10th Street, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA.
| | - Swapnaa Jayaraman
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 East 10th Street, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
| | - Elizabeth Clerkin
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 East 10th Street, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
| | - Chen Yu
- Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 East 10th Street, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
| |
Collapse
|