1
|
Lador-Weizman Y, Deutsch A. The influence of language-specific properties on the role of consonants and vowels in a statistical learning task of an artificial language: A cross-linguistic comparison. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2024; 77:2296-2311. [PMID: 38262925 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241229721] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2024]
Abstract
The contribution of consonants and vowels in spoken word processing has been widely investigated, and studies have found a phenomenon of a Consonantal bias (C-bias), indicating that consonants carry more weight than vowels. However, across languages, various patterns have been documented, including that of no preference or a reverse pattern of Vowel bias. A central question is how the manifestation of the C-bias is modulated by language-specific factors. This question can be addressed by cross-linguistic studies. Comparing native Hebrew and native English speakers, this study examines the relative importance of transitional probabilities between non-adjacent consonants as opposed to vowels during auditory statistical learning (SL) of an artificial language. Hebrew is interesting because its complex Semitic morphological structure has been found to play a central role in lexical access, allowing us to examine whether morphological properties can modulate the C-bias in early phases of speech perception, namely, word segmentation. As predicted, we found a significant interaction between language and consonant/vowel manipulation, with a higher performance in the consonantal condition than in the vowel condition for Hebrew speakers, namely, C-bias, and no consonant/vowel asymmetry among English speakers. We suggest that the observed interaction is morphologically anchored, indicating that phonological and morphological processes interact during early phases of auditory word perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yaara Lador-Weizman
- Seymour Fox School of Education, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Avital Deutsch
- Seymour Fox School of Education, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Højen A, Madsen TO, Bleses D. Danish 20-month-olds' recognition of familiar words with and without consonant and vowel mispronunciations. PHONETICA 2023; 80:309-328. [PMID: 37533184 DOI: 10.1515/phon-2023-2001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/04/2023]
Abstract
Although several studies initially supported the proposal by Nespor et al. (Nespor, Marina, Marcela Peña & Jacques Mehler. 2003. On the different roles of vowels and consonants in speech processing and language acquisition. Lingue e Linguaggio 2. 221-247) that consonants are more informative than vowels in lexical processing, a more complex picture has emerged from recent research. Current evidence suggests that infants initially show a vowel bias in lexical processing and later transition to a consonant bias, possibly depending on the characteristics of the ambient language. Danish infants have shown a vowel bias in word learning at 20 months-an age at which infants learning French or Italian no longer show a vowel bias but rather a consonant bias, and infants learning English show no bias. The present study tested whether Danish 20-month-olds also have a vowel bias when recognizing familiar words. Specifically, using the Intermodal Preferential Looking paradigm, we tested whether Danish infants were more likely to ignore or accept consonant than vowel mispronunciations when matching familiar words with pictures. The infants successfully matched correctly pronounced familiar words with pictures but showed no vowel or consonant bias when matching mispronounced words with pictures. The lack of a bias for Danish vowels or consonants in familiar word recognition adds to evidence that lexical processing biases are language-specific and may additionally depend on developmental age and perhaps task difficulty.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anders Højen
- School of Communication and Culture and TrygFonden's Centre for Child Research, Aarhus University, Aarhus V, Denmark
| | - Thomas O Madsen
- Department of Language, Culture, History and Communication, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
| | - Dorthe Bleses
- School of Communication and Culture and TrygFonden's Centre for Child Research, Aarhus University, Aarhus V, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bianchi I, Burro R. The Perception of Similarity, Difference and Opposition. J Intell 2023; 11:172. [PMID: 37754901 PMCID: PMC10532253 DOI: 10.3390/jintelligence11090172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2023] [Revised: 08/10/2023] [Accepted: 08/15/2023] [Indexed: 09/28/2023] Open
Abstract
After considering the pervasiveness of same/different relationships in Psychology and the experimental evidence of their perceptual foundation in Psychophysics and Infant and Comparative Psychology, this paper develops its main argument. Similarity and diversity do not complete the panorama since opposition constitutes a third relationship which is distinct from the other two. There is evidence of this in the previous literature investigating the perceptual basis of opposition and in the results of the two new studies presented in this paper. In these studies, the participants were asked to indicate to what extent pairs of simple bi-dimensional figures appeared to be similar, different or opposite to each other. A rating task was used in Study 1 and a pair comparison task was used in Study 2. Three main results consistently emerged: Firstly, opposition is distinct from similarity and difference which, conversely, are in a strictly inverse relationship. Secondly, opposition is specifically linked to something which points in an allocentrically opposite direction. Thirdly, alterations to the shape of an object are usually associated with the perception of diversity rather than opposition. The implications of a shift from a dyadic (same/different) to a triadic (similar/different/opposite) paradigm are discussed in the final section.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ivana Bianchi
- Department of Humanities, University of Macerata, 62100 Macerata, Italy
| | - Roberto Burro
- Department of Human Sciences, University of Verona, 37129 Verona, Italy;
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Schaadt G, Werwach A, Obrig H, Friederici AD, Männel C. Maturation of consonant perception, but not vowel perception, predicts lexical skills at 12 months. Child Dev 2023; 94:e166-e180. [PMID: 36716199 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13892] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
