1
|
Roach JP, Eniwaye B, Booth V, Sander LM, Zochowski MR. Acetylcholine Mediates Dynamic Switching Between Information Coding Schemes in Neuronal Networks. Front Syst Neurosci 2019; 13:64. [PMID: 31780905 PMCID: PMC6861375 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2018] [Accepted: 10/14/2019] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Rate coding and phase coding are the two major coding modes seen in the brain. For these two modes, network dynamics must either have a wide distribution of frequencies for rate coding, or a narrow one to achieve stability in phase dynamics for phase coding. Acetylcholine (ACh) is a potent regulator of neural excitability. Acting through the muscarinic receptor, ACh reduces the magnitude of the potassium M-current, a hyperpolarizing current that builds up as neurons fire. The M-current contributes to several excitability features of neurons, becoming a major player in facilitating the transition between Type 1 (integrator) and Type 2 (resonator) excitability. In this paper we argue that this transition enables a dynamic switch between rate coding and phase coding as levels of ACh release change. When a network is in a high ACh state variations in synaptic inputs will lead to a wider distribution of firing rates across the network and this distribution will reflect the network structure or pattern of external input to the network. When ACh is low, network frequencies become narrowly distributed and the structure of a network or pattern of external inputs will be represented through phase relationships between firing neurons. This work provides insights into how modulation of neuronal features influences network dynamics and information processing across brain states.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James P Roach
- Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
| | - Bolaji Eniwaye
- Department of Physics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
| | - Victoria Booth
- Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States.,Department of Mathematics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States.,Department of Anesthesiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
| | - Leonard M Sander
- Department of Physics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States.,Center for the Study of Complex Systems, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
| | - Michal R Zochowski
- Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States.,Department of Physics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States.,Center for the Study of Complex Systems, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States.,Biophysics Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Zhao L, Alsop DC, Detre JA, Dai W. Global fluctuations of cerebral blood flow indicate a global brain network independent of systemic factors. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2019; 39:302-312. [PMID: 28816098 PMCID: PMC6365600 DOI: 10.1177/0271678x17726625] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Global synchronization across specialized brain networks is a common feature of network models and in-vivo electrical measurements. Although the imaging of specialized brain networks with blood oxygenation sensitive resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI) has enabled detailed study of regional networks, the study of globally correlated fluctuations with rsfMRI is confounded by spurious contributions to the global signal from systemic physiologic factors and other noise sources. Here we use an alternative rsfMRI method, arterial spin labeled perfusion MRI, to characterize global correlations and their relationship to correlations and anti-correlations between regional networks. Global fluctuations that cannot be explained by systemic factors dominate the fluctuations in cerebral blood flow. Power spectra of these fluctuations are band limited to below 0.05 Hz, similar to prior measurements of regional network fluctuations in the brain. Removal of these global fluctuations prior to measurement of regional networks reduces all regional network fluctuation amplitudes to below the global fluctuation amplitude and changes the strength and sign of inter network correlations. Our findings support large amplitude, globally synchronized activity across networks that require a reassessment of regional network amplitude and correlation measures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Li Zhao
- 1 Department of Radiology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - David C Alsop
- 1 Department of Radiology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - John A Detre
- 2 Department of Neurology and Radiology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Weiying Dai