Consonants and vowels differentially contribute to lexical acquisition. From 8 months on, infants' preferential reliance on consonants has been shown to predict their lexical outcome. Here, the predictive value of German-learning infants' (n = 58, 29 girls, 29 boys) trajectories of consonant and vowel perception, indicated by the electrophysiological mismatch response, across 2, 6, and 10 months for later lexical acquisition was studied. The consonant-perception trajectory from 2 to 6 months (β = -2.95) and 6 to 10 months (β = -.91), but not the vowel-perception trajectory, significantly predicted receptive vocabulary at 12 months. These results reveal an earlier predictive value of consonant perception for word learning than previously found, and a particular role of the longitudinal maturation of this skill in lexical acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gesa Schaadt
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany.,Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Annika Werwach
- Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, Medical Faculty, University Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany.,Max Planck School of Cognition, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Hellmuth Obrig
- Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, Medical Faculty, University Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany.,Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Angela D Friederici
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Claudia Männel
- Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.,Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, Medical Faculty, University Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany.,Department of Audiology and Phoniatrics, Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Bouchon C, Hochmann JR, Toro JM. Spanish-learning infants switch from a vowel to a consonant bias during the first year of life. J Exp Child Psychol 2022; 221:105444. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105444] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2021] [Revised: 02/12/2022] [Accepted: 03/31/2022] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
6
|
Hochmann JR. Representations of Abstract Relations in Infancy. Open Mind (Camb) 2022; 6:291-310. [PMID: 36891038 PMCID: PMC9987345 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00068] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2022] [Accepted: 10/21/2022] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
relations are considered the pinnacle of human cognition, allowing for analogical and logical reasoning, and possibly setting humans apart from other animal species. Recent experimental evidence showed that infants are capable of representing the abstract relations same and different, prompting the question of the format of such representations. In a propositional language of thought, abstract relations would be represented in the form of discrete symbols. Is this format available to pre-lexical infants? We report six experiments (N = 192) relying on pupillometry and investigating how preverbal 10- to 12-month-old infants represent the relation same. We found that infants' ability to represent the relation same is impacted by the number of individual entities taking part in the relation. Infants could represent that four syllables were the same and generalized that relation to novel sequences (Experiments 1 and 4). However, they failed to generalize the relation same when it involved 5 or 6 syllables (Experiments 2-3), showing that infants' representation of the relation same is constrained by the limits of working memory capacity. Infants also failed to form a representation equivalent to all the same, which could apply to a varying number of same syllables (Experiments 5-6). These results highlight important discontinuities along cognitive development. Contrary to adults, preverbal infants lack a discrete symbol for the relation same, and rather build a representation of the relation by assembling symbols for individual entities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Rémy Hochmann
- CNRS UMR5229 - Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, 67 Boulevard Pinel, 69675, Bron, France.,Université Lyon 1 Claude Bernard, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Benavides-Varela S, Reoyo-Serrano N. Small-range numerical representations of linguistic sounds in 9- to 10-month-old infants. Cognition 2021; 213:104637. [PMID: 33685628 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104637] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2020] [Revised: 02/11/2021] [Accepted: 02/13/2021] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Coordinated studies provide evidence that very young infants, like human adults and nonhuman animals, readily discriminate small and large number of visual displays on the basis of numerical information. This capacity has been considerably less studied in the auditory modality. Surprisingly, the available studies yielded mixed evidence concerning whether numerical representations of auditory items in the small number range (1 to 3) are present early in human development. Specifically, while newborns discriminate 2- from 3-syllable sequences, older infants at 6 and 9 months of age fail to differentiate 2 from 3 tones. This study tested the hypothesis that infants can represent small sets more precisely when listening to ecologically relevant linguistic sounds. The aim was to probe 9- to 10-month-olds' (N = 74) ability to represent sound sets in a working memory test. In experiments 1 and 2, infants successfully discriminated 2- and 3-syllable sequences on the basis of their numerosity, when continuous variables, such as individual item duration, inter-stimulus duration, pitch, intensity, and total duration, were controlled for. In experiment 3, however, infants failed to discriminate 3- from 4-syllable sequences under similar conditions. Finally, in experiment 4, infants were tested on their ability to distinguish 2 and 3 tone sequences. The results showed no evidence that infants discriminated these non-linguistic stimuli. These findings indicate that, by means of linguistic sounds, infants can access a numerical system that yields precise auditory representations in the small number range.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvia Benavides-Varela
- Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padova, Padova, Italy; Department of Neuroscience, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.
| | - Natalia Reoyo-Serrano
- Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Hochmann JR, Toro JM. Negative mental representations in infancy. Cognition 2021; 213:104599. [PMID: 33526259 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104599] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2020] [Revised: 11/19/2020] [Accepted: 01/11/2021] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
How do infants' thoughts compare to the thoughts adults express with language? In particular, can infants entertain negative representations, such as not red or not here? In four experiments, we used pupillometry to ask whether negative representations are possible without an external language. Eleven-month-olds were tested on their ability to detect and represent the abstract structure of sequences of syllables, defined by the relations identity and/or negation: AAAA (four identical syllables; Experiment 1), AAA¬A (three times the syllable A and one final syllable that is not A; Experiment 2), AA(A)(A)¬A (two-to-four times the syllable A and one final syllable that is not A; Experiment 3). Representing the structures in Experiments 2-3 requires a form of negation. Results suggest that infants are able to compute both identity and negation. More generally, these results lend credit to the hypothesis that the infant mind is equipped with rudimentary logical operators before language takes off.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Rémy Hochmann
- CNRS UMR5229 - Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, 67 Boulevard Pinel, 69675 Bron, France; Université Lyon 1, Claude Bernard, France.
| | - Juan M Toro
- Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats, Pg. Lluis Companys, 23, 08019 Barcelona, Spain; Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Roc Boronat, 138, 08018 Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
|
10
|
Panneton R, Bremner JG, Johnson SP. Infancy studies come of age: Jacques Mehler's influence on the importance of perinatal experience for early language learning. Cognition 2020; 213:104543. [PMID: 33323278 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2020] [Revised: 11/27/2020] [Accepted: 12/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
In this paper, we pay homage to Jacques Mehler's empirical and theoretical contributions to the field of infancy studies. We focus on studies of the ability of the human fetus and newborn to attend to, learn from, and remember aspects of the environment, in particular the linguistic environment, as a part of an essential dynamic system of early influence. We provide a selective review of Mehler's and others' studies that examined the perinatal period and helped to clarify the earliest skills and predilections that infants bring to the task of language learning. We then highlight findings on newborns' perceptual skills and biases that motivated a shift in researchers' focus to fetal learning to better understand the role of the maternal voice in guiding newborns' speech perception. Finally, we point to the inspiration drawn from these perinatal approaches to more full-scale empirical treatments of how prenatal experience and behavior have come to be recognized as essential underpinnings to the earliest mental architectures of human cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Scott P Johnson
- University of California, Los Angeles, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Chen H, Lee DT, Luo Z, Lai RY, Cheung H, Nazzi T. Variation in phonological bias: Bias for vowels, rather than consonants or tones in lexical processing by Cantonese-learning toddlers. Cognition 2020; 213:104486. [PMID: 33077170 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104486] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2020] [Revised: 09/30/2020] [Accepted: 10/03/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Consonants and vowels have been considered to fulfill different functions in language processing, vowels being more important for prosodic and syntactic processes and consonants for lexically related processes (Nespor, Peña, & Mehler, 2003). This C-bias hypothesis in lexical processing is supported by studies with adults and infants in many languages such as English, French, Spanish, although a few studies, on Danish and Mandarin, suggest the existence of cross-linguistic variation. The present study explores whether a C-bias exists in a tone language with a complex tone system, Cantonese, by comparing the relative weight given to consonants, vowels, and also tones during word learning. To do so, looking behaviors of Cantonese-learning 20- and 30-month-olds (24 children per age/condition, 6 groups) were recorded by an eyetracker while they watched animated cartoons in Cantonese to learn pairs of novel words. The words differed minimally by either a consonant (e.g., /tœ6/ vs. /kœ6/), a vowel (e.g., /khim3/ vs. /khɛm3/), or a tone (e.g., T2 vs. T5). Analyses on proportional looking times revealed significant learning in 30-month-olds only, and at that age, only for the vowel contrasts. Growth curve analyses revealed better performance for the vowel condition compared to the other two conditions. The present findings establish a V-bias in Cantonese-learning 30-month-olds, adding new evidence from that tone language that the C-bias in lexical processing is not language-general. Implications for theoretical discussions on the origins of this phonological bias, and the impact of tones in early language acquisition, are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hui Chen
- Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, CNRS & Université Paris Descartes, 45 rue des Saints-Pères, 75006 Paris, France.