- 1 Department of Radiology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.,3 Department of Computer Science, Binghamton University, Binghamton, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Addis L, Sproviero W, Thomas SV, Caraballo RH, Newhouse SJ, Gomez K, Hughes E, Kinali M, McCormick D, Hannan S, Cossu S, Taylor J, Akman CI, Wolf SM, Mandelbaum DE, Gupta R, van der Spek RA, Pruna D, Pal DK. Identification of new risk factors for rolandic epilepsy: CNV at Xp22.31 and alterations at cholinergic synapses. J Med Genet 2018; 55:607-616. [PMID: 29789371 PMCID: PMC6119347 DOI: 10.1136/jmedgenet-2018-105319] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2018] [Revised: 04/18/2018] [Accepted: 04/28/2018] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Rolandic epilepsy (RE) is the most common genetic childhood epilepsy, consisting of focal, nocturnal seizures and frequent neurodevelopmental impairments in speech, language, literacy and attention. A complex genetic aetiology is presumed in most, with monogenic mutations in GRIN2A accounting for >5% of cases. OBJECTIVE To identify rare, causal CNV in patients with RE. METHODS We used high-density SNP arrays to analyse the presence of rare CNVs in 186 patients with RE from the UK, the USA, Sardinia, Argentina and Kerala, India. RESULTS We identified 84 patients with one or more rare CNVs, and, within this group, 14 (7.5%) with recurrent risk factor CNVs and 15 (8.0%) with likely pathogenic CNVs. Nine patients carried recurrent hotspot CNVs including at 16p13.11 and 1p36, with the most striking finding that four individuals (three from Sardinia) carried a duplication, and one a deletion, at Xp22.31. Five patients with RE carried a rare CNV that disrupted genes associated with other epilepsies (KCTD7, ARHGEF15, CACNA2D1, GRIN2A and ARHGEF4), and 17 cases carried CNVs that disrupted genes associated with other neurological conditions or that are involved in neuronal signalling/development. Network analysis of disrupted genes with high brain expression identified significant enrichment in pathways of the cholinergic synapse, guanine-exchange factor activation and the mammalian target of rapamycin. CONCLUSION Our results provide a CNV profile of an ethnically diverse cohort of patients with RE, uncovering new areas of research focus, and emphasise the importance of studying non-western European populations in oligogenic disorders to uncover a full picture of risk variation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Addis
- Department of Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, Maurice Wohl Clinical Neuroscience Institute, King’s College London, London, UK
- Neuroscience Discovery Research, Eli Lilly and Company, Surrey, UK
| | - William Sproviero
- Department of Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, Maurice Wohl Clinical Neuroscience Institute, King’s College London, London, UK
| | - Sanjeev V Thomas
- Department of Neurology, Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology, Trivandrum, Kerala, India
| | - Roberto H Caraballo
- Department of Neurology, Hospital de Pediatría Prof. Dr. J.P. Garrahan, Combate de los Pozos 1881, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Stephen J Newhouse
- Department of Biostatistics and Health Informatics, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
- NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, Bethlem Royal Hospital, Beckenham, UK
- Farr Institute of Health Informatics Research, UCL Institute of Health Informatics, University College London, London, UK
| | - Kumudini Gomez
- Department of Paediatrics, University Hospital Lewisham, Lewisham and Greenwich NHS Trust, London, UK
| | - Elaine Hughes
- Department of Paediatric Neurosciences, Evelina London Children’s Hospital, St Thomas’ Hospital, London, UK
| | - Maria Kinali
- Department of Paediatric Neurology, Chelsea and Westminster Hospital, London, UK
| | - David McCormick
- Department of Paediatric Neurosciences, Evelina London Children’s Hospital, St Thomas’ Hospital, London, UK
| | - Siobhan Hannan
- Department of Paediatric Neurology, Chelsea and Westminster Hospital, London, UK
| | - Silvia Cossu
- Neurosurgery Unit, Neuroscience and Neurorehabilitation Department, Bambino Gesù Children Hospital, Rome, Italy
- Neurology Unit, Pediatric Hospital A. Cao, Brotzu Hospital Trust, Cagliari, Italy
| | | | - Cigdem I Akman
- Division of Pediatric Neurology, College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University, New York City, New York, USA
| | - Steven M Wolf
- Department of Neurology, Mount Sinai Health System, New York City, New York, USA
| | - David E Mandelbaum
- Departments of Pediatrics, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, USA
| | - Rajesh Gupta
- Department of Paediatrics, Tunbridge Wells Hospital, Pembury, UK
| | - Rick A van der Spek
- Department of Neurology, Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Dario Pruna
- Neurology Unit, Pediatric Hospital A. Cao, Brotzu Hospital Trust, Cagliari, Italy
| | - Deb K Pal
- Department of Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, Maurice Wohl Clinical Neuroscience Institute, King’s College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Nowak A, Vallacher RR, Zochowski M, Rychwalska A. Functional Synchronization: The Emergence of Coordinated Activity in Human Systems. Front Psychol 2017; 8:945. [PMID: 28659842 PMCID: PMC5468424 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00945] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2016] [Accepted: 05/22/2017] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