| | - Daniel T Lee
- The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, New Territories, Hong Kong
| | - Zili Luo
- The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, New Territories, Hong Kong
| | - Regine Y Lai
- The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, G/F, Leung Kau Kui Building, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong
| | - Hintat Cheung
- The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, New Territories, Hong Kong
| | - Thierry Nazzi
- Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, CNRS & Université Paris Descartes, 45 rue des Saints-Pères, 75006 Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
The role of linguistic experience in the development of the consonant bias. Anim Cogn 2020; 24:419-431. [PMID: 33052544 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-020-01436-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2020] [Revised: 09/18/2020] [Accepted: 09/26/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Consonants and vowels play different roles in speech perception: listeners rely more heavily on consonant information rather than vowel information when distinguishing between words. This reliance on consonants for word identification is the consonant bias Nespor et al. (Ling 2:203-230, 2003). Several factors modulate infants' development of the consonant bias, including fine-grained temporal processing ability and native language exposure [for review, see Nazzi et al. (Curr Direct Psychol Sci 25:291-296, 2016)]. A rat model demonstrated that mature fine-grained temporal processing alone cannot account for consonant bias emergence; linguistic exposure is also necessary Bouchon and Toro (An Cog 22:839-850, 2019). This study tested domestic dogs, who have similarly fine-grained temporal processing but more language exposure than rats, to assess whether a minimal lexicon and small degree of regular linguistic exposure can allow for consonant bias development. Dogs demonstrated a vowel bias rather than a consonant bias, preferring their own name over a vowel-mispronounced version of their name, but not in comparison to a consonant-mispronounced version. This is the pattern seen in young infants Bouchon et al. (Dev Sci 18:587-598, 2015) and rats Bouchon et al. (An Cog 22:839-850, 2019). In a follow-up study, dogs treated a consonant-mispronounced version of their name similarly to their actual name, further suggesting that dogs do not treat consonant differences as meaningful for word identity. These results support the findings from Bouchon and Toro (An Cog 2:839-850, 2019), suggesting that there may be a default preference for vowel information over consonant information when identifying word forms, and that the consonant bias may be a human-exclusive tool for language learning.
Collapse
|
13
|
Von Holzen K, Nazzi T. Emergence of a consonant bias during the first year of life: New evidence from own-name recognition. INFANCY 2020; 25:319-346. [PMID: 32749054 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2019] [Revised: 02/07/2020] [Accepted: 02/23/2020] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Recent evidence suggests that during the first year of life, a preference for consonant information during lexical processing (consonant bias) emerges, at least for some languages like French. Our study investigated the factors involved in this emergence as well as the developmental consequences for variation in consonant bias emergence. In a series of experiments, we measured 5-, 8-, and 11-month-old French-learning infants orientation times to a consonant or vowel mispronunciation of their own name, which is one of the few word forms familiar to infants at this young age. Both 5- and 8-month-olds oriented longer to vowel mispronunciations, but 11-month-olds showed a different pattern, initially orienting longer to consonant mispronunciations. We interpret these results as further evidence of an initial vowel bias, with consonant bias emergence by 11 months. Neither acoustic-phonetic nor lexical factors predicted preferences in 8- and 11-month-olds. Finally, counter to our predictions, a vowel bias at the time of test for 11-month-olds was related to later productive vocabulary outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katie Von Holzen
- Lehrstuhl Linguistik des Deutschen, Schwerpunkt Deutsch als Fremdsprache/Deutsch als Zweitsprache, Technische Universität Dortmund, Dortmund, Germany.,Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France.,CNRS (Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, UMR 8002), Paris, France.,Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
| | - Thierry Nazzi
- Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France.,CNRS (Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, UMR 8002), Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Is the consonant bias specifically human? Long-Evans rats encode vowels better than consonants in words. Anim Cogn 2019; 22:839-850. [PMID: 31222546 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-019-01280-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2018] [Revised: 05/21/2019] [Accepted: 06/11/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
In natural languages, vowels tend to convey structures (syntax, prosody) while consonants are more important lexically. The consonant bias, which is the tendency to rely more on consonants than on vowels to process words, is well attested in human adults and infants after the first year of life. Is the consonant bias based on evolutionarily ancient mechanisms, potentially present in other species? The current study investigated this issue in a species phylogenetically distant from humans: Long-Evans rats. During training, the animals were presented with four natural word-forms (e.g., mano, "hand"). We then compared their responses to novel words carrying either a consonant (pano) or a vowel change (meno). Results show that the animals were less disrupted by consonantal alterations than by vocalic alterations of words. That is, word recognition was more affected by the alteration of a vowel than a consonant. Together with previous findings in very young human infants, this reliance on vocalic information we observe in rats suggests that the emergence of the consonant bias may require a combination of vocal, cognitive and auditory skills that rodents do not seem to possess.