The topical landscape of psychology is highly compartmentalized, with distinct phenomena explained and investigated with recourse to theories and methods that have little in common. Our aim in this article is to identify a basic set of principles that underlie otherwise diverse aspects of human experience at all levels of psychological reality, from neural processes to group dynamics. The core idea is that neural, behavioral, mental, and social structures emerge through the synchronization of lower-level elements (e.g., neurons, muscle movements, thoughts and feelings, individuals) into a functional unit—a coherent structure that functions to accomplish tasks. The coherence provided by the formation of functional units may be transient, persisting only as long as necessary to perform the task at hand. This creates the potential for the repeated assembly and disassembly of functional units in accordance with changing task demands. This perspective is rooted in principles of complexity science and non-linear dynamical systems and is supported by recent discoveries in neuroscience and recent models in cognitive and social psychology. We offer guidelines for investigating the emergence of functional units in different domains, thereby honoring the topical differentiation of psychology while providing an integrative foundation for the field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrzej Nowak
- Department of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and HumanitiesWarsaw, Poland.,Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca RatonFL, United States
| | - Robin R Vallacher
- Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca RatonFL, United States
| | - Michal Zochowski
- Department of Physics and Biophysics Program, University of Michigan, Ann ArborMI, United States
| | - Agnieszka Rychwalska
- The Robert Zajonc Institute for Social Studies, University of WarsawWarsaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Hopkins J. Free Energy and Virtual Reality in Neuroscience and Psychoanalysis: A Complexity Theory of Dreaming and Mental Disorder. Front Psychol 2016; 7:922. [PMID: 27471478 PMCID: PMC4946392 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00922] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2016] [Accepted: 06/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The main concepts of the free energy (FE) neuroscience developed by Karl Friston and colleagues parallel those of Freud's Project for a Scientific Psychology. In Hobson et al. (2014) these include an innate virtual reality generator that produces the fictive prior beliefs that Freud described as the primary process. This enables Friston's account to encompass a unified treatment-a complexity theory-of the role of virtual reality in both dreaming and mental disorder. In both accounts the brain operates to minimize FE aroused by sensory impingements-including interoceptive impingements that report compliance with biological imperatives-and constructs a representation/model of the causes of impingement that enables this minimization. In Friston's account (variational) FE equals complexity minus accuracy, and is minimized by increasing accuracy and decreasing complexity. Roughly the brain (or model) increases accuracy together with complexity in waking. This is mediated by consciousness-creating active inference-by which it explains sensory impingements in terms of perceptual experiences of their causes. In sleep it reduces complexity by processes that include both synaptic pruning and consciousness/virtual reality/dreaming in REM. The consciousness-creating active inference that effects complexity-reduction in REM dreaming must operate on FE-arousing data distinct from sensory impingement. The most relevant source is remembered arousals of emotion, both recent and remote, as processed in SWS and REM on "active systems" accounts of memory consolidation/reconsolidation. Freud describes these remembered arousals as condensed in the dreamwork for use in the conscious contents of dreams, and similar condensation can be seen in symptoms. Complexity partly reflects emotional conflict and trauma. This indicates that dreams and symptoms are both produced to reduce complexity in the form of potentially adverse (traumatic or conflicting) arousals of amygdala-related emotions. Mental disorder is thus caused by computational complexity together with mechanisms like synaptic pruning that have evolved for complexity-reduction; and important features of disorder can be understood in these terms. Details of the consilience among Freudian, systems consolidation, and complexity-reduction accounts appear clearly in the analysis of a single fragment of a dream, indicating also how complexity reduction proceeds by a process resembling Bayesian model selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jim Hopkins
- Research Department of Clinical Educational and Health Psychology, University College LondonLondon, UK
| |
Collapse
|