Collapse
|
15
|
Poltrock S, Chen H, Kwok C, Cheung H, Nazzi T. Adult Learning of Novel Words in a Non-native Language: Consonants, Vowels, and Tones. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1211. [PMID: 30087631 PMCID: PMC6066720 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2017] [Accepted: 06/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
While words are distinguished primarily by consonants and vowels in many languages, tones are also used in the majority of the world's languages to cue lexical contrasts. However, studies on novel word learning have largely concentrated on consonants and vowels. To shed more light on the use of tonal information in novel word learning and its relationship with the development of phonological categories, the present study explored how adults' ability to learn minimal pair pseudowords in a tone language is modulated by their native phonological knowledge. Twenty-four adult speakers of three languages were tested: Cantonese, Mandarin, and French. Eye-tracking was used to record eye movements of these learners, while they were watching animated cartoons in Cantonese. On each trial, adults had to learn two new label-object associations, while the labels differed minimally by a consonant, a vowel, or a tone. Learning would therefore attest to participants' ability to use phonological information to distinguish the paired words. Results first revealed that adult learners in each language group performed better than chance in all conditions. Moreover, compared to native Cantonese adults, both Mandarin- and French-speaking adults performed worse on all three contrasts. In addition, French adults were worse on tones when compared to Mandarin adults. Lastly, no advantage for consonantal information in native lexical processing was found for Cantonese-speaking adults as predicted by the “division of labor” proposal, thus confirming crosslinguistic differences in consonant/vowel weight between speakers of tonal vs. non-tonal languages. These findings establish rapid novel word learning in a non-native language (long-term learning will have to be further assessed), modulated by native phonological knowledge. The implications of the findings of this adult study for further infant word learning studies are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvana Poltrock
- Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France.,CNRS, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Paris, France.,Department Linguistik, Universität Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Hui Chen
- Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France.,CNRS, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Paris, France
| | - Celia Kwok
- Department of Linguistics and Modern Language Studies, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
| | - Hintat Cheung
- Department of Linguistics and Modern Language Studies, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
| | - Thierry Nazzi
- Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France.,CNRS, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Hochmann JR, Carey S, Mehler J. Infants learn a rule predicated on the relation same but fail to simultaneously learn a rule predicated on the relation different. Cognition 2018; 177:49-57. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2017] [Revised: 03/28/2018] [Accepted: 04/03/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
17
|
Nazzi T, Polka L. The consonant bias in word learning is not determined by position within the word: Evidence from vowel-initial words. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 174:103-111. [PMID: 29920448 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.05.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2018] [Revised: 05/21/2018] [Accepted: 05/21/2018] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
The current study used an object manipulation task to explore whether infants rely more on consonant information than on vowel information when learning new words even when the words start with a vowel. Canadian French-learning 20-month-olds, who were taught pairs of new vowel-initial words contrasted either on their initial vowel (opsi/eupsi) or following consonant (oupsa/outsa), were found to have learned the words only in the consonant condition and performed significantly better in the consonant condition than in the vowel condition. These results extend to Canadian French-learning infants the consonant bias in word learning previously found in French-learning infants from France and, crucially, shows that vocalic information has less weight than consonant information in new word learning even when it is the initial sound of the target words, confirming the consonant bias at the lexical level postulated by Nespor et al. (2003). The current findings also suggest that French-learning infants are able to segment vowel-initial words as early as 20 months of age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thierry Nazzi
- Université Paris Descartes, 75006 Paris, France; Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Institut Pluridisciplinaire des Saints Pères, 75270 Paris, France.
| | - Linda Polka
- School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, McGill University, Montréal, Quebec H3A 1G1, Canada; Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, McGill University, Montréal, Quebec H3A 1G1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Von Holzen K, Nishibayashi LL, Nazzi T. Consonant and Vowel Processing in Word Form Segmentation: An Infant ERP Study. Brain Sci 2018; 8:E24. [PMID: 29385046 PMCID: PMC5836043 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci8020024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2017] [Revised: 01/19/2018] [Accepted: 01/25/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Segmentation skill and the preferential processing of consonants (C-bias) develop during the second half of the first year of life and it has been proposed that these facilitate language acquisition. We used Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to investigate the neural bases of early word form segmentation, and of the early processing of onset consonants, medial vowels, and coda consonants, exploring how differences in these early skills might be related to later language outcomes. Our results with French-learning eight-month-old infants primarily support previous studies that found that the word familiarity effect in segmentation is developing from a positive to a negative polarity at this age. Although as a group infants exhibited an anterior-localized negative effect, inspection of individual results revealed that a majority of infants showed a negative-going response (Negative Responders), while a minority showed a positive-going response (Positive Responders). Furthermore, all infants demonstrated sensitivity to onset consonant mispronunciations, while Negative Responders demonstrated a lack of sensitivity to vowel mispronunciations, a developmental pattern similar to previous literature. Responses to coda consonant mispronunciations revealed neither sensitivity nor lack of sensitivity. We found that infants showing a more mature, negative response to newly segmented words compared to control words (evaluating segmentation skill) and mispronunciations (evaluating phonological processing) at test also had greater growth in word production over the second year of life than infants showing a more positive response. These results establish a relationship between early segmentation skills and phonological processing (not modulated by the type of mispronunciation) and later lexical skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katie Von Holzen
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, CNRS-Université Paris Descartes, 75006 Paris, France.
- Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20740, USA.
| | - Leo-Lyuki Nishibayashi
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, CNRS-Université Paris Descartes, 75006 Paris, France.
- Laboratory for Language Development, Riken Brain Science Institute, Wako-shi, Saitama-ken 351-0198, Japan.
| | - Thierry Nazzi
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, CNRS-Université Paris Descartes, 75006 Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Hochmann JR, Tuerk AS, Sanborn S, Zhu R, Long R, Dempster M, Carey S. Children's representation of abstract relations in relational/array match-to-sample tasks. Cogn Psychol 2017; 99:17-43. [PMID: 29132016 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.11.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2017] [Revised: 11/03/2017] [Accepted: 11/03/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Five experiments compared preschool children's performance to that of adults and of non-human animals on match to sample tasks involving 2-item or 16-item arrays that varied according to their composition of same or different items (Array Match-to-Sample, AMTS). They establish that, like non-human animals in most studies, 3- and 4-year-olds fail 2-item AMTS (the classic relational match to sample task introduced into the literature by Premack, 1983), and that robust success is not observed until age 6. They also establish that 3-year-olds, like non-human animal species, succeed only when they are able to encode stimuli in terms of entropy, a property of an array (namely its internal variability), rather than relations among the individuals in the array (same vs. different), whereas adults solve both 2-item and 16-item AMTS on the basis of the relations same and different. As in the case of non-human animals, the acuity of 3- and 4-year-olds' representation of entropy is insufficient to solve the 2-item same-different AMTS task. At age 4, behavior begins to contrast with that of non-human species. On 16-item AMTS, a subgroup of 4-year-olds induce a categorical rule matching all-same arrays to all-same arrays, while matching other arrays (mixed arrays of same and different items) to all-different arrays. These children tend to justify their choices using the words "same" and "different." By age 4 a number of our participants succeed at 2-item AMTS, also justifying their choices by explicit verbal appeals using words for same and different. Taken together these results suggest that the recruitment of the relational representations corresponding to the meaning of these words contributes to the better performance over the preschool years at solving array match-to-sample tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Rémy Hochmann
- CNRS, UMR 5304, Institut des Sciences Cognitives - Marc Jeannerod, 67 Bd Pinel, 69675 Bron, France; Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, France.
| | - Arin S Tuerk
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Sophia Sanborn
- Department of Psychology, UC Berkeley, Tolman Hall, Berkeley, CA 94720-1650, USA
| | - Rebecca Zhu
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Robert Long
- Department of Philosophy, New York University, 5 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Meg Dempster
- Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Claverton Down, Bath, United Kingdom
| | - Susan Carey
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| |
Collapse
|